Social Brain: Evolution 21

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Social Brain: Evolution 21 Social Brain: Evolution 21 Social Brain: Evolution R I M Dunbar, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK large neocortices are clearly a primate speciality – so ã 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. much so, in fact, that when one asks why primates have big brains, one is really in effect asking why they have big neocortices. In the following sections, the selective advantages of large brains are first discussed, in order to identify why Explaining Brain Evolution large brains have evolved in various animal lineages. Although neuroscientists do not often consider the Then the role that cognition plays in these processes is evolutionary history of the brain, understanding how considered. Finally, the question of whether these and why brains have evolved may help explain why forms of social cognition relate to specific brain struc- they have come to have the structure they do. Verte- tures is briefly explored. brate species, for example, vary enormously in both absolute brain size and brain size relative to body size, Theories for the Evolution of Large Brains but some species (notably primates) have dispropor- tionately large brains. Why should that be? Why do all We might expect large-bodied species to have large mammals not have primate-like brains? A second issue brains simply because they have larger muscle masses is that brain researchers have conventionally assumed to coordinate if they are to survive effectively. How- that the brain’s main function is to acquire and process ever, it has long been recognized that some groups of information about the environment, usually informa- species (e.g., primates) have unusually large brains for tion directly relevant to the business of survival (i.e., their body size compared to all other groups of verte- finding food and avoiding predators). While obviously brates. Why this should remain so is unclear, although broadly correct, this view contains an implicit assump- it has been generally assumed that it probably has tion that may be less defendable: the assumption that something to do with the ecology of these species. ‘environment’ here means the physical environment. Initial Explanations Of course, a great deal of brain activity is about the physical environment, and, in the final analysis, it is Initial attention was drawn to the fact that, within the physical properties of the environment that consti- many mammalian taxa, species with larger brains tute all perceptual inputs. have larger territories, or have diets that are more However, for more advanced vertebrates (birds and difficult to locate or process (e.g., diets of ephemeral mammals), and in particular the more intensely social fruits as opposed to more widely available leaves, or of these species, the environmental behavior that of invertebrates that have to be extracted from hiding needs to be processed by the brain is that of the places). Others have pointed to correlations with ‘social’ world. In the limiting case, the social world altriciality (giving birth to babies that are not mature is a virtual world of behavior, the nature of which is enough at birth to move and forage for themselves) understood and manipulated only in the animal’s and the scaling of maternal energy demands, and have mind. Because the dynamic complexities of this virtu- argued that it was spare maternal energy capacity al social world vastly exceed the complexities of the that made large brains possible. By the late 1980s, physical world, it is inevitably much harder to under- however, this assumption began to be questioned: it stand. Hence, intensely social species (such as pri- eventually became clear that the kinds of ecological mates) have brains that are specialized for dealing tasks faced by primates do not differ much from those with this aspect of their world. faced by other species. This realization begged the The emphasis on primates in the early literature on question as to why primates should need a substan- comparative brain evolution led to a natural focus on tially larger brain (in effect, more computing power) neocortex volume. The scaling relationships among to solve the same foraging problems as, say, cows. mammalian brain units are such that neocortex vol- The social brain hypothesis was proposed as an ume increases much faster than that of all other parts explanation. of the brain, and especially among the primates. The initial proposal, known as the ‘Machiavellian Although the neocortex accounts for as little as 10% intelligence hypothesis,’ was intended to explain why of total brain volume in some of the more primitive primates had larger brains for body size than all insectivores and at most 40% in nonprimate mam- other species. It was suggested that this might have mals, it accounts for a minimum of 50% in primates something to do with the fact that primates live in and rises to as high as 80% in humans. Thus, while the unusually complex social systems. Primate sociality is neocortex may be viewed as a mammalian invention, characterized by kinds of behaviors (for example, 22 Social Brain: Evolution tactical deception, the formation of coalitions and left with two main contenders that can broadly be alliances long before they are actually needed) that defined as ecological theories and social theories. are rare or entirely absent in nonprimate species. This Ecological hypotheses argue for a focus on foraging proposal was later renamed the ‘social brain hypo- decisions (how to extract nutrients from the environ- thesis’ (or ‘social intelligence hypothesis’) in order to ment, how to find one’s way about, etc.). In contrast, avoid unnecessary and unintended implications of the social hypotheses argue that the computational sociopolitical scheming and to focus attention more load is created by the demands of tracking social