Dos and Don'ts of Testing the Geographic Mosaic Theory

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Dos and Don'ts of Testing the Geographic Mosaic Theory DOS AND DON’TS OF TESTING THE GEOGRAPHIC MOSAIC 2 THEORY OF COEVOLUTION 4 Richard Gomulkiewicz*†‡, Devin M. Drown*, Mark F. Dybdahl*, William 6 Godsoe§, Scott L. Nuismer§, Kim M. Pepin§#, Benjamin J. Ridenhour§, Christopher Irwin Smith§, and Jeremy B. Yoder§ 8 10 *School of Biological Sciences, Washington State University §Department of Biology, University of Idaho 12 †Department of Mathematics, Washington State University #Current address: Department of Biology, New Mexico State University 14 ‡Corresponding Author. PO Box 644236, School of Biological Sciences, Washington State University, Pullman, WA 99164, phone: +1 (509) 335- 16 2527, fax: +1 (509) 335-3184, email: [email protected] 18 Key words: hot spot, cold spot, selection mosaic, trait remixing, local maladaptation 20 Running Head: Testing the geographic mosaic theory Main Text Word Count: 4814 1 ABSTRACT 2 The geographic mosaic theory of coevolution is stimulating much new research on interspecific interactions. We provide a guide to the fundamental 4 components of the theory, its processes, and main predictions. Our primary objectives are to clarify misconceptions regarding the geographic mosaic theory 6 of coevolution and to describe how empiricists can test the theory rigorously. In particular, we explain why confirming the three main predicted empirical 8 patterns (spatial variation in traits mediating interactions among species, trait mismatching among interacting species, and few species-level coevolved traits) 10 does not provide unequivocal support for the theory. We suggest that strong empirical tests of the geographic mosaic theory of coevolution should focus on 12 its underlying processes: coevolutionary hot and cold spots, selection mosaics, and trait remixing. We describe these processes and discuss potential ways 14 each can be tested. 16 2 INTRODUCTION 2 Coevolution among species and the impacts of geography on evolution have always been major research areas within evolutionary biology. However, 4 interest in studying coevolution in a geographic context has recently escalated. A search of the ISI Web of Science shows a dramatic increase over the last 6 fifteen years in the number of publications considering both coevolution and geography (Figure 1). Moreover, a growing portion of these papers cite the 8 publications of John N. Thompson, whose book “The Geographic Mosaic of Coevolution” (Thompson 2005) is the latest treatise on this emerging sub- Figure 1 10 discipline (Figure 1). Thompson’s book and papers provide a compendium of about here the empirical literature on interacting species whose ranges span broad and 12 variable landscapes. They also advance the geographic mosaic theory of coevolution, which is a particular thesis regarding the role of evolutionary and 14 ecological processes in determining patterns of coevolutionary outcome across geographical localities. 16 The geographic mosaic theory of coevolution (“GMTC”) hypothesizes that three processes are the primary drivers of coevolutionary dynamics: 18 intermingled coevolutionary hot and cold spots, selection mosaics, and trait remixing. For a pair of interacting species, a “hot spot” is a location where the 20 fitness of both species is affected by the distribution of traits in the other species. In contrast, in a “cold spot” the fitness of at least one of the species is 22 unaffected by the other. The term “selection mosaic” refers to variability in the functions that describe reciprocal fitness interactions across space. Finally, 3 “trait remixing” includes gene flow across landscapes, random genetic drift 2 within populations, extinction and recolonization of local populations, and mutation. The GMTC predicts these three processes lead to three observable 4 patterns: spatial variation in the traits mediating an inter-specific interaction, trait mismatching among interacting species, and few species-level coevolved 6 traits (Thompson 1999, 2005). Here, we aim to explain these hypothesized processes and predicted 8 patterns clearly, and highlight empirical approaches to use—and to avoid— when testing the geographic mosaic theory of coevolution. Note that this brief 10 review does not consider the historical context or development of the GMTC; interested readers should consult Thompson (2005). 