Territoriality and Home Range Concepts As Applied to Mammals William Henry Burt Journal of Mammalogy, Vol. 24, No. 3. (Aug., 1943), Pp

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Territoriality and Home Range Concepts As Applied to Mammals William Henry Burt Journal of Mammalogy, Vol. 24, No. 3. (Aug., 1943), Pp Territoriality and Home Range Concepts as Applied to Mammals William Henry Burt Journal of Mammalogy, Vol. 24, No. 3. (Aug., 1943), pp. 346-352. Stable URL: http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0022-2372%28194308%2924%3A3%3C346%3ATAHRCA%3E2.0.CO%3B2-O Journal of Mammalogy is currently published by American Society of Mammalogists. Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/about/terms.html. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use. Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at http://www.jstor.org/journals/asm.html. Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. The JSTOR Archive is a trusted digital repository providing for long-term preservation and access to leading academic journals and scholarly literature from around the world. The Archive is supported by libraries, scholarly societies, publishers, and foundations. It is an initiative of JSTOR, a not-for-profit organization with a mission to help the scholarly community take advantage of advances in technology. For more information regarding JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. http://www.jstor.org Wed Aug 22 16:42:28 2007 346 JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY TERRITORIALITY AND HOME RASGE COSCEPTS AS APPLIED TO MAhfMALS TERRITORIALITY The behavioristic trait manifested by a display of property ownership-a defense of certain positions or things-reaches its highest development in the human species. Man considers it his inherent right to own property either as an individual or as a member of a society or both. Further, he is ever ready to protect that property against aggressors, even to the extent at times of sacrificing his own life if necessary. That this behavioristic pattern is not peculiar to man, but is a fundamental characteristic of animals in general, has been shown for diverse animal groups. (For an excellent historical account and summary on territoriality, with fairly complete bibliography, the reader is referred to a paper by Mrs. Sice, 1941). It does not necessarily follow that this trait is found in all animals, nor that it is developed to the same degree in those that are known to possess it, but its wide distribution among the verte- brates (see Evans, L. T., 1938, for reptiles), and even in some of the invertebrates, lends support to the theory that it is a basic characteristic of animals and that the potentialities are there whether the particular animal in question displays the characteristic. Heape (1931, p. 74) went so far as to say: "Thus, although the matter is often an intricate one, and the rights of terri- tory somewhat involved, there can, I think, be no question that territorial rights are established rights amongst the majority of species of animals. There can be no doubt that the desire for acquisition of a definite territorial area, the determination to hold it by fighting if necessary, and the recognition of individual as well as tribal territorial rights by others, are dominant characteristics in all animals. In fact, it may be held that the recognition of territorial rights, one of the most significant attributes of civilization, was not evolved by man, but has ever been an inherent factor in the life history of all animals." Undoubtedly significant is the fact that the more we study the detailed be- havior of animals, the larger is the list of kinds known to display some sort of territoriality. There have been many definitions to describe the territory of different animals under varying circumstances. The best and simplest of these, in my mind, is by Xoble (1939); "territory is any defended area." Soble's definition may be modified to fit any special case, yet it is all-inclusive and to the point. Territory should not be confused with "home rangev--an entirely different concept that will be treated more fully later. The territoriality concept is not a new one (see ?;ice, 19-21). It has been only in the last twenty years, however, that it has been developed and brought to the front as an important biological phenomenon in the lower animals. Howard's book "Territory in Bird Life" (1920) stimulated a large group of workers, chiefly in the field of ornithology, and there has hardly been a bird life-history study since that has not touched on this phase of their behavior. BURT--TERRITORIALITY AND HOME RANGE 347 In the field of mammals, much less critical work has been