Nature, Nurture, and Development: from Evangelism Through Science Toward Policy and Practice

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Nature, Nurture, and Development: from Evangelism Through Science Toward Policy and Practice Child Development, January/February 2002, Volume 73, Number 1, Pages 1–21 Nature, Nurture, and Development: From Evangelism through Science toward Policy and Practice Michael Rutter During the second half of the 20th century there was an immense increase in both empirical findings on, and conceptual understanding of, the effects of nature, nurture, and developmental processes on psychological functioning—both normal and abnormal. Unfortunately, the good science has also been accompanied by ex- cessive polarizing claims and by unwarranted extrapolations. This article provides a summary review of the real gains in knowledge, outlines some of the misleading claims, and notes the potential for research and for science-led improvements in policies and practice. The need to bring about a better interpretation of genetic, psychosocial, and developmental research strategies and theoretical concepts is emphasized. INTRODUCTION that may be occurring. To a considerable extent, it is the failure to do so that has led to many of the polar- Over the last half century, there has been an explosion izing battles and absurd claims. of knowledge on the effects of nature, nurture, and developmental processes. As a result, we have a much improved understanding of many of the mechanisms NATURE: GENETIC RISK AND involved in normal and abnormal development, PROTECTIVE MECHANISMS which carries with it a huge potential for improving children’s lives. Unfortunately, these advances have With regard to the influences that reflect nature, been accompanied by as much misleading scientific quantitative genetics and molecular genetics are dis- evangelism and journalistic hype as by good science cussed separately, because they have rather different and honest reporting. As a consequence, both the pages patterns of strengths and limitations. of scientific journals and the media have been full of the most absurd confrontations and polarizations. These Quantitative Genetics have given rise to an unhelpful level of misunder- standing of the true scientific advances and, more es- Quantitative genetics uses various population de- pecially, about their meaning and the implications for signs (most particularly twin and adoptee studies) to policy and practice. Of course, there have also been quantify the relative strength of genetic and nonge- numerous examples of good reporting by scientists netic factors with respect to population variance; that and by journalists. There is every reason to be in- is, individual differences with respect to some trait or debted to both. The need is to avoid the twin dangers disorder (McGuffin & Rutter, in press; Rutter et al., of destructive cynicism and gullible expectation. 1990; Rutter, Silberg, O’Connor, & Simonoff, 1999a). Nature, nurture, and development are dealt with Nongeneticists tend to assume that the only focus is in this article as separate topics (with the focus being on the heritability quotient, but that is not the case. mainly on their effects on psychopathology). In each The available techniques are able to partition genetic case, the real advances in knowledge are considered influences into those that are additive—that is, due to first, some of the misleading claims are outlined sec- a mixture of many genes without there being any ond, and the potential for research and for improve- requirement for a particular pattern or combination— ments in policies and practice are noted third. Al- and those that are nonadditive—that is, reflect interac- though these are considered as supposedly separate tions among different genes (epistasis) or among dif- influences, the truth is that they are closely inter- ferent alleles of the same gene (dominance). Similarly, twined. The separation is heuristically useful for test- nongenetic influences can be subdivided into so-called ing causal hypotheses, but it is crucial that such hy- shared and nonshared effects—those that tend to potheses deal with the different forms of interplay make siblings similar and those that tend to make 2001 Presidential Address to the Society for Research in © 2002 by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc. Child Development. All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2002/7301-0001 2 Child Development them unalike, respectively. Gene–environment corre- timate variations (surprising though that may seem), lations and interactions may also be identified. Al- because all indicate substantial genetic effects and the though traditional analyses have, in the past, tended figures are population specific; that is, they apply to assume that genetic and nongenetic influences are only to the particular samples studied at a particular entirely separable, there is no need to make that as- time. Even a heritability as high as 90% does not mean sumption. Quantitative genetic studies have increas- that changed environmental conditions could not ingly tested for, and found, major interplay between make a huge impact. This is not just speculation; the genetic and