Magical Thinking and How This Form of Thought Develops Over the Course of Childhood and Is Maintained in Adulthood

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Magical Thinking and How This Form of Thought Develops Over the Course of Childhood and Is Maintained in Adulthood OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Thu Jan 10 2013, NEWGEN CHAPTER 4 Magical Th inking Karl S. Rosengren and Jason A. French Abstract This chapter examines the concept of magical thinking and how this form of thought develops over the course of childhood and is maintained in adulthood. Rather than assuming magical thinking to be a remnant of childhood and an immature mind, it is suggested that a number of universal cognitive processes lead to the emergence of magical thinking in early childhood and its maintenance into adulthood. This chapter suggests that magical thinking reaches a peak in childhood partly because of the emergence of pretense and imagination as well as substantial cultural support for magical beliefs in children. Although this form of thinking declines somewhat in later childhood and adulthood, unconscious cognitive processes serve to maintain magical thinking throughout the life span. The chapter also explores the relation between magical thinking and religious thoughts and beliefs, superstition, and other forms of strange beliefs in adulthood. Key Words: cognitive biases, cognitive development, essentialism, magical belief, magical thinking, similarity and contagion Every day we are confronted with a variety of a variety of factors, including innate biases, socio- events for which we clearly know the cause. We’re cultural support for particular types of explana- hungry because we were in a hurry and missed break- tions, and individual diff erences in the willingness fast. Sue realizes that John is mad at her because she to accept one or more possible explanations (see forgot to meet him after work. But for other events chapters 3 and 22). Our goal in the current chapter the causes are less evident. Why does the house creak is to provide a review of the literature on the devel- at night? Is it because of changes in the weather con- opment of one form of reasoning, magical think- ditions, strong winds, or some other cause? Could ing, a form of reasoning that takes a nonscientifi c it be a ghost? Why did the streetlight go off just as perspective with respect to mysterious and anoma- we walked by? Was it because of a burned out bulb, lous events. In doing so, we relate magical thinking faulty wiring, or is it caused in some way by our to other forms of cognition and explore the situa- physical presence? How do we explain the myriad tions and contexts that appear to activate magical of events that we witness on a daily basis, especially thinking. Why is it for example that individuals those for which no discernable cause is observed? feel uncomfortable handling the clothes of a dead Th is search for causality in everyday life, for both person, especially, if the individual performed some mundane and mysterious events, seems to be a uni- heinous crime? Why is it that we tend to avoid loca- versal cognitive process. How particular individuals tions where some bad event has occurred, such as a reason about diff erent events and make reference to murder? And why is it that we take causal credit for diff erent types of explanation seems to depend on the occurrence of seemingly random events? Many 42 004_MarjorieTaylor_Ch04.indd4_MarjorieTaylor_Ch04.indd 4422 11/10/2013/10/2013 22:56:21:56:21 PPMM OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Thu Jan 10 2013, NEWGEN researchers attribute the feelings, thoughts, and magical thinking, in terms of illogical and irratio- behaviors associated with these types of events to nal thought, is considered a general characteristic magical thinking. of young children (Rosengren, Miller, Gutierrez, But what really is magical thinking? Although & Schein, in press). Others, such as Joan Didion, magical thinking has been the focus of both anthro- have used the term to describe seemingly irrational pological (L é vy-Bruhl, 1948; 1966; Malinowski, thoughts and behaviors that in her case dominated 1954; Tambiah, 1990) and psychological (Piaget, her days following the sudden and unexpected death 1929, 1930; Subbotsky, 1985, 1992, 2004) inqui- of her husband (Didion, 2005). But these uses of ries for many years, there is no clearly agreed upon magic generally go beyond merely thinking or act- defi nition of what is meant by the term. For this ing irrationally. Th at is, they seem at some level to reason we begin our review with an exploration of connect to a set of beliefs that treat certain individu- both traditional and more current perspectives on als, events, or occurrences as “special” and “out of magical thinking and then off er our own perspec- the ordinary.” We will return to this issue shortly. tive on how this particular term should be defi ned. Similar uses of the term magical thinking are Afterward we consider whether magical thinking is found