closely on the positive socially integrating aspects that relationships and calculating the pros and cons of really underpin primate sociality. alternative social actions. In both cases, of course, the fundamental issue is Asking the Right Questions the brain’s problem-solving capabilities, but the focus of the problem and the mode of solution are different An important consideration at the outset is to appreci- in each case. However, it is worth emphasizing that, ate that there is an important distinction between either way, the root issue is essentially ecological: all functional hypotheses (those that provide a reason animals have to overcome problems of day-to-day why large brains might be selected for) and explana- survival if they are to contribute offspring to the tions that are essentially about constraints. Most next generation (the root of all evolutionary process- developmental hypotheses fall into the latter category: es). The difference lies in how this is done, and which they identify trajectories along which brain evolution particular computational demand imposes the high- is forced to run, or system limitations that have to be est cognitive load. Ecological hypotheses assume that overcome. Examples of the first include the fact that the basis of an animal’s decision-making in this the way brain tissue is laid down necessarily results in respect is explicitly trial-and-error learning on an unavoidable scaling of all brain parts: if such scaling is individual basis; the social hypotheses assume that very tight, then it may result in all parts of the brain the ecological problems are solved cooperatively enlarging proportionately when evolution imposes (and that the limiting cognitive demand is created by selection on just one component. This might mean the need to coordinate one’s behavior with that of that in order to evolve more capacity in some key others in some way). brain component, it is necessary to evolve everything One way to test between these competing hypo- on a larger scale. Examples of the second kind of theses is to correlate some measure of relative brain limitation would be energetic constraints that limit size with one or more behavioral indices that repre- brain growth: some diets may not provide sufficient sent the hypothesis of interest. Relevant indices that surplus energy that can be channeled into brain have been used include the amount of fruit in the diet growth. Consequently, species that need to evolve a (representing the claim that the cognitive constraint larger brain may only be able to do so if they first lies in finding and monitoring high-quality food change diet in order to bypass the energetic constraint. sources such as fruiting trees), the style of extractive While these kinds of developmental hypotheses are foraging (whether or not food items are extracted perfectly legitimate and tell us something about how from some physical matrix, such as a pod, or dug large brains are evolved, they do not specify why they out of the ground, i.e., an index of complex food- should evolve. Given that brain tissue is among the processing decisions), day journey length and territo- most ‘expensive’ in the body (the so-called expensive ry size (indices of mental mapping capacities), and tissue hypothesis), to merely show that evolutionary social group size (a simple index of social complexity). change in a particular direction is possible is not When these indices have been correlated with indices enough. The costs of evolving (and indeed growing) of relative brain size, only group size is correlated a large brain are such that they provide an extremely with brain size (and specifically with neocortex size) steep gradient, up which selection has to push the (see Figure 1). organism. In the absence of any selective benefit (or More recently, a number of new analyses have been advantage), the costs impose strong negative selection undertaken that use new and more powerful statisti- that will constantly favor a reduction in size (or, at cal methods that allow the multivariate influences of best, a stable state).
Recommended publications
  • The Natural Science Underlying Big History
    Review Article [Accepted for publication: The Scientific World Journal, v2014, 41 pages, article ID 384912; printed in June 2014 http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2014/384912] The Natural Science Underlying Big History Eric J. Chaisson Harvard-Smithsonian Center for Astrophysics Harvard University, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02138 USA [email protected] Abstract Nature’s many varied complex systems—including galaxies, stars, planets, life, and society—are islands of order within the increasingly disordered Universe. All organized systems are subject to physical, biological or cultural evolution, which together comprise the grander interdisciplinary subject of cosmic evolution. A wealth of observational data supports the hypothesis that increasingly complex systems evolve unceasingly, uncaringly, and unpredictably from big bang to humankind. This is global history greatly extended, big history with a scientific basis, and natural history broadly portrayed across ~14 billion years of time. Human beings and our cultural inventions are not special, unique, or apart from Nature; rather, we are an integral part of a universal evolutionary process connecting all such complex systems throughout space and time. Such evolution writ large has significant potential to unify the natural sciences into a holistic understanding of who we are and whence we came. No new science (beyond frontier, non-equilibrium thermodynamics) is needed to describe cosmic evolution’s major milestones at a deep and empirical level. Quantitative models and experimental tests imply that a remarkable simplicity underlies the emergence and growth of complexity for a wide spectrum of known and diverse systems. Energy is a principal facilitator of the rising complexity of ordered systems within the expanding Universe; energy flows are as central to life and society as they are to stars and galaxies.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolution of Nervous Systems and Brains 2