12 VERIFYING PREDICTED PATTERNS IS NOT ENOUGH 14 The geographic mosaic theory of coevolution predicts that hot and cold spots, selection mosaics, and trait remixing should result in the three major 16 patterns of coevolution described above. Although mathematical models confirm that these predicted patterns follow from the assumptions of the GMTC 18 (e.g., Gomulkiewicz et al. 2000), identical patterns could be generated without invoking selection mosaics, trait remixing, or intermingled coevolutionary hot 20 and cold spots. We illustrate this point using the example of an antagonistic, cyclical interaction, although similar arguments can be readily developed for all 22 major forms of ecological interactions. 4 Spatially variable traits. The GMTC predicts that traits mediating 2 species interactions should be spatially variable, yet this pattern will almost inevitably be produced by antagonistic coevolution even in the absence of 4 selection mosaics, trait remixing or occasional cold spots. For example, take coevolution between a host and parasite mediated by quantitative traits in each 6 species (Figure 2). In the absence of gene flow, coevolutionary dynamics are likely to be out of phase among patches because of slight variations in initial 8 phenotype frequencies (Morand et al. 1996). Consequently, traits mediating the Figure 2 about here interaction are likely to vary across space (Figure 2). 10 Sensitivity to historical genotype frequencies also occurs in mutualistic and competitive interactions (Parker 1999; Nuismer et al. 2000; Thompson et al. 12 2004). Documenting spatial variation in traits mediating an interaction therefore does not by itself provide clear support for the GMTC over alternative 14 explanations. However, such observations may be critical in identifying traits of importance to an interaction, and may warrant further study in the context of 16 the GMTC. Trait mismatches. A second major pattern predicted by the GMTC is 18 that the traits mediating an interaction will be well matched in some localities but mismatched in others. This prediction has been evaluated in multiple 20 empirical studies by comparing the extent to which traits mediating an interaction mismatch across multiple locales (Brodie et al. 2002; Zangerl and 22 Berenbaum 2003; Siepielski and Benkman 2004). Evaluating whether the results of these studies support the GMTC, however, is made difficult by the 5 absence of any formal definition for a “match” or “mismatch” (Thompson 2 1994, 1999, 2005). An intuitive definition of matching can be made in terms of functional 4 relationships among coevolving traits. For example, pollinators and the flowers they pollinate must emerge at about the same; the functional relationship 6 between pollinator emergence time and plant flowering time thus defines trait matching. Such relationships have primarily been determined via regression 8 techniques (e.g., Benkman 1999; Brodie et al. 2002; Brodie and Ridenhour 2003; Toju and Sota 2006); the coefficient of determination (R2) then indicates 10 how well matched a pair of traits are across a geographic mosaic. Techniques used in determining allometric scaling relationships (such as reduced major- 12 axis regression; Sokal and Rohlf 1995) may be particularly useful in determining functional trait matching. Note, however, that in some cases trait 14 matching is not expected. For example, trait values should continually increase when "bigger is better" in the absence of fitness trade-offs (i.e. costs) and thus 16 trait matching should not be evident. An alternative, indirect approach to evaluating trait mismatching, is to 18 quantify levels of local maladaptation in interacting species (Nuismer 2006). Local maladaptation has been defined both verbally and mathematically, and 20 has been subject to extensive empirical and theoretical investigation (Gandon et al. 1996; Lively and Jokela 1996; Morand et al. 1996; Kaltz and Shykoff 1998; 22 Kaltz et al. 1999; Oppliger et al. 1999; Gandon and Michalakis 2002; Thrall et al. 2002; Dybdahl and Storfer 2003; Kawecki and Ebert 2004; Lively et al. 6 2004; Nuismer 2006). Local adaptation has been characterized theoretically 2 within two broad frameworks whose appropriateness is under debate: home versus away and local versus foreign (Kawecki and Ebert 2004). In contrast, 4 empirical approaches to determining local adaptation involve either reciprocal cross-infection or reciprocal