in the fi elds of anthropology and psychol- still a commonplace activity in the everyday lives ogy, although there is quite a lot of variation in how of both children and adults. We then explore the this term has been used. As Mayr (1982, p. 44) has research on the development of magical thinking pointed out, many of the controversies in the his- in children. Although never quite a main focus of tory of science can be attributed to scientists in dif- developmental psychologists, this topic was exten- ferent, opposing camps using the same term for very sively explored by Piaget (1929, 1930), and research diff erent concepts. Much of the early research exam- in this area has recently seen resurgence. In this sec- ining magical thinking characterized the thought tion we also examine the relation between magical processes of preindustrialized “primitive” cultures thinking and the emergence of pretense and the (Frazer, 1911) and children (Piaget, 1929) as magi- imagination in childhood. We suggest that these cal, dominated by illogical, irrational thoughts three behaviors—magical thinking, imagination, and beliefs (Rosengren & Hickling, 2000). By this and pretense—appear to emerge in concert in late defi nition any thought process that is not logical, infancy and early childhood and provide a mutually systematic, or scientifi c might be characterized as supportive environment enabling each to fl ourish magical. Th is is not that dissimilar to the defi nition in their own right. We also examine how magical provided by Eckblad and Chapman (1983, p. 215), thinking might be related to other forms of think- who suggested that magical thinking involves ing. Specifi cally, we explore the relation among “belief in forms of causation that by conventional magical thinking, pretense, and imagination and standards are invalid.” creativity; between magical and religious thinking; In our view there are a number of problems with between magical thinking and superstition; and last this defi nition of magical thinking. First, this per- the relation between magical thinking and psycho- spective uses magical thinking as a pejorative label pathology. We end the chapter with a summary and for thinking that diff ers either from that of educated some thoughts on future directions for research in adults in technologically advanced societies or the this area. majority of society in general. Th is pejorative view was quite common in the study of magic from an Magical Th inking Defi ned anthropological perspective before the 1970s. With On the surface, the term magical thinking seems an increase in more relativistic cultural accounts, quite easy to defi ne. It is a phrase that is used in such as situated cultural cognition, and distancing both common everyday language and more esoteric from notions of primitivism, the study of magic in academic discussions. In common usage the term diff erent cultures declined substantially (Sorensen, magic is tied to a wide range of events, from a beau- 2007). Second, this defi nition ignores the fact that tiful sunset or rainbow to seemingly unexplainable even in technological advanced societies many deci- phenomena or events. Th e label is also used for par- sions are not based on rational and logical thought ticular individuals who think or behave in certain (Gilovich, Griffi n, & Kahneman, 2002; Tversky & ways. Magic has also been used to label thinking Kahneman, 1974, 1979). For example, when gam- that is characterized to be illogical or irrational. bling, people often place too much importance on Indeed, for many preschool teachers, especially one aspect of an event, which produces an error those schooled in a Piagetian view of development, in predicting future outcomes of that event. Th ese Rosengren, French 43 004_MarjorieTaylor_Ch04.indd4_MarjorieTaylor_Ch04.indd 4433 11/10/2013/10/2013 22:56:23:56:23 PPMM OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Thu Jan 10 2013, NEWGEN decisions appear to be made on the basis of cogni- conventional wisdom of the majority of scientists. tive biases and heuristic. Although these kinds of Although the cutting edge of science may be labeled decisions are not based on formal logic or reason, as “magical,” as in Arthur C. Clarke’s third law (“Any they are not generally labeled as magical. suffi ciently advanced technology is indistinguishable A third critique of the traditional view of magical from magic,” Clarke, 1962), we suggest that very thinking is that it ignores the fact that thinking that few individuals engaging in the rigorous process of appears irrational or illogical to an educated adult scientifi c inquiry would be labeled as engaging in may be the result of lack of knowledge or experi- magical thinking. Th us, there is something more ence in a particular domain or diff erent types of to magical thinking than being ignorant, thinking knowledge or experience. For example, under most illogically or irrationally, or holding beliefs that go circumstances most adults will agree that 1 + 1 = 2. against the established science of the day.