    Evolution of Nervous Systems and Brains 2 Gerhard Roth and Ursula Dicke The modern theory of biological evolution, as estab- drift”) is incomplete; they point to a number of other lished by Charles Darwin and Alfred Russel Wallace and perhaps equally important mechanisms such as in the middle of the nineteenth century, is based on (i) neutral gene evolution without natural selection, three interrelated facts: (i) phylogeny – the common (ii) mass extinctions wiping out up to 90 % of existing history of organisms on earth stretching back over 3.5 species (such as the Cambrian, Devonian, Permian, and billion years, (ii) evolution in a narrow sense – Cretaceous-Tertiary mass extinctions) and (iii) genetic modi fi cations of organisms during phylogeny and and epigenetic-developmental (“ evo - devo ”) self-canal- underlying mechanisms, and (iii) speciation – the ization of evolutionary processes [ 2 ] . It remains uncer- process by which new species arise during phylogeny. tain as to which of these possible processes principally Regarding the phylogeny, it is now commonly accepted drive the evolution of nervous systems and brains. that all organisms on Earth are derived from a com- mon ancestor or an ancestral gene pool, while contro- versies have remained since the time of Darwin and 2.1 Reconstruction of the Evolution Wallace about the major mechanisms underlying the of Nervous Systems and Brains observed modi fi cations during phylogeny (cf . [1 ] ). The prevalent view of neodarwinism (or better In most cases, the reconstruction of the evolution of “new” or “modern evolutionary synthesis”) is charac- nervous systems and brains cannot be based on fossil- terized by the assumption that evolutionary changes ized material, since their soft tissues decompose, but are caused by a combination of two major processes, has to make use of the distribution of neural traits in (i) heritable variation of individual genomes within a extant species.
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolution of the Brain, the Human Nature of Cortical Circuits, and Intellectual Creativity
    REVIEW ARTICLE published: 16 May 2011 NEUROANATOMY doi: 10.3389/fnana.2011.00029 The evolution of the brain, the human nature of cortical circuits, and intellectual creativity Javier DeFelipe1,2,3* 1 Instituto Cajal, Consejo Superior de Investigaciones Científicas, Madrid, Spain 2 Laboratorio Cajal de Circuitos Corticales, Centro de Tecnología Biomédica, Universidad Politécnica de Madrid, Madrid, Spain 3 Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red sobre Enfermedades Neurodegenerativas, Madrid, Spain Edited by: The tremendous expansion and the differentiation of the neocortex constitute two major events Idan Segev, The Hebrew University of in the evolution of the mammalian brain. The increase in size and complexity of our brains opened Jerusalem, Israel the way to a spectacular development of cognitive and mental skills. This expansion during Reviewed by: Kathleen S. Rockland, Massachusetts evolution facilitated the addition of microcircuits with a similar basic structure, which increased Institute of Technology, USA the complexity of the human brain and contributed to its uniqueness. However, fundamental Ranulfo Romo, Universidad Nacional differences even exist between distinct mammalian species. Here, we shall discuss the issue Autónoma de México, Mexico of our humanity from a neurobiological and historical perspective. *Correspondence: Javier DeFelipe, Laboratorio Cajal de Keywords: human nature, evolution, cortical circuits, brain size, number synapses, pyramidal neurons Circuitos Corticales, Centro de Tecnología Biomédica, Universidad
    [Show full text]
  • Technology and Human Brain Evolution
    Volume 15, Number 2 Bulletin of the General Anthropology Division Fall , 2008 ated by the recognition that primate eco- Successful Teaching Tools and Brain Size logical and technological adaptations are socially learned, and that multiple inter- Cultivating the Teaching Technology and Human Brain acting causes and constraints will need Moment Evolution to be considered in any full explanation of human brain evolution. In short, tech- By Elizabeth Chin* By Dietrich Stout nology is social, and the early specula- Occidental College University College London tions of Darwin and Engels continue to eaching, for me, is fundamentally Mastery over nature began with look remarkably current. about facilitating an atmosphere the development of the hand, with Tin which students are able to labour…the development of labour The earliest direct evidence of homi- learn, and creating opportunities for all necessarily helped to bring the mem- nid technological activity, and thus social of us to broaden our understanding of bers of society closer together by in- interaction, comes from stone tools and what learning is and how it can be ac- creasing cases of mutual support and cut-marked bones dating back as much complished. Because my classroom is a joint activity, and by making clear the as 2.6 million years at Gona (Ethiopia). space in which learning is collaborative advantage of joint activity to each in- The ensuing ~2.4 million years of the Lower Paleolithic witnessed a technologi- and processual as opposed to authori- dividual. In short, men in the making cal progression from simple ‘Oldowan’ tarian and outcome oriented, students arrived at the point where they had stone chips to skillfully shaped something to say to each other—first often have to rethink many of their as- ‘Acheulean’ bifacial cutting tools, accom- labour, after it and then with it speech sumptions about knowledge and learn- panied by a nearly threefold increase in ing, teaching and education.
