Eyestonishing Notes for Teachers and Home Educators

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Eyestonishing Notes for Teachers and Home Educators EyeStonishing Notes for Teachers and Home Educators The activity starts with a consideration of the biology of the eye; how it functions and how our brains interpret the messages. Understandably this is kept brief but interested children can easily research this topic themselves. Firstly, it is important to emphasise that the medium being used – the internet – might not show illusions to best effect. If viewers are using a tablet or mobile phone, the screen will be too small for peripheral vision effects. If the screen resolution is low, again effects might not be demonstrable. We therefore include all the images in high definition for you to download and examine. Experience shows that simply copying these high-resolution images into a simple App like ‘Paint’ and displaying them on a big screen is effective. Now, some science …… Our brain and eyes work so well together that we believe what we are seeing is correct but our visual organs (eyes and brain) can sometimes be fooled into seeing things that aren’t there or incorrectly seeing things that are there. These effects are called optical illusions. Many illusions are created because we don’t see objects on their own. Instead we see them in context and surrounded by other objects which can influence our vision. By playing around with the context of colours, shapes, and tones and by changing the viewpoint of the audience you can create a wide range of illusions that will have you questioning what is real and what is not real. Video 1 Here an introductory section talks about the eye and vision. It moves on to consider: Scintillating Grids • Dark circles / blobs appear at the point where the white lines cross on the grid. Staring at one central white dot will leave your peripheral vision seeing black dots. • The mysterious blobs only appear when the crossing lines are straight, vertical and horizontal. When this happens the contrast between the lines and the black spaces creates a lateral inhibition effect. This occurs when signals from photoreceptors in your eyes conflict with each other, creating the impression of dark blobs where the white lines cross • Your eyes and brain use contrasts between light and shade to define the shape of objects. Your ability to see in 3D depends on how light and shadow are laid out. When you see a surface that has differently lit areas your visual system enhances the contrast to see it more clearly. This is called lateral inhibition and it works so well that it can create a lot of powerful visual illusions. Motion • Your eyes are so sensitive to light and motion that, sometimes you see movement even when there isn’t any. This is because your eyes make tiny movements that you are not aware of. When you look at an image you do not actually look at it in a steady way. Instead your eyes constantly jerk around, locating interesting parts of the scene to builds up a mental ‘map’ of the whole picture. Scientists call these random eye movements ‘Saccades’. • As your eyes try to follow the patterns, they make tiny movements, switching between the lines and the colours. This tricks the brain into seeing movement where there isn’t any. • This effect will not be apparent on a small or low-resolution screen. • Pulsing Star o The pulsating (right below) may be caused by differences between your central and peripheral vision. Your central vision is sharper and more sensitive to colour while peripheral vision is more sensitive to motion. The difference can create illusions of movement. • In a spin o As your eyes try to follow the patterns, (left below) they make tiny movements, switching between the lines and the colours. This tricks the brain into seeing movement where there isn’t any. 3-D Stereo images • Stereograms (also known as ‘autostereograms’ or ‘magic eye’) are 3D images hidden within a 2D pattern. In order to see the 3D image, move your face nearer to the screen. At this distance, your eyes cannot focus on the image and they focus somewhere behind the image. Now slowly move away from the image while trying to keep your eyes out-of-focus until you see the hidden image. Seeing a stereogram is tricky and you may have to be patient because it can take a little time. • The first stereogram is of a butterfly, then a DNA helix. In the garden it looks like a crocodile! While a bear is centrally located in this one. The bear looks like it is sat in a box or on a window sill. • These 3D images have all been easily visible on a computer screen – with a bit of practice. Surprisingly, they have been hardest to see when the You Tube videos have been shown in full screen mode. Video 2 Impossible shapes and Impossible Art • Impossible Cube Note how parts of the cube seem to be simultaneously in front of and behind other parts of the cube. You will see something which appears physically possible yet which you know is not. • Impossible triangle Follow the shape of the triangle starting at the top point; note how the left side seems to extend away from you, and the right side seems to extend towards you, yet they seem to lie on the same plane when they reach and are connected by the bottom vertices. You will see something which appears physically possible yet which you know is not. The shape of the triangle might at first appear feasible, but upon reflection one can see that it would be physically impossible to construct such a shape. • The