Recommended publications
  • Looking at Femininity Sympodcast Programme
    Looking at Femininity Sympodcast Cardiff University Twitter: @AtFemininity Instagram: @lookingatfemininity Lookingatfemininity.wordpress.com 2 Episode Panel Air Date Title: Early Twentieth Century Challenges to Femininity Monday 1 February 2021 @ Speakers: 10am 1 Rebekka Jolley Ellye Van Grieken Jessica McIvor Title: Mobile Femininity Monday 15 February 2021 @ Speakers: 10am Caleb Sivyer 2 Devika Karnard Christina Thatcher Joan Passey Title: Centre-Staged Bodies Monday 1 March 2021 @ Speakers: 10am 3 Eve Froude Sarah Merton Maria Manning Title: The Surreal-Feminine: Being and Looking Monday 15 March 2021 @ Speakers: 10am 4 Alessia Zinnari Christy Heflin Nadia Albaladejo Garcia Title: Musings of The Debutante Coven Monday 29 March 2021 @ Speakers: 10am 5 Molly Gilroy Rachel Ashenden Tasmin Petrie Title: Girlhood/YA Monday 12 April 2021 @ 10am Speakers: 6 Rosie Couch Lewis Kellet Heather J. Matthews Title: Masculine Femininities Monday 26 April 2021 @ 10am Speakers: 7 Olivia Howe Martha O’Brien Title: Artistic Disruption Monday 10 May 2021 @ 10 Speakers: am 8 Dorka Tamás Rachel Carney Marilia Kaisar 3 Episode 1: Early Twentieth Century Challenges to Femininity Speakers: Rebekka Jolley, Ellye Van Grieken, Jessica McIvor ‘Ladies there is no neutral position for us to assume’: Femininity and Performative Acts in Gertrude Stein’s Ladies Voices, Counting her Dresses, and White Wines. Rebekka Jolley (Liverpool Hope University) Gertrude Stein’s opera The Mother of Us All (1946) is based on the life of Susan B. Anthony a social reformer and part of the women’s suffrage movement. The character of Susan B. Anthony states ‘‘Ladies there is no neutral position for us to assume’ (p.70).
    [Show full text]
  • Silhouette194800agne R 9/ C
    m/ <": : .( ^ } ''^e ^-Pt^i ^ . i.,-4 ^i Digitized by the Internet Archive in 2010 with funding from Lyrasis IVIembers and Sloan Foundation http://www.archive.org/details/silhouette194800agne r 9/ c The 1948 Silhouette is published by the students of Agnes Scott College, Decattir, Oeorgia. under the direction of Margaret "S'ancey. editor, and Jean da Siha, business manager. PRESSER HALL ^L 1948 SILHOUETTE aiieae .==rJ^eJiica Uan To MISS M. KATHRYN CLICK. tvlw encourages its to claim for our own the inner resources of beauty and trutli in our heritage of liberal 'educatioii, we dedicate THE 1948 SILHOUETTE. 65916 THE nGHES SCOTT IDERLS LIUE RS UlE SEEK... high intellectual attainment , prtv 3r\7^ CTJ hHk W^^m^^ \m nil mm^^^m . sinnple religious faith physical well being . service that reflects a sane attitude toward other people. A moment of relaxation be- tween classes brings many to the bookstore. Buttrick Hall, center of most academic activity. Sometimes you find a cut. The favorite place for organ- ization meetings and social functions is Murphey Candler building. Dr. von Schuschnigg drew a throng of listeners at the reception after his lecture. Murphey Candler is the scene of popcorn feasts as well as receptions. In Presser we find the stimulation of music and play practice as well as the serenity of beloved chapel programs. The newest Agnes Scott daughters fast be- come part of us in such traditional events as the C.A. picnic on the little quad. \ w^ r ;^i Prelude -to a festive evening —signing away s. B 1 the vital statistics at the hostess's desk in Main.