    [Show full text]
  • Remarkable Preservation of Brain Tissues in an Early Cretaceous Iguanodontian Dinosaur
    Downloaded from http://sp.lyellcollection.org/ by guest on September 25, 2021 Remarkable preservation of brain tissues in an Early Cretaceous iguanodontian dinosaur MARTIN D. BRASIER1†, DAVID B. NORMAN2*, ALEXANDER G. LIU2,3*, LAURA J. COTTON4, JAMIE E. H. HISCOCKS5, RUSSELL J. GARWOOD6,7, JONATHAN B. ANTCLIFFE8,9,10 & DAVID WACEY3,11 1Department of Earth Sciences, University of Oxford, South Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3AN, UK 2Department of Earth Sciences, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EQ, UK 3School of Earth Sciences, University of Bristol, Life Sciences Building, 24 Tyndall Avenue, Bristol BS8 1TQ, UK 4School of Biological Sciences, The University of Hong Kong, Kadoorie Biological Sciences Building, Pokfulam Road, Hong Kong SAR, China 5Cantelupe Road, Bexhill-on-Sea, East Sussex TN40 1PP, UK 6School of Earth and Environmental Sciences, University of Manchester, Manchester M13 9PL, UK 7Department of Earth Sciences, Natural History Museum, Cromwell Road, London SW7 5BD, UK 8Institute of Earth Sciences, University of Lausanne, 1015 Lausanne, Switzerland 9Department of Zoology, University of Oxford, The Tinbergen Building, South Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3PS, UK 10Oxford University Museum of Natural History, Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3PW, UK 11Centre for Microscopy Characterisation and Analysis, and Australian Research Council Centre of Excellence for Core to Crust Fluid Systems, The University of Western Australia, 35 Stirling Highway, Perth, WA 6009, Australia *Correspondence: [email protected]; [email protected] Abstract: It has become accepted in recent years that the fossil record can preserve labile tissues. We report here the highly detailed mineralization of soft tissues associated with a naturally occur- ring brain endocast of an iguanodontian dinosaur found in c.
    [Show full text]
  • How Humans Evolved Large Brains: Comparative Evidence
    Evolutionary Anthropology 23:65–75 (2014) ARTICLE How Humans Evolved Large Brains: Comparative Evidence KARIN ISLER* AND CAREL P. VAN SCHAIK The human brain is about three times as large as that of our closest living rela- (see Dunbar and Shultz6). Each spe- tives, the great apes. Overall brain size is a good predictor of cognitive perform- cies does not just occupy an ecologi- ance in a variety of tests in primates.1,2 Therefore, hypotheses explaining the cal niche, it also constructs that evolution of this remarkable difference have attracted much interest. In this niche by influencing the external review, we give an overview of the current evidence from comparative studies conditions.7 An increase in brain size testing these hypotheses. If cognitive benefits are diverse and ubiquitous, it is may change the conditions, and possible that most of the variation in relative brain size among extant primates when such evolutionary feedback is explained by variation in the ability to avoid the fitness costs of increased loops occur, cause and effect become brain size (allocation trade-offs and increased minimum energy needs). This is impossible to disentangle. As we look indeed what we find, suggesting that an energetic perspective helps to comple- at the current endpoints of a long ment approaches to explain variation in brain size that postulate cognitive bene- evolutionary history, such a model fits. The expensive brain framework also provides a coherent scenario for how cannot and should not look for these factors may have shaped early hominin brain expansion. causal pathways, but only for pat- terns of correlated evolution.