transplant experimental designs. These designs 6 involve measuring and comparing the fitness of individuals within their natal and non-natal environments. Several studies have used such an approach to 8 document local maladaptation in at least one member of an interacting species pair (e.g., Morand et al. 1996; Kaltz and Shykoff 1998; Kaltz et al. 1999; 10 Oppliger et al. 1999). Neither trait mismatching nor local maladaptation provide a rigorous test of 12 the GMTC since both can evolve in the absence of selection mosaics, coevolutionary cold spots, or trait remixing. For example, Figure 2 shows there 14 can be strong temporal and spatial variation in the degree of trait mismatching despite lacking the central evolutionary processes invoked by the GMTC. The 16 same is true for local maladaptation which has been demonstrated
Recommended publications
  • UNIT 4 HISTORY of HUMAN EVOLUTION* History of Human Evolution
    UNIT 4 HISTORY OF HUMAN EVOLUTION* History of Human Evolution Contents 4.0 Introduction 4.1 Trends in Human Evolution: Understanding Pre-modern Humans 4.2 Hominization Process 4.2.1 Bipedalism 4.2.2 Opposable Thumb and Manual Dexterity 4.3 Summary 4.4 References 4.5 Answers to Check Your Progress Learning Objectives: After reading this unit you will be able to: analyze the major trends in human evolution; review characteristics which distinguish human from their primate ancestors; learn anatomical and cultural changes associated with the process of hominization; and comprehend the significance of these changes during evolution of human. 4.0 INTRODUCTION Humans first evolved in East Africa about 2.5 million years ago from an earlier genus of apes called Australopithecus, which means ‘Southern Ape’. About 2 million years ago, some of these archaic men and women left their homeland to journey through and settle vast areas of North Africa, Europe and Asia. Since survival in the snowy forests of northern Europe required different traits than those needed to stay alive in Indonesia’s steaming jungles, human populations evolved in different directions. The result was several distinct species, to each of which scientists have assigned a pompous Latin name. Humans in Europe and western Asia evolved into Homo neanderthalensis (‘Man from the Neander Valley’), popularly referred to simply as ‘Neandethals’. Neanderthals, bulkier and more muscular than us Sapiens, were well adapted to the cold climate of Ice Age western Eurasia. The more eastern regions of Asia were populated by Homo erects, ‘Upright Man’, who survived there for close to 2 million years, making it the most durable species ever.
    [Show full text]
  • Spatial Mosaic Evolution of Snail Defensive Traits
    University of New Orleans ScholarWorks@UNO Biological Sciences Faculty Publications Department of Biological Sciences 2007 Spatial Mosaic Evolution of Snail Defensive Traits Steve G. Johnson University of New Orleans, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.uno.edu/biosciences_facpubs Recommended Citation Johnson, S.G., C. Darrin Hulsey, Francisco J. Garcia de Leon. 2007. Spatial mosaic evolution of snail defensive traits. BMC Evolutionary Biology 7:50 (open access in pubmed central) This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Department of Biological Sciences at ScholarWorks@UNO. It has been accepted for inclusion in Biological Sciences Faculty Publications by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks@UNO. For more information, please contact [email protected]. BMC Evolutionary Biology BioMed Central Research article Open Access Spatial mosaic evolution of snail defensive traits Steven G Johnson*1, C Darrin Hulsey2 and Francisco J García de León3 Address: 1Department of Biological Sciences, University of New Orleans, 2000 Lake Shore Drive, New Orleans, LA, 70148 USA, 2Department of Biology, Georgia Tech, 310 Ferst Drive, Atlanta, Georgia, 30332, USA and 3Centro de Investigaciones Biologicas del Noroeste, P.O. Box 128, La Paz, B.C.S. Mexico Email: Steven G Johnson* - [email protected]; C Darrin Hulsey - [email protected]; Francisco J García de León - [email protected] * Corresponding author Published: 30 March 2007 Received: 26 February 2007 Accepted: 30 March 2007 BMC Evolutionary Biology 2007, 7:50 doi:10.1186/1471-2148-7-50 This article is available from: http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2148/7/50 © 2007 Johnson et al; licensee BioMed Central Ltd.