done, but many of the older naturalists certainly were aware of this behavior pattern even though they did not speak of it in modern terms. Hearne (1795) apparently was thinking of property rights (territoriality) when he wrote about the beaver as - HOME RANGE BOUNDARY NEUTRAL AREA ----- TERRITORIAL BOUNDARY NESTING SITE BLANK--UNOCCUPIED SPACE O REFUGE SITE FIG.1. Theoretical quadrat with six occupants of the same species and sex, showing territory and home range concepts as presented in text. follows: "I have seen a large beaver house built in a small island, that had near a dozen houses under one roof; and, two or three of these only excepted, none of them had any communication with each other but by water. As there were beavers enough to inhabit each apartment, it is more than probable that each family knew its own, and always ent'ered at their own door without having any 348 JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY further connection with their neighbors than a friendly intercourse" (in Morgan, 1868, pp. 308-309). Morgan (op. cit., pp. 134-135), also writing of the beaver, made the following observation; "a beaver family consists of a male and female, and their offspring of the first and second years, or, more properly, under two years old. .When the first litter attains the age of two years, and in the third summer after their birth, they are sent out from the parent lodge." Rlor- gan's observation was later confirmed by Bradt (1938). The works of Seton are replete with instances in the lives of different animals that indicate territorial behavior. In the introduction to his "Lives" Seton (1909) states "In the idea of a home region is the germ of territorial rights." Heape (1931) devotes an entire chapter to "territory." Although he uses the term more loosely than I propose to, (he includes home ranges of individuals and feeding ranges of tribes or colonies of animals), he carries through his work the idea of defense of an area either by an individual or a group of individuals. Sot only this, but he draws heavily on the literature in various fields to support his thesis. Al- though the evidence set forth by Seton, Heape, and other early naturalists is of a general nature, mostly garnered from reports by others, it cannot be brushed aside in a casual manner. The old time naturalists were good observers, and, even though their techniques were not as refined as those of present day biolo- gists, there is much truth in what they wrote. A few fairly recent published observations on specific mammals serve to strengthen many of the general statements made by earlier workers. In speak- ing of the red squirrel (Tamiasciurus), Klugh (1927, p. 28) writes; "The sense of ownership seems to be well developed. Both of the squirrels which have made the maple in my garden their headquarters apparently regarded this tree as their private property, and drove away other squirrels which came into it. It is quite likely that in this case it was not the tree, but the stores that were arranged about it, which they were defending." Clarke (1939) made similar observations on the same species. In raising wild mice of the genus Peromyscus in the laboratory, Dice (1929, p. 124) found that "when mice are placed together for mating or to conserve cage space it sometimes happens that fighting takes place, especially at first, and sometimes a mouse is killed. Xearly always the mouse at home in the cage will attack the presumed intruder." Further on he states, "However, when the young are first born, the male, or any other female in the same cage, is driven out of the nest by the mother, who fiercely protects her young." Similarly, Grange (1932, pp. 4-5) noted that snowshoe hares (Lepus americanus) in captivity '(showed a definite partiality for certain spots and corners to which they became accustomed" and that "the female would not allow the male in her territory (cage) during late pregnancy and the males themselves were quarrelsome during the breeding season." Errington (1939) has found what he terms '(intraspecific strife" in wild musk- rats (Ondatra), RiIuch fighting takes place when marshes become overcrowded, especially in fall and winter during readjustment of populations. '(But when invader meets resident in the tunnel system of one of [the] last lodges to be used in a dry marsh, conflict may be indeed savage." Gordon (1936) observed def- inite territories in the western red squirrels (Tamiasciurus jremonti and T. douglasii) during their food gathering activities. He also performed a neat experiment with marked golden mantled squirrels (Citellus lateralis chysodeirus) by placing an abundance of food at the home of a female. This food supply attracted others of the same species.