Ambiguous Ring is one of many impossible figures (or impossible objects or undecidable figures): it depicts an object which could not possibly exist. It’s impossible for the Ambiguous Ring to exist because in order for it to exist rules of Euclidean geometry would have to be violated. Look at the green surface: is it the front or the back surface of the ring? You should experience a sense of perceptual confusion as you note the paradoxical nature of the depicted ring. • The Penrose Stairs Figure was created by Lionel Sharples Penrose (1898 -1972), a British psychiatrist, geneticist, and mathematician, and his son Sir Roger Penrose (1931 -), a British mathematician, physicist and philosopher of science. It was first published in the British Journal of Psychology in 1958 The Penrose Stairs is an impossible figure (or impossible object or undecidable figure): it depicts an object which could not possibly exist. It is impossible for the Penrose Stairs to exist because in order for it to exist rules of Euclidean geometry would have to be violated. For example, if one were to complete a circuit of the stairs, one would end up back at the same level that one began, even though each flight of the stairs continuously rise (or fall, depending on the direction of travel). Note how the stairs appear to continuously ascend when tracing round their path in one direction and descend in the other, yet if one were to complete a circuit one would end up back at the same level that one began. You will see something which appears physically possible yet which you know is not. • Belevedere is a painting by Maurits Cornelis Escher (1898 - 1972), a Dutch graphic artist that contains many impossible figures and impossible elements. The print was created in 1958, and the original version is now in the National Gallery of Canada, in Ontario. Look at the paradoxical placement of many of the pillars - look at where they begin at the top and where they end at the bottom, and yet they appear vertical. Look also at the impossible placement of the ladder. And notice the impossible cube that the boy sitting outside of the building is holding. You should experience a sense of perceptual confusion as you note the paradoxical nature of the building and other features in the painting. Bigger or Smaller? And Lining Up: How long? How far away? How much? We measure distance and estimate the dimensions of objects all the time usually without thinking about it. We can usually tell the difference between the actual size of an object and its apparent size. The actual size of an object cannot be observed (but it can be measured), because it must be viewed from a distance so that your eyes can focus on it. The apparent size of an object depends on it size and how far away it is. Objects that are closer will appear larger than if they are further away. Perspective and size constancy also play a part in how we see things. They obey these simple rules: • For two objects with the same ACTUAL size, the one that looks bigger is thought to be closer. • When we see two objects with the same APPARENT size, the one that appears to be further away is thought to be larger. • Muller-Lyer Illusion: Look at the two horizontal lines; note the apparent difference in length difference. The top horizontal line (with arrow heads pointing outwards) should appear to be longer than the bottom horizontal line (with arrowheads pointing inwards). However, they are in fact the same length. The Müller-Lyer Illusion is named after its creator, Franz Carl Müller-Lyer (1857 - 1916), a German psychiatrist and sociologist, who first published the illusion in the physiology journal Archiv für Anatomie und Physiologie, Physiologische Abteilung in 1889. The Müller-Lyer Illusion is one among a number of illusions where a central aspect of a simple line image – e.g.
Recommended publications
  • Fibonacci Number
    Fibonacci number From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia • Have questions? Find out how to ask questions and get answers. • • Learn more about citing Wikipedia • Jump to: navigation, search A tiling with squares whose sides are successive Fibonacci numbers in length A Fibonacci spiral, created by drawing arcs connecting the opposite corners of squares in the Fibonacci tiling shown above – see golden spiral In mathematics, the Fibonacci numbers form a sequence defined by the following recurrence relation: That is, after two starting values, each number is the sum of the two preceding numbers. The first Fibonacci numbers (sequence A000045 in OEIS), also denoted as Fn, for n = 0, 1, … , are: 0, 1, 1, 2, 3, 5, 8, 13, 21, 34, 55, 89, 144, 233, 377, 610, 987, 1597, 2584, 4181, 6765, 10946, 17711, 28657, 46368, 75025, 121393, ... (Sometimes this sequence is considered to start at F1 = 1, but in this article it is regarded as beginning with F0=0.) The Fibonacci numbers are named after Leonardo of Pisa, known as Fibonacci, although they had been described earlier in India. [1] [2] • [edit] Origins The Fibonacci numbers first appeared, under the name mātrāmeru (mountain of cadence), in the work of the Sanskrit grammarian Pingala (Chandah-shāstra, the Art of Prosody, 450 or 200 BC). Prosody was important in ancient Indian ritual because of an emphasis on the purity of utterance. The Indian mathematician Virahanka (6th century AD) showed how the Fibonacci sequence arose in the analysis of metres with long and short syllables. Subsequently, the Jain philosopher Hemachandra (c.1150) composed a well-known text on these.