non-genetic factors, such that the out- findings with respect to height both in childhood and comes cannot sensibly be attributed to just one or the in adult life show the reality (Tizard, 1975). Height is other, because they depend on both (Rutter & Silberg, one of the most strongly genetically influenced of all in press). human characteristics, but it has increased enor- Quantitative genetic designs rely on many as- mously over the course of the 20th century (Kuh, sumptions, and it is crucial that these be put to the test Power, & Rodgers, 1991; van Wieringen, 1986), almost in a rigorous fashion, making explicit just which are certainly due to improved nutrition. Genetic influ- necessary (Rutter, Pickles, Murray, & Eaves, 2001; Rut- ences on the timing of the menarche are also strong ter et al., 1999a). Critics have rightly pointed to prob- but, again, the age of menarche has fallen greatly over lems, but any dispassionate reading of the evidence the last 100 years or so (Tanner, 1962). It is clear that leads to the inescapable conclusion that genetic fac- environmental factors can bring about major changes tors play a substantial role in the origins of individual in features that are strongly genetically influenced. differences with respect to all psychological traits, Despite this reservation, we may conclude that both normal and abnormal (McGuffin & Rutter, in there can be no doubt about the importance of genetic press; Rutter, Silberg, O’Connor, & Simonoff, 1999b). (as well as experiential) influences on individual dif- In a few cases (such as with autism, schizophrenia, bi- ferences, especially with respect to persistent traits polar disorder, and attention deficit disorder with hy- and chronic or recurrent disorders. Genetic influences peractivity), genetic factors account for most of the are particularly strong with respect to several mental variance in populations—over 70%. For the great ma- disorders, where the evidence from other biological jority of psychological characteristics, however, the studies indicates the likelihood that brain abnormali- genetic effects are not as strong. Thus, the heritabili- ties are implicated in the causal processes. This applies, ties of unipolar depression, delinquency, and parent- for example, to autism (Bailey, Phillips, & Rutter, 1996; ing qualities are in the 20% to 40% range. Lord & Bailey, in press) and schizophrenia (Keshavan Two different answers can and should be given to & Murray, 1997), and, in both cases, the quantitative the question of how much faith we can place on these genetic findings point to the likelihood that several estimates. First, there is every reason to believe that interacting genes are involved—that is, synergistic they are close to correct. This conclusion is based on interaction among genes may be implicated. the fact that numerous studies have produced broadly Strengths and achievements. Several other, rather dif- comparable findings; that substantial genetic effects ferent aspects of the genetic findings warrant empha- are evident from twin, adoptee, and family data; and sis. To begin, it is necessary to note the pervasiveness of careful consideration of the findings indicates that genetic influences across all psychological traits, even nongenetic explanations for the pattern of results those involving attitudes or social behavior (Plomin, found lack plausibility. 1994). Thus, genetic effects have been found for fea- The second answer, however, is that the estimates tures as diverse as divorce (Jockin, McGue, & Lykken, must be regarded as very approximate. This is because 1996; McGue & Lykken, 1992), religiosity (Eaves, the precise figures produced by different investiga- D’Onofrio, & Russell, 1999), and various aspects of tions are often rather different, and different methods parenting style (Kendler, 1996a). Critics have been quick often come up with somewhat discrepant findings. to scorn such findings, arguing that it is ridiculous to The use of parent reports and child reports, and of suppose that there could be a gene for divorce or crime twin and adoptee data, have made these points appar- (Rose, 1995, 1998). They are right, of course, that it is ent. In the past, arguments have raged over whether extremely unlikely that such a gene could ever occur. the heritability of, for example, IQ is this figure or that They also point to the indefinite boundaries of the figure, but the important thing to bear in mind is that phenotype, or behavior, being studied. Both argu- it does not really matter. With respect to some traits, ments, however, miss the point completely. The mes- there is real doubt whether the heritability in the pop- sage is that the workings of the mind are based on the ulations studied is 20% or 40% or 60%. There are no functioning of the brain, and that genetic influences theoretical or policy implications from such wide es- apply to individual differences on all somatic features. Rutter 3 It must be anticipated, therefore, that there will be a ge- genetically vulnerable and the most impact on the ge- netic effect on all behaviors. Biologically speaking, this netically susceptible. is exactly what one would expect, and what has been The last example of a specific finding of importance found.