Recommended publications
  • Microcebus Murinus)
    Sex-specific differences in dispersal propensities and their consequences for grey mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus) D i s s e r t a t i o n zur Erlangung des Doktorgrades der Mathematisch‐Naturwissenschaftlichen Fakultäten der Georg‐August‐Universität zu Göttingen vorgelegt von Susanne Schliehe‐Diecks aus Glandorf Göttingen 2012 Referent: Prof. Dr. Peter M. Kappeler Korreferent: Prof. Dr. Eckhard W. Heymann Tag der mündlichen Prüfung: 16.07.2012 Grey mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus), nocturnal, solitary active primates CONTENTS CONTENTS GENERAL INTRODUCTION........................................................................................................1 CHAPTER 1: DISPERSAL MOVEMENTS Walk the line – Dispersal movements of gray mouse lemurs………..............................11 with Manfred Eberle and Peter M. Kappeler Behavioral Ecology & Sociobiology (2012): in press CHAPTER 2: PROXIMATE MECHANISMS OF NATAL DISPERSAL Ready to go? On the relationship between natal dispersal and body mass in gray mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus)...............................................................37 with Manfred Eberle and Peter M. Kappeler Behavioral Ecology: submitted CHAPTER 3: SEX‐SPECIFIC BEHAVIOURAL DIFFERENCES On the application of mixed hidden Markov models to multiple behavioural time series....................................................................................................................57 with Peter M. Kappeler and Roland Langrock Interface Focus (2012) 2: 180‐189 CHAPTER 4: CONSEQUENCES OF NATAL DISPERSAL The
    [Show full text]
  • When Should a Territory Resident Attack?
    ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2001, 62, 749–759 doi:10.1006/anbe.2001.1799, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on When should a territory resident attack? PAUL V. SWITZER*, JUDY A. STAMPS† & MARC MANGEL‡ *Department of Biological Sciences, Eastern Illinois University †Section of Evolution and Ecology, University of California, Davis ‡Department of Environmental Studies, University of California, Santa Cruz (Received 10 August 2000; initial acceptance 2 October 2000; final acceptance 15 March 2001; MS. number: A8855) Models of territorial defence tend to omit two characteristics of many territorial systems: repeated intrusions by the same individual and the learning processes of residents and intruders. Here we present state-dependent, dynamic models of feeding territories, designed to investigate temporal patterns of resident aggression towards intruders that are capable of spatial learning. We compare two types of models: (1) a nomadic intruder model, in which intruders never visit the same territory twice, and (2) a single, repeat intruder model, in which an intruder may repeatedly intrude into a given territory but is less likely to do so after being attacked. These two models produce qualitatively and quantitatively different patterns of aggression by residents. For instance, residents with intruders that may repeatedly intrude have high initial attack rates, regardless of initial probability of intrusion, but their attack rates decline over time. In contrast, residents in the nomadic intruder model do not attack intruders if intrusion rates are moderately high, and their attack rates are constant and high for most of the period of territory tenure. In addition, residents of both nomadic and repeat intruder scenarios stopped attacking intruders for a short period before voluntarily abandoning their feeding territories.
    [Show full text]
  • White-Tailed Deer Scientific Name: Odocoileus Virginianus
    ODNR Division of Wildlife Life History Notes White-tailed Deer Scientific Name: Odocoileus virginianus Publication 101 (R503) Introduction with heavy, long guard hairs and a thick under- The white-tailed deer, commonly referred to coat that provides excellent insulation. White as the whitetail, is perhaps Ohio’s best-known patches are found around the eyes, on the wildlife species. It is seen in the state’s wildlife throat, belly, tail (underside), and insides of the areas, parks, and nature preserves as well as in legs. When in flight, the large white tail or flag, the backyards of rural and suburban residents. flipped up in the air can be the easiest way to The state’s only big game animal, it has pro- spot the deer. vided table fare for generations of the state’s Whitetails, especially in Ohio, are also well inhabitants from Native Americans to thou- known for their antlers. The whitetail buck grows sands of sportsmen and women today. its first set of antlers when it is a year old. Each However, the white-tailed deer hasn’t always year, a buck’s antlers begin growing in the been as abundant in the state as it is today. early spring. The developing antler is covered As a matter of fact, there was a period of time with a thick velvety skin rich with blood vessels (1904 to 1923) when the deer was absent in the and nerves. Decreasing day length in the late state. As Ohio was settled, habitat was eliminat- summer and early fall triggers many physical ed and hunting was unregulated.