    [Show full text]
  • Children's Ethnobiological Notions Of
    Journal of Ethnobiology 2018 38(2): 261–275 Children’s Ethnobiological Notions of Contamination and Contagions among Maasai Agro-Pastoralists of Northern Tanzania Jennifer W. Roulette1*, Casey J. Roulette 2, Robert J. Quinlan1,3, Douglas R. Call3,4, Barry S. Hewlett1, Mark A. Caudell1,3, and Marsha B. Quinlan1,3 Abstract. Humans and other living organisms harbor disease-causing pathogenic microorganisms. These microorganisms are often transmitted through physical contact with contaminated objects, such as food, water, or other people. While some theoretical and empirical research examines the ontogeny of contamination and contagion beliefs, cross-cultural research on this topic is limited. To help remedy this paucity of data, we conducted an ethnobiological study of contagion and contamination beliefs among Maasai children (n = 42) in the Simanjiro district, Tanzania. Participants include 36 middle-aged schoolchildren and six four-year-olds. We contrast the children’s beliefs with those of 12 local adults. To measure children’s views of contagion and contamination, we developed sentence-framed yes/no elicitation tasks using three different stimuli—a fly, a cough, and a cough from someone with respiratory symptoms. Qualitative semi-structured interviews were conducted to further understand children’s ethnotheories of contamination and contagion. Children generally reported that coughs and flies are directly contaminating, whereas they offered mixed results for associational and indirect contamination. Children discussed time, psychological contagion, saliva, wind, and the supernatural/natural as key elements to their beliefs, reasons, and personal actions taken to minimize contamination/contagion risk. We found education to be significantly positively correlated with children reporting that flies and coughs were directly contaminating, while age had no effect.
    [Show full text]
  • Nifty Wars Agri in Titr Ffiattry
    Nifty Wars Agri in titr ffiattry Fifty years ago in the Fancy is researched by Dorothy Mason, from her col- lection of early out of print literature. SUPERSTITION AND WITCHCRAFT A very remarkable peculiarity of the domestic cat, and possibly one that has had much to do with the ill favour with which it has been regarded, especially in the Middle Ages, is the extraordinary property which its fur possesses of yielding electric sparks when hand-rubbed or by other friction, the black in a larger degree than any other colour, even the rapid motion of a fast retreating cat through rough, tangled underwood having been known to produce a luminous effect. In frosty weather it is the more noticeable, the coldness of the weather apparently giving intensity and brilliancy, which to the ignorant would certainly be attributed to the interfer- ence of the spiritual or superhuman. To sensitive natures and nervous temperaments the very contact with the fur of a black cat will often produce a startling thrill or absolutely electric shock. That carefully observant naturalist, Gilbert White, speaking of the frost of 1785, notes ; "During those two Siberian days my parlour cat was so elec- tric, that had a person stroked her and not been properly insulated, the shock might have been given to a whole circle of people." Possibly from this lively, fiery, sparkling tendency, combined with its noiseless motion and stealthy habits, our ancesters were led in the happily bygone superstitious days to regard the unconscious animal as a "familiar" of Satan or some other evil spirit, which generally appeared in the form of a black cat; hence witches were said to have a black cat as their "familiar," or could at will change themselves into the form of a black cat with eyes of fire.