    [Show full text]
  • The Web of Life Evolution in Action
    The Web of Life Evolution in Action Presentations at the National Association of Biology Teachers Annual Conferences 1998 – 2001 by the Society for the Study of Evolution Society for Molecular Biology and Evolution Table of Contents On Observing Evolution .............................................................................................. 1 DefendingEvolution .................................................................................................... 5 Humans as the World’s Greatest................................................................................... 7 Evolutionary Force ...................................................................................................... 7 Evolution in Our Lives .............................................................................................. 18 Putting the Scientific Method into Biological Taxonomy – Teaching the Phylogenetic System................................................................................................... 20 SNPs : Why all the excitement? ................................................................................ 23 Macroevolution: Evolution on a big scale................................................................. 24 Why Evolution Matters .............................................................................................. 29 Applied Evolution: Technology for the 21st Century................................................. 34 DNA and Early Human HistoryNeandertals and Early Humans: But Did They Mate? ..................................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Magic Traits: Distinguishing the Important from the Trivial
    Letters Trends in Ecology and Evolution January 2012, Vol. 27, No. 1 Magic traits: distinguishing the important from the trivial Benjamin C. Haller1, Luis F. De Le´ on1,2, Gregor Rolshausen1, Kiyoko M. Gotanda1 and Andrew P. Hendry1 1 Department of Biology and Redpath Museum, McGill University, 859 Sherbrooke Street West, Montreal, Quebec, Canada, H3A 2K6 2 Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute, Apartado Postal 2072, Balboa, Panama´ Servedio et al. [1], following Gavrilets [2], define a magic implies that such a trait is, in a sense, an ordinary trait that trait as ‘a trait subject to divergent selection and a trait contributes to non-random mating, but that is, at times, in a contributing to non-random mating that are pleiotropic ‘magic environment’ that subjects it to divergent selection; expressions of the same gene(s)’. This clarified definition is the magic comes from the trait–environment interaction. certainly helpful, but we outline here several pivotal ques- Thus, a crucial question emerges: how consistently diver- tions for empirical research, particularly surrounding the gent, through time and across space, must selection be for a crucial concept of effect size. trait to be magic and also important for speciation? Again, The effect size of a magic trait, defined by Servedio et al. we argue that expected effect size is the key: divergent [1] as ‘how much the trait contributed to the evolution of selection must be sufficiently strong and consistent to actu- increased reproductive isolation’, determines whether a ally drive divergence. magic trait is actually important for speciation (an ‘impor- The second pillar of the definition is non-random mat- tant magic trait’) or is a ‘trivial magic trait’ (a magic trait of ing.
    [Show full text]
  • Mosaic Evolution Africa (Broom and Robinson 1947; Le Gros Clark 1947; Washburn and Patterson 1951)
    but bipedal Australopithecus fossils from South Mosaic evolution Africa (Broom and Robinson 1947; Le Gros Clark 1947; Washburn and Patterson 1951). In JEREMY M. DESILVA 1959, Wilfrid Le Gros Clark (see le gros clark, Dartmouth College, USA wilfrid edward) first applied the term mosaic evolution to describe this disjunction between brain and locomotor evolution in the australo- Different parts of a species’ biology evolve at dif- piths (Le Gros Clark 1959). It is now commonly ferent rates, resulting in organisms possessing a acceptedthattwoofthemostdistinctivehuman combination of primitive and derived characteris- characteristics—bipedalism and large brains tics. This differential pace of evolutionary change —evolved at different rates and at different times is commonly referred to as mosaic evolution.An in our lineage (McHenry 1975) and that the early exposition of this idea was put forward by disconnect between locomotion and encephal- W. K. Gregory (see gregory, william king) ization extends into the Pliocene (White 1980). (1910), who noted that organisms are a combi- Ever since Ernst Mayr (see mayr, ernst) cited nation of what he called caenotelic (derived) and the evolution of the Hominidae (see hominidae: paleotelic (primitive) features. Robert Broom (see conceptual history) as a “classic example” broom, robert) (1924) used the metaphor of (Mayr 1963, 344) of evolutionary mosaicism, the a palimpsest—the ancient practice of repeatedly term has been widely employed in the scientific writing and erasing text on the same piece of literature on human evolution. parchment—to refer to this phenomenon. The “Mosaic evolution” is also commonly used to concept was further elaborated and popularized refer to the sequential acquisition of evolutionary by G.