    [Show full text]
  • In This Research Report We Will Explore the Gestalt Principles and Their Implications and How Human’S Perception Can Be Tricked
    The Gestalt Principles and there role in the effectiveness of Optical Illusions. By Brendan Mc Kinney Abstract Illusion are created in human perception in relation to how the mind process information, in this regard on to speculate that the Gestalt Principles are a key process in the success of optical illusions. To understand this principle the research paper will examine several optical illusions in the hopes that they exhibit similar traits used in the Gestalt Principles. In this research report we will explore the Gestalt Principles and their implications and how human’s perception can be tricked. The Gestalt Principles are the guiding principles of perception developed from testing on perception and how human beings perceive their surroundings. Human perception can however be tricked by understanding the Gestalt principles and using them to fool the human perception. The goal of this paper is to ask how illusions can be created to fool human’s perception using the gestalt principles as a basis for human’s perception. To examine the supposed, effect the Gestalt Principles in illusions we will look at three, the first being Rubin’s Vase, followed by the Penrose Stairs and the Kanizsa Triangle to understand the Gestalt Principles in play. In this context we will be looking at Optical Illusion rather than illusions using sound to understand the Gestalt Principles influence on human’s perception of reality. Illusions are described as a perception of something that is inconsistent with the actual reality (dictionary.com, 2015). How the human mind examines the world around them can be different from the actuality before them, this is due to the Gestalt principles influencing people’s perception.
    [Show full text]
  • The Psychological Intersection of Motion Picture, the Still Frame, and Three-Dimensional Form
    MOMENTUM, MOMENT, EPIPHANY: THE PSYCHOLOGICAL INTERSECTION OF MOTION PICTURE, THE STILL FRAME, AND THREE-DIMENSIONAL FORM by MARK GERSTEIN B.A. The University of Chicago, 1986 A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Fine Arts in the School of Visual Arts and Design in the College of Arts and Humanities at the University of Central Florida Orlando, Florida Spring Term 2018 ABSTRACT My journey from Hollywood Film production to a Fine Arts practice has been shaped by theory from Philosophy of Mind, Cognitive Psychology, Film, and Art, leading me to a new visual vocabulary at the intersection of motion picture, the still image, and three-dimensional form. I create large mixed media collages by projecting video onto photographs and sculptural forms, breaking the boundaries of the conventional film frame and exceeding the dynamic range of typical visual experience. My work explores emotional connections and fissures within family, and hidden meanings of haunting memories and familiar places. I am searching for an elusive type of perceptual experience characterized by an instantaneous shift in perspective—an “aha” moment of epiphany when suddenly I have the overpowering feeling that I am both seeing and aware that I am seeing. ii To Lori, Joshua and Maya, for your infinite patience and unconditional love. iii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I want to take this opportunity to acknowledge all my Studio Art colleagues who have so graciously tolerated my presence in their sandbox over these last few years. In particular, I want to thank my Thesis Committee: Carla Poindexter, my chair, for her nuanced critique, encouragement and unwavering belief in the potential of my work, and ability to embrace the seemingly contradictory roles of mentor and colleague; Jo Anne Adams, for her attention to detail and narrative sensibility that comes from our shared background in the film industry; and Ryan Buyssens, for showing me the possibilities of technology and interactivity, and reminding me to never lose sight of the meaning in my art.
    [Show full text]
  • Principles of Three-Dimensional Computer Design for Understanding Impossible Figures
    International Journal of Engineering and Management Sciences (IJEMS) Vol. 5. (2020). No. 2 DOI: 10.21791/IJEMS.2020.2.21. Principles of Three-Dimensional Computer Design for Understanding Impossible Figures T. DOVRAMADJIEV ITechnical University of Varna, Bulgaria, MTF, Dept. Industrial Design, [email protected] Abstract: For a better understanding of the impossible figures, it is advisable to use modern technological means by which the design of the geometry of the models gives a complete understanding of how they are made. Computer- aided 3D design completely solves this problem. That is, on the one hand, the ultimate visual variant of impossible figures is created, on the other hand, there is the possibility for real manipulation, movement, rotation and other models of space. In this study, 3D models of impossible figures are fully constructed, which are applied in the educational process in order to develop logical thinking. The steps of creating 3D geometry using open source software Blender 3D are described in details. Keywords: 3D, Blender, logic, impossible, figures Introduction Impossible figures are combinations of geometric elements positioned in specific compilations that create the illusion of completed objects, but at the same time have an extremely impossible vision. This is especially the case when certain details are interwoven in a particular order or position. Creating them requires a rich imagination and understanding of three-dimensional space. Impossible figures are a good tool for developing logical thinking, creating creativity in adolescents, and are often used in the educational process. Impossible figures are present in the visual arts, architecture and spatial form shaping.