    [Show full text]
  • Paranormal, Superstitious, Magical, and Religious Beliefs
    Paranormal, superstitious, magical, and religious beliefs Kia Aarnio Department of Psychology University of Helsinki, Finland Academic dissertation to be publicly discussed, by due permission of the Faculty of Behavioural Sciences at the University of Helsinki in Auditorium XII, Fabianinkatu 33, on the 19th of October, 2007, at 12 o’clock UNIVERSITY OF HELSINKI Department of Psychology Studies 44: 2007 2 Supervisor Marjaana Lindeman, PhD Department of Psychology University of Helsinki Finland Reviewers Professor Stuart Vyse Department of Psychology Connecticut College USA Timo Kaitaro, PhD Department of Law University of Joensuu Finland Opponent Professor Pekka Niemi Department of Psychology University of Turku Finland ISSN 0781-8254 ISBN 978-952-10-4201-0 (pbk.) ISBN 978-952-10-4202-7 (PDF) http://www.ethesis.helsinki.fi Helsinki University Printing House Helsinki 2007 3 CONTENTS ABSTRACT.......................................................................................................................... 6 TIIVISTELMÄ ....................................................................................................................... 7 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS....................................................................................................... 8 LIST OF ORIGINAL PUBLICATIONS ................................................................................ 10 1 INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................................. 11 1.1. Defining paranormal beliefs 12 1.1.1.
    [Show full text]
  • Issue 3 (Digital Edition)
    Translation: Chen Ruoxi’s The Grey- Eyed Black Cat Chen Ruoxi Author1 Hui-Min Lin Independent Scholar2 This is an old wives’ tale in my village: ‘a grey-eyed black cat is bad luck incarnated; death follows at its tail’. 1 Dear Qing, I received your letter and the book you gave your friend to bring over. I like the book very much. Thank you. 1 This a translation of the Chinese text ‘陳若曦自選集’, written by Chen Ruoxi. The original text is from Chen Ruoxi’s Self-Collection. Taipei, TW: Linking Publishing, 1976. 2 To cite this translation: Chen Ruoxi and Hui-Min Ling, translator. ‘The Grey-Eyed Black Cat’, Journal of Languages, Texts, and Society, vol. 3 (Spring 2019): 78-90. Journal of Languages, Texts, and Society, Vol. 3 (Spring 2019), 207–222. © 2019 by Chen Ruoxi and translated by Hui-Min Lin. 208 C. Ruoxi and H. Lin In the letter, you asked about Wen. But Qing, how can I tell you? Poor Wen! A fortuneteller foretold her short life by reading her face. Who would have thought she would really die at such a young age? I am afraid to think of her. I see her shadow everywhere all the time. Whenever I think of her, I can’t help cursing the so-called fate and wonder how on earth no one seems to ever escape from its grasp? And if there is such a thing called fate, who’s the master of it? I can’t wait to be the first to mock him! I remember it vividly.
    [Show full text]
  • Magical Thinking: Outcome Bias Affects Children’S Evaluation of Testimony
    Magical Thinking: Outcome Bias Affects Children’s Evaluation of Testimony Iris Oved Gail Heyman David Barner ([email protected]) ([email protected]) ([email protected]) Department of Psychology, 9500 Gilman Drive San Diego, CA 92093 USA Abstract informants based on factors like their age (Jaswal & Neely, 2006; VanderBorght, & Jaswal, 2009), and their previous In a series of three experiments we examined how preschool children assess testimony in relation to the relative desirability of history of accuracy (Birch, Vauthier, & Bloom, 2008; the outcome for themselves and for the individual providing the Corriveau & Harris, 2009; Jaswal & Neely, 2006), and that testimony. The first two experiments reveal evidence for an they sometimes weigh the testimony over their own direct outcome bias: children are more likely to believe an extraordinary perceptual experience (Jaswal, 2010). claim when they have little to lose in doing so (Exp.1), and when There are also other factors that influence how humans they stand to gain if the claim is true (Exp. 2). The final evaluate testimony when forming beliefs. In some cases, experiment (Exp. 3) showed that children are less likely to believe beliefs that are fundamental to how we understand the world extraordinary claims when the person making the claim has are formed in direct contravention with expert testimony ulterior motives (e.g., stands to potentially gain from the child’s and despite considerable physical evidence to the contrary belief). These data show that children’s beliefs acquired from testimony are subject to outcome bias, and that children are (e.g., climate change, evolution). In these cases many claims capable of exercising skepticism when the source of testimony is are assessed on the basis of prior convictions, in conformity likely to have ulterior motives.