Recommended publications
  • Specificity and Heterogeneity in Children's Responses to Profound
    BRITISH JOURNAL OF PSYCHIATRY !2001), 179, 97^103 Specificity and heterogeneity in children's selected at random from within bands stratified according to age of entry to the responses to profound institutional privation{{ UK, and a comparison group of 52 non- deprived within-UK adoptees placed before the age of 6 months for details see Rutter MICHAEL L. RUTTER, JANA M. KREPPNER and THOMAS G. O'CONNOR et aletal, 1998,1998).). Of these, 156 children from on behalf of the English and Romanian Adoptees $ERA) study team* Romania and 50 within-UK adoptees had complete data-sets and were used for analyses where these were required. Four- fifths of the Romanian adoptees had been reared in institutions for most of their life and nearly all had been admitted to Background The sequelae of Studies of children suffering profound institutions in the neonatal period mean profound early privation are varied. institutional privation in infancy and early age of admission was 0.34 months). The childhood have shown that it results in major conditions in the Romanian institutions Aims Todelineate the behavioural developmental impairment, that there is were generally extremely bad see Kaler & patterns that are specifically associated considerable developmental catch-up follow- Freeman, 1994;Castle et aletal, 1999;Johnson, ingingadoption into well-functioning families, 2001);and the condition of most of the with institutional privation. and that there are clinically significantficantsigni children on arrival in the UK was also very MethodMethod A group of165 children sequelae in some of the children Chisholm poor. Their mean weight was 2.4 standard et aletal, 1995;Ames, 1997;Fisher et aletal, 1997;,1997; deviations below the mean with over half adopted from Romania before the age Chisholm, 1998;Rutter et aletal, 1998, 2001; below the third percentile) and the retro- of 42 months were compared at 4 years O'ConnorO'Connor et aletal, 2000).
    [Show full text]
  • Lineage-Specific Mapping of Quantitative Trait Loci
    Heredity (2013) 111, 106–113 & 2013 Macmillan Publishers Limited All rights reserved 0018-067X/13 www.nature.com/hdy ORIGINAL ARTICLE Lineage-specific mapping of quantitative trait loci C Chen1 and K Ritland We present an approach for quantitative trait locus (QTL) mapping, termed as ‘lineage-specific QTL mapping’, for inferring allelic changes of QTL evolution along with branches in a phylogeny. We describe and analyze the simplest case: by adding a third taxon into the normal procedure of QTL mapping between pairs of taxa, such inferences can be made along lineages to a presumed common ancestor. Although comparisons of QTL maps among species can identify homology of QTLs by apparent co- location, lineage-specific mapping of QTL can classify homology into (1) orthology (shared origin of QTL) versus (2) paralogy (independent origin of QTL within resolution of map distance). In this light, we present a graphical method that identifies six modes of QTL evolution in a three taxon comparison. We then apply our model to map lineage-specific QTLs for inbreeding among three taxa of yellow monkey-flower: Mimulus guttatus and two inbreeders M. platycalyx and M. micranthus, but critically assuming outcrossing was the ancestral state. The two most common modes of homology across traits were orthologous (shared ancestry of mutation for QTL alleles). The outbreeder M. guttatus had the fewest lineage-specific QTL, in accordance with the presumed ancestry of outbreeding. Extensions of lineage-specific QTL mapping to other types of data and crosses, and to inference of ancestral QTL state, are discussed. Heredity (2013) 111, 106–113; doi:10.1038/hdy.2013.24; published online 24 April 2013 Keywords: Quantitative trait locus mapping; evolution of mating systems; Mimulus INTRODUCTION common ancestor of these two taxa, back to the common ancestor of Knowledge of the genetic architecture of complex traits can offer all three taxa and forwards again to the third taxon.