    [Show full text]
  • Sounds, Spectra, Audio Illusions, and Data Representations
    Sounds, spectra, audio illusions, and data representations Edoardo Milotti, Dipartimento di Fisica, Università di Trieste Introduction to Signal Processing Techniques A. Y. 2016-17 Piano notes Pure 440 Hz sound BacK to the initial recording, left channel amplitude (volt, ampere, normalized amplitude units … ) time (sample number) amplitude (volt, ampere, normalized amplitude units … ) 0.004 0.002 0.000 -0.002 -0.004 0 1000 2000 3000 4000 5000 time (sample number) amplitude (volt, ampere, normalized amplitude units … ) 0.004 0.002 0.000 -0.002 -0.004 0 1000 2000 3000 4000 5000 time (sample number) squared amplitude frequency (frequency index) Short Time Fourier Transform (STFT) Fourier Transform A single blocK of data Segmented data Fourier Transform squared amplitude frequency (frequency index) squared amplitude frequency (frequency index) amplitude of most important Fourier component time Spectrogram time frequency • Original audio file • Reconstruction with the largest amplitude frequency component only • Reconstruction with 7 frequency components • Reconstruction with 7 frequency components + phase information amplitude (volt, ampere, normalized amplitude units … ) time (sample number) amplitude (volt, ampere, normalized amplitude units … ) time (sample number) squared amplitude frequency (frequency index) squared amplitude frequency (frequency index) squared amplitude Include only Fourier components with amplitudes ABOVE a given threshold 18 Fourier components frequency (frequency index) squared amplitude Include only Fourier components with amplitudes ABOVE a given threshold 39 Fourier components frequency (frequency index) squared amplitude frequency (frequency index) Glissando In music, a glissando [ɡlisˈsando] (plural: glissandi, abbreviated gliss.) is a glide from one pitch to another. It is an Italianized musical term derived from the French glisser, to glide.
    [Show full text]
  • The Freudian Slip Staff
    The Freudian Slip CSB/SJU Psychology Department Newsletter College of Saint Benedict & Saint John’s University Sigmund Freud, photo- graph (1938) May 2013 DSM-5 By Hannah Stevens at who is making these decisions. The decisions about changes are made by 13 different work The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of groups specializing in different sections. There are Mental Disorders or DSM is a manual giving the a total of 160 psychiatrists, psychologists, and Staff criteria for all recognized mental disorders. The other health professionals. It is a long process that first DSM dates back to before World War II and requires looking at recent research and debating Rachel Heying, provided seven different categories of mental with other professionals. health: mania, melancholia, monomania, paresis, “Imperfections dementia, dipsomania, and epilepsy. Since then The changes in the DSM-5 will have a of Perception” the DSM has been revised to fit mental health as significant impact on the field of psychology. we know it today. It is currently in its fifth revision Many practicing psychologist will have to relearn Hannah Stevens, and is due to come out this May. The current new criteria for mental disorders, as well as new “DMV-5” DSM, the DSM-IV-TR (text revision) reflects what mental disorders all together. This may be the information we have gained in mental health and cause of some of the controversy, however hope- Natalie Vasilj, our newest knowledge and research on it. In past fully with new research the DSM-5 will better rep- “Mental Health revisions major changes have been made because resent mental health.