    [Show full text]
  • Artificial Mosaic Brain Evolution of Relative Telencephalon Size Improves Cognitive
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.04.03.438307; this version posted April 4, 2021. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license. 1 Artificial mosaic brain evolution of relative telencephalon size improves cognitive 2 performance in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata) 3 4 5 6 Authors: Zegni Triki1 *, Stephanie Fong1, Mirjam Amcoff1 and Niclas Kolm1 7 8 Affiliation: 9 1Department of Zoology, Stockholm University, Svante Arrheniusväg 18 B, Stockholm, Sweden 10 11 12 13 * Corresponding author: Zegni Triki, email: [email protected] 14 1 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.04.03.438307; this version posted April 4, 2021. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license. 15 Abstract 16 17 The telencephalon is a brain region believed to have played an essential role during cognitive 18 evolution in vertebrates. However, till now, all the evidence on the evolutionary association 19 between telencephalon size and cognition stem from comparative studies. To experimentally 20 investigate the potential evolutionary association between cognitive abilities and 21 telencephalon size, we used male guppies artificially selected for large and small 22 telencephalon relative to the rest of the brain. In a detour task, we tested a functionally 23 important aspect of executive cognitive ability; inhibitory control abilities.
    [Show full text]
  • Developmental Origins of Mosaic Evolution in the Avian Cranium
    Developmental origins of mosaic evolution in the SEE COMMENTARY avian cranium Ryan N. Felicea,b,1 and Anjali Goswamia,b,c aDepartment of Genetics, Evolution, and Environment, University College London, London WC1E 6BT, United Kingdom; bDepartment of Life Sciences, The Natural History Museum, London SW7 5DB, United Kingdom; and cDepartment of Earth Sciences, University College London, London WC1E 6BT, United Kingdom Edited by Neil H. Shubin, The University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, and approved December 1, 2017 (received for review September 18, 2017) Mosaic evolution, which results from multiple influences shaping genes. For example, manipulating the expression of Fgf8 generates morphological traits and can lead to the presence of a mixture of correlated responses in the growth of the premaxilla and palatine ancestral and derived characteristics, has been frequently invoked in in archosaurs (11). Similarly, variation in avian beak shape and describing evolutionary patterns in birds. Mosaicism implies the size is regulated by two separate developmental modules (7). hierarchical organization of organismal traits into semiautonomous Despite the evidence for developmental modularity in the avian subsets, or modules, which reflect differential genetic and develop- skull, some studies have concluded that the cranium is highly in- mental origins. Here, we analyze mosaic evolution in the avian skull tegrated (i.e., not subdivided into semiautonomous modules) (9, using high-dimensional 3D surface morphometric data across a 10, 12). In light of recent evidence that diversity in beak mor- broad phylogenetic sample encompassing nearly all extant families. phology may not be shaped by dietary factors (12), it is especially We find that the avian cranium is highly modular, consisting of seven critical to investigate other factors that shape the evolution of independently evolving anatomical regions.