    [Show full text]
  • Marine Ecology Progress Series 570:233
    Vol. 570: 233–246, 2017 MARINE ECOLOGY PROGRESS SERIES Published April 27 https://doi.org/10.3354/meps12073 Mar Ecol Prog Ser OPEN ACCESS Visitor noise at a nesting colony alters the behavior of a coastal seabird Rachel T. Buxton1,*, Reina Galvan1, Megan F. McKenna2, Cecilia L. White1, Victoria Seher3 1Department of Fish, Wildlife and Conservation Biology, Colorado State University, Fort Collins, Colorado 80523-1474, USA 2Natural Sounds and Night Skies Division, National Park Service, Fort Collins, Colorado 80525, USA 3Golden Gate National Recreation Area, Fort Mason, San Francisco, California 94123, USA ABSTRACT: Exposure to park visitors can disrupt animal behavior. Management strategies often aim to eliminate direct human disturbance; however, elevated visitor noise levels may remain. Coastal seabird colonies frequently overlap with scenic locations, resulting in high visitor noise and potentially altered behavior, habitat use, and fitness. We examine the impact of visitor noise on Brandt’s cormorants Phalacrocorax penicillatus at Alcatraz Island, an important nesting site and one of California’s most visited attractions. We used paired acoustic and video recorders to investigate the relationship between visitor noise levels and the behavior and relative abundance of cormorants in colonies adjacent to and far from a heavily visited building. Visitors were not visible from the cormorant colonies. At cormorant colonies adjacent to the visited building, distur- bance-related behaviors increased with visitor noise. Conversely, there was no relationship be - tween behavior and visitor noise in colonies far from the visited building. Cormorant disturbance behavior increased and abundance decreased when gulls were present at colonies adjacent to the visited building, whereas there was no relationship between gulls and behavior or abundance at colonies far from the visited building.
    [Show full text]
  • Managing Your Woodland for White-Tailed Deer St
    MANAGING YOUR WOODLAND FOR Managing Your Woodland for White-tailed Deer St. Paul, MN 55155-4040 Paul, St. Road 500 Lafayette SectionWildlife of Department of Natural Resources White-tailed deer are Minnesota’s most abundant Behavior and Home Range and popular big game animal with a population of White-tailed deer make seasonal changes in the White-tailed approximately one million. Through the early to mid- use and size of their home range in response to 2000’s, deer populations were at historic high levels and changing weather, food availability and cover needs. In have only recently been reduced through liberal hunting Minnesota’s northern forests an adult doe’s seasonal Deer regulations. They are found throughout Minnesota and home range is between 120-900 acres. Yearling does thrive in many diverse landscapes across the state’s more establish home ranges near their mother, while yearling than 79,000 square miles. Deer are very adaptable and bucks are more likely to establish home ranges some can be found in agricultural areas near river bottoms distance from their mother’s home range. In late and in farm woodlots, in open grass and brushland summer, adult does, fawns, and yearlings gather in landscapes, and in northern forests. An important aspect social groups, staying together through the winter and of maintaining desirable deer populations is employing spring. a hunting season harvest strategy that targets antlerless deer, and by providing the proper habitat, or food, cover, Adult bucks occupy 300-1,300 acre home ranges water, and space. By learning about the basics habitat and expand their home ranges during the fall rutting needs of deer, and through active habitat and hunting season.