    [Show full text]
  • Pleiotropic Scaling and QTL Data Arising From: G
    NATURE | Vol 456 | 4 December 2008 BRIEF COMMUNICATIONS ARISING Pleiotropic scaling and QTL data Arising from: G. P. Wagner et al. Nature 452, 470–473 (2008) Wagner et al.1 have recently introduced much-needed data to the debate on how complexity of the genotype–phenotype map affects the distribution of mutational effects. They used quantitative trait loci (QTLs) mapping analysis of 70 skeletal characters in mice2 and regressed the total QTL effect on the number of traits affected (level of pleiotropy). From their results they suggest that mutations with higher pleiotropy have a larger effect, on average, on each of the T affected traits—a surprising finding that contradicts previous models3–7. We argue that the possibility of some QTL regions con- taining multiple mutations, which was not considered by the authors, introduces a bias that can explain the discrepancy between one of the previously suggested models and the new data. Wagner et al.1 define the total QTL effect as vffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi u uXN 10 20 30 ~t 2 T Ai N i~1 Figure 1 | Impact of multiple mutations on the total QTL effect, T. For all where N measures pleiotropy (number of traits) and Ai the normal- mutations, strict scaling according to the Euclidean superposition model is ized effect on the ith trait. They compare an empirical regression of T assumed. Black filled circles, single-mutation QTLs. Open circles, QTLs with on N with predictions from two models for pleiotropic scaling. The two mutations that affect non-overlapping traits. Red filled circles, total Euclidean superposition model3,4 assumes that the effect of a muta- effects of double-mutant QTL with overlapping trait ranges (see Methods).
    [Show full text]
  • 1 ROBERT PLOMIN on Behavioral Genetics Genetic Links Common
    Genetic links • Background – Why has educational psychology ben so slow ROBERT PLOMIN to accept genetics? on Behavioral Genetics • Beyond nature vs. nurture – 3 findings from genetic research with far- reaching implications for education: October, 2004 The nature of nurture The abnormal is normal Generalist genes • Molecular Genetics Quantitative genetics: twin and adoption studies Common medical disorders Identical twins Fraternal twins (monozygotic, MZ) (dizygotic, DZ) 1 Twin concordances in TEDS at 7 years Twins Early Development Study (TEDS) for low reading and maths Perceptions of nature/nurture: % parents (N=1340) and teachers Beyond nature vs. nurture: (N=667) indicating that genes are at least as important as environment (Walker & Plomin, Ed. Psych., in press) The nature of nurture • Genetic influence on experience • Genotype-environment correlation – Children’s genetic propensities are correlated with their experiences passive evocative active 2 Beyond nature vs. nurture: Genotype-environment correlation The nature of nurture • Passive • Genotype-environment correlation – e.g., family background (SES) and school – Children’s genetic propensities are correlated with achievement their experiences passive • Evocative evocative – Children evoke experiences for genetic reasons Active • Active • Molecular genetics – Children actively create environments that foster – Find genes associated with learning experiences their genetic propensities Beyond nature vs. nurture: The one-gene one-disability (OGOD) model The abnormal is normal • Common learning disabilities are the quantitative extreme of the same genetic and environmental factors responsible for normal variation in learning abilities. • There are no common learning disabilities, just the extremes of normal variation. 3 OGOD (rare) and QTL (common) disorders The quantitative trait locus (QTL) model Beyond nature vs.