    [Show full text]
  • Teacher's Notes Superstitions
    Teacher’s Notes Superstitions Type of activity: vocabulary, gap-fi lling, speaking 4. Ask the students to fold their worksheets so that Focus: vocabulary connected to superstitions they can only see Task 1. In pairs, the students Level: pre-intermediate look at the items bringing good and bad luck and Time: 45 minutes take turns to make sentences about each of the superstitions, trying to remember what was said in Task 2. Preparation: – one copy of the Student’s Worksheet per 5. Ask the students to unfold their worksheets and student look at T ask 3. In pairs or small groups, the students discuss the questions. Monitor as they do this, then collect feedback, developing the Procedure: discussion to fi nd out the students’ attitudes to superstitions. 1. Write ‘good luck / bad luck’ on the board and ask the students to give you examples of things Extension / Homework assignment: Ask the which could bring either of these, introducing the students to search the Internet to fi nd out the topic of superstitions. possible origins of some of the superstitions. 2. Distribute the Student’s Worksheets and ask the students to work on Task 1 in pairs. They should complete the table with the words and expressions, deciding whether the items listed have something to do with good or bad luck (explain that crossing your fi ngers is an equivalent of holding your thumbs). Check with the whole group. Key: good luck: knocking on wood, a four-leaf clover, salt, a rabbit’s foot, crossing your fi ngers, a horseshoe / bad luck: a black cat, a ladder, an owl, a broken mirror, salt 3.
    [Show full text]
  • Magical Thinking and Religious Thinking
    E-Theologos, Vol. 3, No. 2 DOI 10.2478/v10154-012-0017-6 Magical Thinking and Religious Thinking Marcin Cholewa - Marek Gilski The Pontifical University of John Paul II in Kraków Although one would think that the development of science and technology promotes the elimination of magical practices and magical thinking, yet observation of contemporary culture seems to contradict this. Symptoms of magical approach to reality are becoming more noticeable: well-stocked esoteric bookshops, TV channels devoted to the subject in question, fortune-tellers' offices, authors writing about the return of magic 1. The development of science and technology and at the same time the return to the practice of magic and magical thinking is only a seeming paradox. Goethe once aptly wrote that aiming toward magic is the result of a specific contact of the human mind with science and technology. It is about an encounter that leads to disappointment, helplessness emerging from improperly set cognitive goals. In this way the belief is created that human reason is helpless, that it has been defeated, despite being able to develop and popularise science and technology. This attitude was adopted by Doctor Faust 2: Ah! Now I’ve done Philosophy, I’ve finished Law and Medicine, And sadly even Theology: Taken fierce pains, from end to end. Now here I am, a fool for sure! No wiser than I was before: [...] And see that we can know - nothing! It almost sets my heart burning. 1 M. INTROVIGNE, Powrót magii , Kraków 2005. 2 See J. Goethe, Faust , translation Poetryintranslation . January 7 2012.
    [Show full text]
  • Sympathetic Magic in Disgust and Other Domains
    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 1586 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1986, Vol. SO, No. 4, 703-712 0022-3514/86/$00,75 Operation of the Laws of Sympathetic Magic in Disgust and Other Domains Paul Rozin, Linda Millman, and Carol Nemeroff University of Pennsylvania Two laws of sympathetic magic were described by Frazer and Mauss at the beginning of this century to account for magical belief systems in traditional cultures. In this study, we show that these laws fit well with a variety of behaviors in American culture, in responses to disgusting, dangerous, or valued objects. The first law, contagion, holds that "once in contact, always in contact." That is, there can be a permanent transfer of properties from one object (usually animate) to another by brief contact. For example, in this study we show that drinks that have briefly contacted a sterilized, dead cockroach become undesirable, or that laundered shirts previously worn by a disliked person are less desirable than those previously worn by a liked or neutral person. The second law, similarity, holds that "the image equals the object," and that action taken on an object affects similar objects. In this study, we demonstrate this law by showing, for example, that people reject acceptable foods (e.g., fudge) shaped into a form that represents a disgusting object (dog feces), or that people are less accurate at throwing darts at pictures of the faces of people they like. With these and other measures, we found a great deal of evidence for the operation of the laws of sympathetic magic in all 50 of the subjects we studied.