    [Show full text]
  • Factors Affecting Breeding Territory Size and Placement of the Florida Grasshoper Sparrow (Ammodramus Savannarum Floridanus)
    University of Central Florida STARS Electronic Theses and Dissertations, 2004-2019 2009 Factors Affecting Breeding Territory Size And Placement Of The Florida Grasshoper Sparrow (ammodramus Savannarum Floridanus) Jill Aldredge University of Central Florida Part of the Biology Commons Find similar works at: https://stars.library.ucf.edu/etd University of Central Florida Libraries http://library.ucf.edu This Masters Thesis (Open Access) is brought to you for free and open access by STARS. It has been accepted for inclusion in Electronic Theses and Dissertations, 2004-2019 by an authorized administrator of STARS. For more information, please contact [email protected]. STARS Citation Aldredge, Jill, "Factors Affecting Breeding Territory Size And Placement Of The Florida Grasshoper Sparrow (ammodramus Savannarum Floridanus)" (2009). Electronic Theses and Dissertations, 2004-2019. 4042. https://stars.library.ucf.edu/etd/4042 FACTORS AFFECTING BREEDING TERRITORY SIZE AND PLACEMENT OF THE FLORIDA GRASSHOPPER SPARROW (AMMODRAMUS SAVANNARUM FLORIDANUS) by JILL NICOLE ALDREDGE B.S. University of Kansas, 2000 A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science in the Department of Biology in the College of Sciences at the University of Central Florida Orlando, Florida Summer Term 2009 ABSTRACT For most taxa, maximizing fitness depends on maintaining access to adequate resources. Territories provide exclusive use of resources for an individual or a family group, thus facilitating successful reproduction. The economic defensibility of a territory depends on the quality, abundance, and distribution of its resources as well as the amount of competition that an individual must endure to maintain exclusive access. The benefits of defense must outweigh the costs for territoriality to be profitable.
    [Show full text]
  • Frequently Asked Questions Updated
    FREQUENTLY ASKED QUESTIONS MOUNTAIN LIONS - PHYSICAL CHARACTERISTICS, ADAPTATIONS & HABITS What is the difference between a mountain lion, a cougar and a puma? There are many common names given to these charismatic predators and mountain lion, cougar and puma are all correct for the species. The scientific name for this beautiful cat is Puma concolor (meaning: lion of one color). In South America, where they occur as far south as southern Chile, they are mostly known as puma. What are the characteristics of mountain lions? Mountain lions are North America’s big cat. They are solitary predators, mainly active at twilight and night (crepuscular and nocturnal). They have a tawny brown coat, with short course hair, light undersides, and an extremely long, black-tipped tail, about 2/3 their body length. Mountain lions have a relatively thin neck, small head with rounded ears, and a wide face with a distinctive short white muzzle ringed with darker fur. They have a fairly stocky build, with slightly longer back legs and big paws with sharp, retractable claws. They are the top predators in their ecosystems but are generally timid in their interaction with humans avoiding contact where possible. Mountain lions tend to run away or climb trees to escape danger. They do not roar like other large cats, rather making a variety of sounds more closely resembling domestic cats, but just louder. What are the differences between male and female mountain lions? Mountain lion male and females look the same, however, male’s testicles can be visible at the base of the tail.
    [Show full text]
  • Cougars (Mountain Lions)
    Cougars (Mountain Lions) Sleek and graceful, cougars (Puma concolor, Fig. 1) are solitary and secretive animals rarely seen in the wild. Also known as mountain lions or pumas, cougars are known for their strength, agility, and awesome ability to jump. Their exceptionally powerful legs enable them to leap 30 feet from a standstill, or to jump 15 feet straight up a cliff wall. A cougar’s overall strength and powerful jaws allow it to take down and drag prey larger than itself (Fig. 2). Cougars are the largest members of the cat family in North America. Adult males average approximately 140 pounds but in a perfect situation may weigh 180 pounds and measure 7-8 feet long from nose to tip of tail. Adult males stand about 30 inches tall at the shoulder. Adult female cougars average about 25 percent smaller than males. Cougars vary in color from reddish-brown to Figure 1. In rough terrain, cougar daybeds are usually in tawny (deerlike) to gray, with a black tip on their long a cave or a shallow nook on a cliff face or rock outcrop. tail. An average of two kittens are born every other year. (From Christensen, Mammals of the Pacific Northwest: A Cougars occur throughout Washington where suitable Pictorial Introduction.) cover and prey are found. The cougar population for the year 2002 was estimated to be 2,400 to 3,500 animals. Statewide, the cougar population is likely declining. The Department of Fish and Wildlife has nine management zones around the state designated for “maintain” or “decline,” and adjusts harvest levels accordingly.