    [Show full text]
  • Genetic Architecture Supports Mosaic Brain Evolution and Independent Brain–Body Size Regulation
    ARTICLE Received 8 May 2012 | Accepted 23 Aug 2012 | Published 25 Sep 2012 DOI: 10.1038/ncomms2086 Genetic architecture supports mosaic brain evolution and independent brain–body size regulation Reinmar Hager1,*, Lu Lu2,3,*, Glenn D. Rosen4 & Robert W. Williams2 The mammalian brain consists of distinct parts that fulfil different functions. Finlay and Darlington have argued that evolution of the mammalian brain is constrained by developmental programs, suggesting that different brain parts are not free to respond individually to selection and evolve independent of other parts or overall brain size. However, comparisons among mammals with matched brain weights often reveal greater differences in brain part size, arguing against strong developmental constraints. Here we test these hypotheses using a quantitative genetic approach involving over 10,000 mice. We identify independent loci for size variation in seven key parts of the brain, and observe that brain parts show low or no phenotypic correlation, as is predicted by a mosaic scenario. We also demonstrate that variation in brain size is independently regulated from body size. The allometric relations seen at higher phylogenetic levels are thus unlikely to be the product of strong developmental constraints. 1 Computational and Evolutionary Biology Group, Faculty of Life Sciences, University of Manchester, Oxford Road, Manchester M13 9PT, UK. 2 Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology, University of Tennessee Health Science Center, Memphis, Tennessee 38163, USA. 3 Jiangsu Key Laboratory of Neuroregeneration, Nantong University, China. 4 Department of Neurology, Beth Isreal Deaconess Medical Center, 330 Brookline Avenue, Boston, Massachusetts 02215, USA. *These authors contributed equally to this work. Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to R.H.
    [Show full text]
  • Quantitative Uniqueness of Human Brain Evolution Revealed Through Phylogenetic Comparative Analysis Ian F Miller1,2*, Robert a Barton3, Charles L Nunn2,4
    RESEARCH ARTICLE Quantitative uniqueness of human brain evolution revealed through phylogenetic comparative analysis Ian F Miller1,2*, Robert A Barton3, Charles L Nunn2,4 1Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, Princeton University, Princeton, United States; 2Department of Evolutionary Anthropology, Duke University, Durham, United States; 3Evolutionary Anthropology Research Group, Department of Anthropology, University of Durham, Durham, United Kingdom; 4Duke Global Health Institute, Duke University, Durham, United States Abstract While the human brain is clearly large relative to body size, less is known about the timing of brain and brain component expansion within primates and the relative magnitude of volumetric increases. Using Bayesian phylogenetic comparative methods and data for both extant and fossil species, we identified that a distinct shift in brain-body scaling occurred as hominins diverged from other primates, and again as humans and Neanderthals diverged from other hominins. Within hominins, we detected a pattern of directional and accelerating evolution towards larger brains, consistent with a positive feedback process in the evolution of the human brain. Contrary to widespread assumptions, we found that the human neocortex is not exceptionally large relative to other brain structures. Instead, our analyses revealed a single increase in relative neocortex volume at the origin of haplorrhines, and an increase in relative cerebellar volume in apes. DOI: https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.41250.001 *For correspondence: Introduction [email protected] Primates vary almost a thousand-fold in endocranial volume – a measure which closely approximates brain size – ranging from 1.63 mL in mouse lemurs (Isler et al., 2008) to 1478 mL in humans Competing interests: The (Robson and Wood, 2008).