    [Show full text]
  • Epigenome-Wide Study Identified Methylation Sites Associated With
    nutrients Article Epigenome-Wide Study Identified Methylation Sites Associated with the Risk of Obesity Majid Nikpay 1,*, Sepehr Ravati 2, Robert Dent 3 and Ruth McPherson 1,4,* 1 Ruddy Canadian Cardiovascular Genetics Centre, University of Ottawa Heart Institute, 40 Ruskin St–H4208, Ottawa, ON K1Y 4W7, Canada 2 Plastenor Technologies Company, Montreal, QC H2P 2G4, Canada; [email protected] 3 Department of Medicine, Division of Endocrinology, University of Ottawa, the Ottawa Hospital, Ottawa, ON K1Y 4E9, Canada; [email protected] 4 Atherogenomics Laboratory, University of Ottawa Heart Institute, Ottawa, ON K1Y 4W7, Canada * Correspondence: [email protected] (M.N.); [email protected] (R.M.) Abstract: Here, we performed a genome-wide search for methylation sites that contribute to the risk of obesity. We integrated methylation quantitative trait locus (mQTL) data with BMI GWAS information through a SNP-based multiomics approach to identify genomic regions where mQTLs for a methylation site co-localize with obesity risk SNPs. We then tested whether the identified site contributed to BMI through Mendelian randomization. We identified multiple methylation sites causally contributing to the risk of obesity. We validated these findings through a replication stage. By integrating expression quantitative trait locus (eQTL) data, we noted that lower methylation at cg21178254 site upstream of CCNL1 contributes to obesity by increasing the expression of this gene. Higher methylation at cg02814054 increases the risk of obesity by lowering the expression of MAST3, whereas lower methylation at cg06028605 contributes to obesity by decreasing the expression of SLC5A11. Finally, we noted that rare variants within 2p23.3 impact obesity by making the cg01884057 Citation: Nikpay, M.; Ravati, S.; Dent, R.; McPherson, R.
    [Show full text]
  • QUANTITATIVE TRAIT LOCUS ANALYSIS: in Several Variants (I.E., Alleles)
    TECHNOLOGIES FROM THE FIELD QUANTITATIVE TRAIT LOCUS ANALYSIS: in several variants (i.e., alleles). QTL analysis allows one MULTIPLE CROSS AND HETEROGENEOUS to map, with some precision, the genomic position of STOCK MAPPING these alleles. For many researchers in the alcohol field, the break­ Robert Hitzemann, Ph.D.; John K. Belknap, Ph.D.; through with respect to the genetic determination of alcohol- and Shannon K. McWeeney, Ph.D. related behaviors occurred when Plomin and colleagues (1991) made the seminal observation that a specific panel of RI mice (i.e., the BXD panel) could be used to identify KEY WORDS: Genetic theory of alcohol and other drug use; genetic the physical location of (i.e., to map) QTLs for behavioral factors; environmental factors; behavioral phenotype; behavioral phenotypes. Because the phenotypes of the different strains trait; quantitative trait gene (QTG); inbred animal strains; in this panel had been determined for many alcohol- recombinant inbred (RI) mouse strains; quantitative traits; related traits, researchers could readily apply the strategy quantitative trait locus (QTL) mapping; multiple cross mapping of RI–QTL mapping (Gora-Maslak et al. 1991). Although (MCM); heterogeneous stock (HS) mapping; microsatellite investigators recognized early on that this panel was not mapping; animal models extensive enough to answer all questions, the emerging data illustrated the rich genetic complexity of alcohol- related phenotypes (Belknap 1992; Plomin and McClearn ntil well into the 1990s, both preclinical and clinical 1993). research focused on finding “the” gene for human The next advance came with the development of diseases, including alcoholism. This focus was rein­ U microsatellite maps (Dietrich et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Aetiology of Autism: Findings and Questions*
    Journal of Intellectual Disability Research 231 pp – Blackwell Science, LtdOxford, UKJIRJournal of Intellectual Disability Research-Blackwell Publishing Ltd, 4231238Original ArticleAetiology of autismM. Rutter invited review Aetiology of autism: findings and questions* M. Rutter SGDP Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Denmark Hill, London, UK Abstract Keywords autism spectrum disorders, epidemiology, genetic influences, incidence, measles- Background Although there is good evidence that mumps-rubella vaccine autism is a multifactorial disorder, an adequate understanding of the genetic and non-genetic causes has yet to be achieved. Introduction Methods Empirical research findings and concep- tual reviews are reviewed with respect to evidence on Before turning to what is known on the aetiology of possible causal influences. autism, it is necessary to note that, over the last few Results Much the strongest evidence concerns the decades, there have been important changes in the importance of susceptibility genes, but such genes diagnostic concept. During the s, it was gener- have yet to be identified. Specific somatic conditions ally assumed that autism was a rare, seriously hand- (such as tuberous sclerosis and the fragile X anomaly) icapping disorder, usually associated with intellectual account for a small proportion of cases. Over recent disability (ID), constituting a condition that was decades there has been a major rise in the rate of qualitatively distinct from variations in social and diagnosed autism. The main explanation for this rise communicative competence within the normal range. is to be found in better ascertainment and a broad- Both epidemiological and genetic research findings ening of the diagnostic concept. Nevertheless, some have forced a change in concept as a result of the degree of true rise cannot be firmly excluded.