    [Show full text]
  • A Role for Relaxed Selection in the Evolution of the Language Capacity
    A role for relaxed selection in the evolution of the language capacity Terrence W. Deacon1 Department of Anthropology, University of California, Berkeley, CA 94720 Explaining the extravagant complexity of the human language and Adaptation is the natural counterpart to functional design, but the our competence to acquire it has long posed challenges for natural idea that exquisite biological design might be achieved in the absence selection theory. To answer his critics, Darwin turned to sexual of any information about the context of use seemed absurd. Deeply selection to account for the extreme development of language. Many ingrained intuitions, gained through the difficult experience of de- contemporary evolutionary theorists have invoked incredibly lucky signing and constructing even simple artifacts and machines, made it mutation or some variant of the assimilation of acquired behaviors to unquestionable that only considerable planning and knowledge innate predispositions in an effort to explain it. Recent evodevo about the relevant properties of the materials and tasks involved approaches have identified developmental processes that help to could yield reliable functional outcomes. Moreover, the difficulties explain how complex functional synergies can evolve by Darwinian encountered multiply geometrically with increasing complexity be- means. Interestingly, many of these developmental mechanisms bear cause of the way that changing one component can interfere with the a resemblance to aspects of Darwin’s mechanism of natural selection,
    [Show full text]
  • Evolution of the Size and Functional Areas of the Human Brain
    ANRV287-AN35-20 ARI 9 September 2006 8:42 Evolution of the Size and Functional Areas of the Human Brain P. Thomas Schoenemann Department of Behavioral Sciences, University of Michigan–Dearborn, Dearborn, Michigan 48128; email: [email protected] Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006. 35:379–406 Key Words First published online as a Review in neuroanatomy, encephalization, behavior, adaptation, selection Advance on June 16, 2006 The Annual Review of Anthropology is online Abstract at anthro.annualreviews.org The human brain is one of the most intricate, complicated, and This article’s doi: impressive organs ever to have evolved. Understanding its evolu- by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org 10.1146/annurev.anthro.35.081705.123210 tion requires integrating knowledge from a variety of disciplines in Copyright c 2006 by Annual Reviews. the natural and social sciences. Four areas of research are particu- All rights reserved larly important to this endeavor. First, we need to understand basic 0084-6570/06/1021-0379$20.00 principles of brain evolution that appear to operate across broad classes of organisms. Second, we need to understand the ways in which human brains differ from the brains of our closest living rela- tives. Third, clues from the fossil record may allow us to outline the manner in which these differences evolved. Finally, studies of brain structure/function relationships are critical for us to make behav- ioral sense of the evolutionary changes that occurred. This review highlights important questions and work in each of these areas.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolution of the Brain in Humans – Paleoneurology
    1326 Evolution of the Brain in Humans – Paleoneurology References cranial sensory placodes. J Exp Zool B Mol Dev Evol 304:340–346 1. Forey P, Janvier P (1994) Agnathans and the origin 20. McCormick CA (1992) Evolution of central auditory of jawed vertebrates. Nature 361:129–134 pathways in anamniotes. In: Webster DB, Fay RR, 2. Lauder GV, Liem KF (1983) The evolution and Popper AN (eds) The evolutionary biology of hearing. interrelationships of the actinopterygian fishes. Bull Springer Verlag, New York, p 323 Museum Comp Zool 150:95–197 21. Wullimann MF (1998) The central nervous system. In: 3. Furlong R, Holland PWH (2002) Bayesion phylogenetic Evans DH (ed) Physiology of fishes. CRC, Boca Raton, analysis supports monophyly of Ambulacraria and of FL, p 245–282 cyclostomes. Zool Sci 19:593–599 4. Mallat J, Sullivan J (1998) 28S and 18S rDNA sequences support the monophyly of lampreys and hagfishes. Mol Biol Evol 15:1706–1718 5. Hoegg S, Brinkmann H, Taylor JS, Meyer A (2004) Phylogenetic timing of the fish-specific genome duplica- tion correlates with phenotypic and taxonomic diversifi- Evolution of the Brain in cation in fishes. J Molecular Evol 59:190–203 Humans – Paleoneurology 6. Jerison H (2001) The evolution of neural and behavioral complexity. In: Roth G, Wullimann MF (eds) Brain evolution and cognition. Spektrum Akademischer Verlag/ 1 2 Wiley, Berlin, pp 523–554 RALPH L. HOLLOWAY ,CHET C. SHERWOOD , 3 4 7. van Dongen PAM (1998) Brain size in vertebrates. PATRICK R. HOF ,JAMES K. RILLING In: Nieuwenhuys R, ten Donkelaar HJ, Nicholson C (eds) 1Department of Anthropology, Columbia University, The central nervous system of vertebrates, Springer, – New York, NY, USA Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 2099 2134 2 8.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolutionary Approaches to Creativity
    P1: OJL Trim: 7in × 10in Top: 0.498in Gutter: 0.871in CUUS1027-15 cuus1027/Kaufman ISBN: 978 0 521 51366 1 May 10, 2010 20:17 CHAPTER 15 Evolutionary Approaches to Creativity Liane Gabora and Scott Barry Kaufman 1. Introduction Studies at the intersection of creativity and evolution are not limited to investi- Many species engage in acts that could be gations into the biological evolution of a called creative (J.C. Kaufman & A.B. Kauf- highly creative species. Creative ideas them- man, 2004). However, human creativity is selves might be said to evolve through cul- unique in that it has completely transformed ture. Human creativity is distinctive because the planet we live on. We build skyscrap- of the adaptive and open-ended manner in ers, play breathtaking cello sonatas, send which change accumulates. Inventions build ourselves into space, and even decode our on previous ones in ways that enhance their own DNA. Given that the anatomy of the utility or aesthetic appeal, or make them human brain is not so different from that applicable in different situations. There is no of the great apes, what enables us to be so a priori limit to how a creative idea might creative? Recent collaborations at the fron- unfold over time. A cartoon character may tier of anthropology, archaeology, psychol- inspire the name and logo for a hockey team ogy, and cognitive science are culminating (the Mighty Ducks), which might in turn in speculative but increasingly sophisticated inspire toys, cereal shapes, cigarette lighter efforts to piece together an answer to this designs, or for that matter work its way into question.