    [Show full text]
  • Extreme Environmental Change and Evolution: Stress-Induced
    Extremeenvironmentalchangea ndevolution: stress-inducedmorphologicalvariationi sstrongly concordantwithpatternsof evolutionarydivergence inshrewmandibles AlexanderV .Badyaev *{ and KerryR. Foresman Division of Biological Sciences,The University of Montana, Missoula, MT 59812-1002,USA Morphologicalstructures oftenconsist ofsimpler traits whichcan be viewed as either integrated(e.g. correlateddue to functional interdependency) or non-integrated (e.g. functionally independent) traits. Thecombination of along-term stabilizingselection onthe entire structure witha short-term directional selection onan adaptively important subset oftraits shouldresult inlong historical persistence of integratedfunctional complexes, with environmentally induced variation and macroevolutionary change con¢ned mostly to non-integrated traits. Weexperimentallysubjected populationsof three closelyrelated species of Sorex shrews toenvironmental stress. Aspredicted, wefoundthat most ofthe variationin shrew mandibularshape was localized between rather thanwithin the functionalcomplexes; the patterns of integrationdid not change between the species. Thestress-induced variationwas con¢ ned to non- integratedtraits andwas highly concordant with the patterns ofevolutionarychange öspecies di¡ered in the same set ofnon-integrated traits whichwere most sensitive tostress withineach species. Wesuggest that lowenvironmental and genetic canalization of non-integrated traits mayhave caused these traits to bemost sensitive notonly to the environmentalbut alsoto genetic perturbations
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 15 Pa'iterns of Evolution in the Mammalian
    Reprinted from: "Patterns of Evolution" edited by A. Hallam C> 1977 Elsevier Scientific Publishing Company, Amsterdam CHAPTER 15 PA'ITERNS OF EVOLUTION IN THE MAMMALIAN FOSSIL RECORD PmLIP D. GINGERICH Introduction Mammals are the most successful and most intelligent of land vertebrates. While most mammals today remain terrestrial like the ancestral mammalian stock, one progressive group has invaded the air and others have invaded the sea. Living mammals differ rather sharply from other living vertebrates in bear­ ing their young alive, in suckling their young (hence the class name Mammalia). and in sometimes "educating" their young. Mammals are warm-blooded, or endothermic, and their high metabolic rates make possible a sustained high level of continuous activity. Osteologically, living mammals also differ sharply from most other vertebrat.es in possessing a double occipital condyle on the skull, a single mandibular bone (the dentary) which articulates directly with the squamosal bone of the cranium, an eardrum supported by an ossified ectotym­ panic bone, three auditory ossicles, only two tooth generations - deciduous artd pemu!.nent, cheek teeth of c.omplicated morphology, a single bony nasal opening in the skull, and epiphyses on the long bones. While living mammals are well separated from other groups of vertebrat.es today, the fossil record shows clearly their origin from a reptilian stock and permits one to trace the origin and radiation of mammals in considerable detail. If one could choose any one anatomical system of mammals for preservation in the fossil record, the system yielding the most information about the animals would undoubtedly be the dentition, and it is fortunate that this is the most commonly preserved element of fossil mammals.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolution, Science, and Society: Evolutionary Biology and the National Research Agenda
    . Evolution, Science, and Society: Evolutionary Biology and the National Research Agenda [working draft as of 28 September 1998] Prepared by: Editorial Chair: Douglas J. Futuyma (State University of New York -Stony Brook) Organizational Chair: Thomas R. Meagher (Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey) Steering Committee: Michael J. Donoghue (Harvard University) Charles H. Langley (University of California-Davis) Linda Maxson (University of Iowa) Working Group: Albert F. Bennett (University of California-Irvine) H. Jane Brockmann (University of Florida) Marcus W. Feldman (Stanford University) Walter M. Fitch (University of California-Irvine) Laurie R. Godfrey (University of Massachusetts) James Hanken (University of Colorado) David Jablonski (University of Chicago) Carol B. Lynch (University of Colorado) Leslie Real (Indiana University) Margaret A. Riley (Yale University) J. John Sepkoski, Jr. (University of Chicago) Vassiliki Betty Smocovitis (University of Florida) Under the sponsorship of: The A. P. Sloan Foundation The National Science Foundation Endorsed by the following scientific societies: American Society of Naturalists Animal Behavior Society [PENDING] Ecological Society of America Genetics Society of America Paleontological Society [PENDING] Society for Molecular Biology and Evolution [PENDING] Society of Systematic Biologists Society for the Study of Evolution [PENDING] 1 . CONTENTS PREAMBLE I. INTRODUCTION II. WHAT IS EVOLUTION? III. WHAT IS EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY? A. Subdisciplines of Evolutionary Biology B. Perspectives from Evolutionary Biology IV. HOW IS EVOLUTION STUDIED? V. SOME ACCOMPLISHMENTS OF EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY VI. CONTRIBUTIONS OF EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY TO THE BIOLOGICAL SCIENCES A. Molecular Biology B. Developmental Biology C. Physiology and Morphology D. Neurobiology and Behavior VII. EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY: MEETING SOCIETAL NEEDS A. Human Health and Medicine B.
    [Show full text]