    [Show full text]
  • Optical Illusion Art As Radical Interface
    Perceptual Play: Optical Illusion Art as Radical Interface Julian Oliver, 2008 In 1992 Joachim Sauter and Dirk Lüsebrink presented a work called Zerseher ('Iconoclast') at Ars Electronica, where they were awarded a prize in the Interactive Art category. In this work, audiences were invited to destroy a (digital) copy of an antique painting just by looking at it. Zerseher provides a context for direct manipulation of the visible work. The position and orientation of the viewer's eyes are tracked using a camera which, in turn, directs a digital brush over the surface of the image, smearing pixels around. It's not hard to see why the piece was awarded; here the premise explored in philosophy and poetry, that the gaze might have a mutating or productive power of its own, was explicitly manifested: the mere act of looking had the power to alter (or destroy) the artefact itself. Historically speaking, the idea that the gaze might be a primally destabilising force may have arisen from the fact that seeing itself is not always reliable; that the act of seeing – and perceiving what is seen – is something to be suspicious of. Many works of philosophy and poetry have asserted the primary fragility of perception, particularly as related to the ocular sense. This can be traced back as far as Plato in Western thought, who was early in identifying that what is seen propagates as an object of thought – a mental image – separated from the world and that it is there that interpretation occurs: "The image stands at the junction of a light which comes from the object and another which comes from the gaze".1 In recent times, the study of perception2 has been as active in scienctific thought as it has in philosophy.
    [Show full text]
  • Visual Prostheses for the Blind
    Review Visual prostheses for the blind 1,2 1,2 1,2,3 Robert K. Shepherd , Mohit N. Shivdasani , David A.X. Nayagam , 1,2 1,2 Christopher E. Williams , and Peter J. Blamey 1 Bionics Institute, 384-388 Albert St East Melbourne, 3002, Victoria, Australia 2 Medical Bionics Department, University of Melbourne, 384-388 Albert St East Melbourne, 3002, Victoria, Australia 3 Department of Pathology, University of Melbourne, Parkville, 3010, Victoria, Australia After more than 40 years of research, visual prostheses It is estimated that 285 million people are visually are moving from the laboratory into the clinic. These impaired worldwide; 39 million of whom are blind [2]. devices are designed to provide prosthetic vision to the Although uncorrected refractive errors are the main cause blind by stimulating localized neural populations in one of visual impairment, diseases associated with degenera- of the retinotopically organized structures of the visual tion of the retinal photoreceptors result in severe visual pathway – typically the retina or visual cortex. The long loss with few or no therapeutic options for ongoing clinical gestation of this research reflects the many significant management. Importantly, significant numbers of RGCs technical challenges encountered including surgical ac- are spared following the loss of photoreceptors. Although cess, mechanical stability, hardware miniaturization, there are major alterations to the neural circuitry of these hermetic encapsulation, high-density electrode arrays, surviving neurons [3], their presence provides the potential and signal processing. This review provides an introduc- to restore vision using electrical stimulation delivered by tion to the pathophysiology of blindness; an overview of an electrode array located close to the retina (Box 1).
    [Show full text]
  • BYU Museum of Art M. C. Escher: Other Worlds Education Resources
    BYU Museum of Art M. C. Escher: Other Worlds Education Resources Escher Terminology Crystallography: A branch of science that examines the structures and properties of crystals; greatly influenced the development of Escher’s tessellations de Mesquita, S. J.: A master printmaker at the School for Architecture and Decorative Arts, where Escher was studying, who encouraged Escher to pursue art rather than architecture H. S. M. Coxeter: A mathematician whose geometric “Coxeter Groups” became known as tessellations. Like Escher, Coxeter also loved music and its mathematical properties. The initial ideas for Circle Limit came from Coxeter. Infinity/Droste Effect: An image appearing within itself in smaller and smaller versions on to infinity; for example, when one mirror placed in front and one behind, creating an in infinite tunnel of the image Lithograph: A printing process in which the image to be printed is drawn on a flat stone surface and treated to hold ink while the negative space is treated to repel ink. Mezzotint: A method of engraving a copper or steel plate by scraping and burnishing areas to produce effects of light and shadow. Möbius strips: A surface that has only one side and one edge, making it impossible to orient; often used to symbolize eternity; used to how red ants is Escher’s Möbius Strip II Necker cube: An optical illusion proposed by Swiss crystallographer Louis Albert Necker, where the drawing of a cube has no visual cues as to its orientation; in Escher’s Belvedere, the Necker cube becomes an “impossible” cube. 1
    [Show full text]
  • Imaging the Foveal Cone Mosaic with a MEMS-Based Adaptive Optics Scanning Laser Ophthalmoscope
    Imaging the Foveal Cone Mosaic with a MEMS-based Adaptive Optics Scanning Laser Ophthalmoscope By Yiang Li A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Vision Science in the Graduate Divisions of the University of California, Berkeley Committee in charge: Professor Austin J. Roorda Professor Masayoshi Tomizuka Professor Martin S. Banks Fall 2010 Imaging the Foveal Cone Mosaic with a MEMS-based Adaptive Optics Scanning Laser Ophthalmoscope Copyright 2010 By Yiang Li University of California, Berkeley Abstract Imaging the foveal cone mosaic with a MEMS-based adaptive optics scanning laser ophthalmoscope By Yiang Li Doctor of Philosophy in Vision Science University of California, Berkeley Professor Austin J. Roorda, Chair Our knowledge of the structure of the human photoreceptor mosaic is mostly based on histological data. Imaging microscopic structure in intact eyes has traditionally been difficult due to structural imperfections in the eye’s optics called aberrations. The introduction of adaptive optics (AO) into vision science has allowed us to access the living human retina at microscopic levels, opening up new possibilities for both basic and clinical research. This dissertation concerns the advancement of AO technology for retinal imaging while emphasizing its application to imaging the foveal cone photoreceptor mosaic in living human eyes. Foveal cones provide a fundamental challenge for today’s AO systems due to their small size (2 µm diameter). As a result, much of my effort has been put towards improving AO system performance to resolve these small cells consistently. I have improved the wavefront correction capabilities of an adaptive optics scanning laser ophthalmoscope (AOSLO) using a single MEMS deformable mirror, so that the smallest foveal cones in some eyes can now be resolved.