    [Show full text]
  • Article the Power to Detect Quantitative Trait Loci Using
    The Power to Detect Quantitative Trait Loci Using Resequenced, Experimentally Evolved Populations of Diploid, Sexual Organisms James G. Baldwin-Brown,*,1 Anthony D. Long,1 and Kevin R. Thornton1 1Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of California, Irvine *Corresponding author: E-mail: [email protected]. Associate editor: John Parsch Abstract A novel approach for dissecting complex traits is to experimentally evolve laboratory populations under a controlled environment shift, resequence the resulting populations, and identify single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) and/or genomic regions highly diverged in allele frequency. To better understand the power and localization ability of such an evolve and resequence (E&R) approach, we carried out forward-in-time population genetics simulations of 1 Mb genomic regions under a large combination of experimental conditions, then attempted to detect significantly diverged SNPs. Our analysis indicates that the ability to detect differentiation between populations is primarily affected by selection coef- ficient, population size, number of replicate populations, and number of founding haplotypes. We estimate that E&R studies can detect and localize causative sites with 80% success or greater when the number of founder haplotypes is over 500, experimental populations are replicated at least 25-fold, population size is at least 1,000 diploid individuals, and the selection coefficient on the locus of interest is at least 0.1. More achievable experimental designs (less replicated, fewer founder haplotypes, smaller effective population size, and smaller selection coefficients) can have power of greater than 50% to identify a handful of SNPs of which one is likely causative. Similarly, in cases where s 0.2, less demanding experimental designs can yield high power.
    [Show full text]
  • Glossary/Index
    Glossary 03/08/2004 9:58 AM Page 119 GLOSSARY/INDEX The numbers after each term represent the chapter in which it first appears. additive 2 allele 2 When an allele’s contribution to the variation in a One of two or more alternative forms of a gene; a single phenotype is separately measurable; the independent allele for each gene is inherited separately from each effects of alleles “add up.” Antonym of nonadditive. parent. ADHD/ADD 6 Alzheimer’s disease 5 Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder/Attention A medical disorder causing the loss of memory, rea- Deficit Disorder. Neurobehavioral disorders character- soning, and language abilities. Protein residues called ized by an attention span or ability to concentrate that is plaques and tangles build up and interfere with brain less than expected for a person's age. With ADHD, there function. This disorder usually first appears in persons also is age-inappropriate hyperactivity, impulsive over age sixty-five. Compare to early-onset Alzheimer’s. behavior or lack of inhibition. There are several types of ADHD: a predominantly inattentive subtype, a predomi- amino acids 2 nantly hyperactive-impulsive subtype, and a combined Molecules that are combined to form proteins. subtype. The condition can be cognitive alone or both The sequence of amino acids in a protein, and hence pro- cognitive and behavioral. tein function, is determined by the genetic code. adoption study 4 amnesia 5 A type of research focused on families that include one Loss of memory, temporary or permanent, that can result or more children raised by persons other than their from brain injury, illness, or trauma.
    [Show full text]
  • The Issue of Prevalence of Autism/ASD
    International Electronic Journal of Elementary Education, December 2016, 9(2), 263-306. The Issue of Prevalence of Autism/ASD Kamil ÖZERK a * a University of Oslo, Norway Received: May, 2016 / Revised: July, 2016/ Accepted: October, 2016 Abstract From a purely educationist perspective, gaining a deeper understanding of several aspects related to the prevalence of autism/ASD in a given population is of great value in planning and improving educational and psychological intervention for treatment, training, and teaching of children with this disorder. In this article, I present and discuss numerous facets of prevalence studies, beginning with assessing the changes in diagnostic manuals (DSM and ICD) over time. Based on the existing available research literature and empirical studies published during 2000 to 2016, I address the geographical- dimension and age-dimension of prevalence of autism/ASD. Over 50 studies from 21 countries reveals that prevalence rates of autism/ASD among children are on rise. There are inter-national and intra-national, regional/territorial variations with regard to prevalence rates, and I present and discuss possible factors/factor-groups that can explain these variations. Regardless of their geographic location, children with autism/ASD can be treated, trained, and taught, but to do so effectively requires reliable prevalence studies that can properly inform policy makers and higher institutions about the steps that must be taken in the field in order to improve the learning conditions of these children with special needs. Moving forward, it’s essential that studies of geographical dimension and age dimension of prevalence of autism/ASD must be supplemented by other (i.e.