    [Show full text]
  • Territory Tango
    PREPARATION: Make craft dough (or paper territories for Modification). CLASS TIME: one class period VOCABULARY: territory, forest interior species, fragmentation TEACHER’S MATERIALS: craft dough–enough for one lump of dough per student (or paper and compasses for Modification); round cookie cutters or similar items, such as jar lids–one for each GUIDE student; rolling pins or other similar items; waxed paper; a table knife NEXT GENERATION SCIENCE STANDARDS: MS-ESS3-4, MS-LS2-4, MS-LS2-5 ACTIVITY Territory Tango OVERVIEW the state not covered by prairie was covered by forests Students make craft dough models of bird territories to of oak and hickory, maple and basswood, as well as learn about habitat requirements of interior forest mixed broadleaved trees and conifers, and other forest species compared to edge species. types. In these great forest tracts lived many kinds of mammals and birds. CONCEPTS n People and some birds depend on forests for their Some wildlife species need large forest stands to find needs. food and shelter to successfully rear their young. Among n them are many species of birds, called interior forest Bird populations are affected by human impact on species, because they live in the interior of large forest- their habitat. ed areas. The ovenbird and scarlet tanager are exam- n Each forest management option may limit other for- ples of interior forest birds. est uses. n As Illinois was being settled, trees were cut for lumber Human use and management of forests affects bird and to clear the land for farming, much as people are populations.
    [Show full text]
  • Lemurs of Madagascar – a Strategy for Their
    Cover photo: Diademed sifaka (Propithecus diadema), Critically Endangered. (Photo: Russell A. Mittermeier) Back cover photo: Indri (Indri indri), Critically Endangered. (Photo: Russell A. Mittermeier) Lemurs of Madagascar A Strategy for Their Conservation 2013–2016 Edited by Christoph Schwitzer, Russell A. Mittermeier, Nicola Davies, Steig Johnson, Jonah Ratsimbazafy, Josia Razafindramanana, Edward E. Louis Jr., and Serge Rajaobelina Illustrations and layout by Stephen D. Nash IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group Bristol Conservation and Science Foundation Conservation International This publication was supported by the Conservation International/Margot Marsh Biodiversity Foundation Primate Action Fund, the Bristol, Clifton and West of England Zoological Society, Houston Zoo, the Institute for the Conservation of Tropical Environments, and Primate Conservation, Inc. Published by: IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group, Bristol Conservation and Science Foundation, and Conservation International Copyright: © 2013 IUCN Reproduction of this publication for educational or other non-commercial purposes is authorized without prior written permission from the copyright holder provided the source is fully acknowledged. Reproduction of this publication for resale or other commercial purposes is prohibited without prior written permission of the copyright holder. Inquiries to the publisher should be directed to the following address: Russell A. Mittermeier, Chair, IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group, Conservation International, 2011 Crystal Drive, Suite 500, Arlington, VA 22202, USA Citation: Schwitzer C, Mittermeier RA, Davies N, Johnson S, Ratsimbazafy J, Razafindramanana J, Louis Jr. EE, Rajaobelina S (eds). 2013. Lemurs of Madagascar: A Strategy for Their Conservation 2013–2016. Bristol, UK: IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group, Bristol Conservation and Science Foundation, and Conservation International. 185 pp. ISBN: 978-1-934151-62-4 Illustrations: © Stephen D.
    [Show full text]
  • Territorial Defense Strategies in the Northern Cardinal (Cardinalis Cardinalis): Who Is the Bigger Threat?