    [Show full text]
  • Why Not Try Some of Our Fun Science Experiments?
    FUN SCIENCE EXPERIMENTS WHAT ARE YOU DOING DURING YOUR EXTRA TIME AT HOME? WHY NOT TRY SOME OF OUR FUN SCIENCE EXPERIMENTS? Whether we are drilling holes to determine the structural strength of the soil or surveying endangered plants and animals, we use science every day at RSK to explore the world around us. And we all started out as curious kids like you. CLICK ON A TOPIC BELOW… 1. Centre of gravity 2. Pinhole camera 3. Blind spot 4. Binocular vision 5. Optical illusions 6. Air pressure 7. Water pressure 8. Acids and alkalis FUN SCIENCE EXPERIMENTS We’ve teamed up with our friends to help budding explorers find out about our world. 1. Finding the centre of gravity WHAT IS GRAVITY? Gravity is a force of attraction between all objects, everywhere in the universe. It’s what pulls you to the ground, whether you’re in the UK or on the other side of the world in New Zealand. WHAT IS THE CENTRE OF GRAVITY? The centre of gravity is the average location of the weight of an object. (Weight describes the force between an object, or mass, and the centre of the Earth.) Think of an oil drum. As long as its centre of gravity lies inside the base of the drum, it will right itself. If its centre of gravity lies outside the base, it will topple over. CENTRE OF GRAVITY BANG! WHY DOES IT MATTER? Here’s one reason. Can you think of other examples? JUST A BIT HIGHER! CENTRE OF GRAVITY CAN YOU FIND THE CENTRE OF GRAVITY? 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Constructing Drawings of Impossible Figures with Axonometric Blocks and Pseudo-3D Manipulations
    Proceedings of Bridges 2014: Mathematics, Music, Art, Architecture, Culture Constructing Drawings of Impossible Figures with Axonometric Blocks and Pseudo-3D Manipulations Tiffany C. Inglis David R. Cheriton School of Computer Science University of Waterloo [email protected] Abstract Impossible figures are found in graphical designs, mathematical art, and puzzle games. There are various techniques for constructing these figures both in 2D and 3D, but most involve tricks that are not easily generalizable. We describe a simple framework that uses axonometric blocks for construction and permits pseudo-3D manipulations even though the figure may not have a real 3D counterpart. We use this framework to create impossible figures with complex structures and decorative patterns. Introduction Impossible figures [1, 6] are optical illusions involving objects that can be drawn in 2D but are infeasible in 3D (i.e., cannot be physically constructed). Figure 1a shows the impossible cube and the Penrose triangle, both of which have geometric requirements that are impossible to satisfy in 3D, and the blivet, which relies on negative space to create an illusion. Impossible figures are also featured in many of M. C. Escher’s lithographs [7], such as the Penrose stairs—an ever-ascending staircase—that is featured in both Ascending and Descending and Waterfall. Many of these impossible figures resemble 3D objects that can be constructed out of blocks under parallel projec- tion. It seems natural that one should be able to apply the same block manipulations used to construct real 3D objects to construct these impossible figures. However, since impossible figures have no 3D counterparts, defining a set of pseudo-3D manipulations is non-trivial.
    [Show full text]