    [Show full text]
  • Mapping Quantitative Trait Loci in Selected Breeding Populations: a Segregation Distortion Approach
    Heredity (2015) 115, 538–546 & 2015 Macmillan Publishers Limited All rights reserved 0018-067X/15 www.nature.com/hdy ORIGINAL ARTICLE Mapping quantitative trait loci in selected breeding populations: A segregation distortion approach Y Cui1,2, F Zhang1,JXu1,3,ZLi1 and S Xu2 Quantitative trait locus (QTL) mapping is often conducted in line-crossing experiments where a sample of individuals is randomly selected from a pool of all potential progeny. QTLs detected from such an experiment are important for us to understand the genetic mechanisms governing a complex trait, but may not be directly relevant to plant breeding if they are not detected from the breeding population where selection is targeting for. QTLs segregating in one population may not necessarily segregate in another population. To facilitate marker-assisted selection, QTLs must be detected from the very population which the selection is targeting. However, selected breeding populations often have depleted genetic variation with small population sizes, resulting in low power in detecting useful QTLs. On the other hand, if selection is effective, loci controlling the selected trait will deviate from the expected Mendelian segregation ratio. In this study, we proposed to detect QTLs in selected breeding populations via the detection of marker segregation distortion in either a single population or multiple populations using the same selection scheme. Simulation studies showed that QTL can be detected in strong selected populations with selected population sizes as small as 25 plants. We applied the new method to detect QTLs in two breeding populations of rice selected for high grain yield. Seven QTLs were identified, four of which have been validated in advanced generations in a follow-up study.
    [Show full text]
  • Pleiotropic Patterns of Quantitative Trait Loci for 70 Murine Skeletal Traits
    Copyright Ó 2008 by the Genetics Society of America DOI: 10.1534/genetics.107.084434 Pleiotropic Patterns of Quantitative Trait Loci for 70 Murine Skeletal Traits Jane P. Kenney-Hunt,*,1 Bing Wang,* Elizabeth A. Norgard,* Gloria Fawcett,* Doug Falk,* L. Susan Pletscher,* Joseph P. Jarvis,* Charles Roseman,† Jason Wolf‡ and James M. Cheverud* *Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, Missouri 63110, †Department of Anthropology, University of Illinois, Urbana, Illinois 61801 and ‡Faculty of Life Sciences, University of Manchester, Manchester M13 9PT, United Kingdom Manuscript received November 11, 2007 Accepted for publication February 1, 2008 ABSTRACT Quantitative trait locus (QTL) studies of a skeletal trait or a few related skeletal components are becoming commonplace, but as yet there has been no investigation of pleiotropic patterns throughout the skeleton. We present a comprehensive survey of pleiotropic patterns affecting mouse skeletal morphology in an intercross of LG/J and SM/J inbred strains (N ¼ 1040), using QTL analysis on 70 skeletal traits. We identify 798 single- trait QTL, coalescing to 105 loci that affect on average 7–8 traits each. The number of traits affected per locus ranges from only 1 trait to 30 traits. Individual traits average 11 QTL each, ranging from 4 to 20. Skeletal traits are affected by many, small-effect loci. Significant additive genotypic values average 0.23 standard deviation (SD) units. Fifty percent of loci show codominance with heterozygotes having intermediate phenotypic values. When dominance does occur, the LG/J allele tends to be dominant to the SM/J allele (30% vs.
    [Show full text]