    The University of Southern Mississippi The Aquila Digital Community Master's Theses Fall 12-1-2015 Territorial Defense Strategies in the Northern Cardinal (Cardinalis cardinalis): Who is the Bigger Threat? Kaylee M. Gentry University of Southern Mississippi Follow this and additional works at: https://aquila.usm.edu/masters_theses Part of the Animal Studies Commons, Behavior and Ethology Commons, and the Ornithology Commons Recommended Citation Gentry, Kaylee M., "Territorial Defense Strategies in the Northern Cardinal (Cardinalis cardinalis): Who is the Bigger Threat?" (2015). Master's Theses. 161. https://aquila.usm.edu/masters_theses/161 This Masters Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by The Aquila Digital Community. It has been accepted for inclusion in Master's Theses by an authorized administrator of The Aquila Digital Community. For more information, please contact [email protected]. TERRITORIAL DEFENSE STRATEGIES IN THE NORTHERN CARDINAL (CARDINALIS CARDINALIS): WHO IS THE BIGGER THREAT? by Kaylee Michelle Gentry A Thesis Submitted to the Graduate School and the Department of Biological Sciences at The University of Southern Mississippi in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Science Approved: ________________________________________________ Dr. Frank Moore, Committee Chair Distinguished Professor, Biological Sciences ________________________________________________ Dr. Jodie Jawor, Thesis Director Associate Professor, Biological Sciences ________________________________________________ Dr. David Echevarria, Committee Member Associate Professor, Psychology ________________________________________________ Dr. Karen S. Coats Dean of the Graduate School December 2015 ABSTRACT TERRITORIAL DEFENSE STRATEGIES IN THE NORTHERN CARDINAL (CARDINALIS CARDINALIS): WHO IS THE BIGGER THREAT? by Kaylee Michelle Gentry December 2015 This thesis examines the use of defensive strategies in relation to territories year round in the northern cardinal (Cardinalis cardinalis).
    [Show full text]
  • Some Aspects of the Behavior and Ecology of the Sea Otter, Enhydra Lutris, in Montague Strait, Prince William Sound, Alaska
    SOME ASPECTS OF THE BEHAVIOR AMD ECOLOGY OF THE SEA OTTER, ENHYDRA LUTRIS, IN MONTAGUE STRAIT, PRINCE WILLIAM SOUND, ALASKA By Donald G~ Calkins SOME ASPECTS OF THE BEHAVIOR AND ECOLOGY OF THE SEA OTTER, ENHYDRA LUTRIS, IN MONTAGUE STRAIT, PRINCE WILLIAM SOUND, ALASKA APPROVED: Chairman Department Head APPROVED: ~ct Ud.. Date ~/f/72- Dean of the College of Biological ~ Sciences and Renewable Resources Vice President for Research and Advanced Study SOME ASPECTS OF THE BEHAVIOR AND ECOLOGY OF THE SEA OTTER, ENHYDRA LUTRIS, IN MONTAGUE STRAIT, PRINCE WILLIAM SOUND, ALASKA A THESIS Presented to the Faculty of the University of Alaska in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE By Donald G. Calkins, B.A. College, Alaska ~y~n ABSTRACT Distribution. feeding habits, and territoriality and general behavior of the sea otter (Enhydra lutris) were studied in Montague Strait, Prince William Sound, Alaska during May through September, 1971. Otters were found associated with certain features of the habitat such as: kelp beds (Nereocystis luetkeana), a lagoon, a favored feeding area, and a sheltered area. No otters were seen hauled out on land nor were any areas used exclusively by males or females seen. Montague Strait otters feed primarily on three groups of prey animals: pelecypods, decapods, and asteroids. The most important species in each of these groups are Saxidomus giganteus, Telmessus cheiragonus and Evasterias troschelii, respectively. The otters expended 81% of their feeding effort on pelecypods, 7~ on decapods, and 0.8% on asteroids. Clams were dug from the bottom by the otters, and opened with the aid of stones.
    [Show full text]