    [Show full text]
  • 1.01 a History of Ideas in Evolutionary Neuroscience
    1.01 A History of Ideas in Evolutionary Neuroscience G F Striedter, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA ª 2007 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. 1.01.1 Common Plan versus Diversity 1 1.01.2 Scala Naturae versus Phylogenetic Bush 3 1.01.3 Relative Size versus Absolute Size 4 1.01.4 Natural Selection versus Developmental Constraints 7 1.01.5 One Law, Many Laws, or None 9 1.01.6 Conclusions and Prospects 10 Glossary repeatedly (Northcutt, 2001; Striedter, 2005)andis treated piecemeal in several articles of this book, I allometry The notion that changes in the size of shall not review it fully. Instead, I will discuss a an object (e.g., the body or the brain) selection of the field’s historically most important entail predictable changes in the pro- ideas and how they fit into the larger context of portional sizes of its components. In evolutionary theory. I also emphasize ideas that are, contrast, isometric scaling involves no or were, controversial. Specifically, I present the changes in an object’s proportions. convergence The independent evolution of similar field’s central ideas in contrast pairs, such as structures or functions from non- ‘common plan versus diversity’ and ‘natural selec- homologous ancestral precursors. tion versus constraints’. This approach scrambles developmental The notion that the mechanisms of the chronology of theoretical developments but constraint development bias the production of helps to disentangle the diverse strands of thought phenotypic variants that natural selec- that currently characterize evolutionary neu- tion can act on. roscience. It also helps to clarify which future encephalization Brain size relative to what one would directions are likely to be most fruitful for the field.
    [Show full text]
  • Tilly Edinger and the Science of Paleoneurology
    Brain Research Bulletin, Vol. 48, No. 4, pp. 351–361, 1999 Copyright © 1999 Elsevier Science Inc. Printed in the USA. All rights reserved 0361-9230/99/$–see front matter PII S0361-9230(98)00174-9 HISTORY OF NEUROSCIENCE The gospel of the fossil brain: Tilly Edinger and the science of paleoneurology Emily A. Buchholtz1* and Ernst-August Seyfarth2 1Department of Biological Sciences, Wellesley College, Wellesley, MA, USA; and 2Zoologisches Institut, Biologie-Campus, J.W. Goethe-Universita¨ t, D-60054 Frankfurt am Main, Germany [Received 21 September 1998; Revised 26 November 1998; Accepted 3 December 1998] ABSTRACT: Tilly Edinger (1897–1967) was a vertebrate paleon- collection and description of accidental finds of natural brain casts, tologist interested in the evolution of the central nervous that is, the fossilized sediments filling the endocrania (and spinal system. By combining methods and insights gained from com- canals) of extinct animals. These can reflect characteristic features parative neuroanatomy and paleontology, she almost single- of external brain anatomy in great detail. handedly founded modern paleoneurology in the 1920s while Modern paleoneurology was founded almost single-handedly working at the Senckenberg Museum in Frankfurt am Main. Edinger’s early research was mostly descriptive and conducted by Ottilie (“Tilly”) Edinger in Germany in the 1920s. She was one within the theoretical framework of brain evolution formulated of the first to systematically investigate, compare, and summarize by O. C. Marsh in the late 19th
    [Show full text]