    [Show full text]
  • The Black Cat:” a Reflection of Pre-Civil War Slavery
    Walker 1 Hannah Walker “The Black Cat:” A Reflection of Pre-Civil War Slavery In 1843, Edgar Allan Poe published “The Black Cat” against a tumultuous political backdrop regarding the “peculiar institution,” slavery. Within Poe’s lifetime alone, the Missouri Compromise banned slavery north of Missouri, the Nat Turner Rebellion displayed the increasing power of slave uprisings, and William Lloyd Garrison’s Liberator spearheaded the movement to abolish slavery (Biagiarelli). Though Poe, a Virginia native, never formally stated his political stance on slavery, literary critics look to his work as a reflection of antebellum sentiments coming to a boil in the years preceding the Civil War. While most critics rationalize the events in “The Black Cat” with either supernatural or psychological explanations, other critics point to the political context of Poe’s time to illuminate the strange events of the story. Some of these critics, such as Leland Person and Lesley Ginsberg, interpret “The Black Cat” solely as a literary reenactment of the Nat Turner Rebellion while others, such as Joan Dayan, read the story as a reflection of Poe’s personal political views. However, by labeling each character as a historical player within the institution of slavery as a whole, a more ominous statement about racial currents of Poe’s era appears. Specifically, “The Black Cat” functions as a racial allegory that depicts the injustices of slavery and, ultimately, shows how slavery damns the South. One of the most telling details of “The Black Cat” which reveal it as a racial allegory is the distinct symbolism casting the narrator as a slave owner and the black cat as a slave.
    [Show full text]
  • Sounding Sensory Profiles in the Ancient Near East
    Zurich Open Repository and Archive University of Zurich Main Library Strickhofstrasse 39 CH-8057 Zurich www.zora.uzh.ch Year: 2019 Sounding sensory profiles in the Ancient Near East Edited by: Schellenberg, Annette ; Krüger, Thomas Posted at the Zurich Open Repository and Archive, University of Zurich ZORA URL: https://doi.org/10.5167/uzh-173182 Edited Scientific Work Published Version Originally published at: Sounding sensory profiles in the Ancient Near East. Edited by: Schellenberg, Annette; Krüger, Thomas (2019). Atlanta: Society of Biblical Literature. SOUNDING SENSORY PROFILES IN THE ANCIENT NEAR EAST Edited by Annette Schellenberg and omas Krüger Ancient Near East Monographs Monografías sobre el Antiguo Cercano Oriente Society of Biblical Literature Centro de Estudios de Historia del Antiguo Oriente (UCA) SOUNDING SENSORY PROFILES IN THE ANCIENT NEAR EAST ANCIENT NEAR EAST MONOGRAPHS General Editors Jeffrey Stackert Juan Manuel Tebes Editorial Board Reinhard Achenbach Jeffrey L. Cooley C. L. Crouch Roxana Flammini Christopher B. Hays Emanuel Pfoh Andrea Seri Bruce Wells Number 25 SOUNDING SENSORY PROFILES IN THE ANCIENT NEAR EAST Edited by Annette Schellenberg and Thomas Krüger Atlanta Copyright © 2019 by SBL Press All rights reserved. No part of this work may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying and recording, or by means of any information storage or retrieval system, except as may be expressly permit- ted by the 1976 Copyright Act or in writing from the publisher. Requests for permission should be addressed in writing to the Rights and Permissions Office, SBL Press, 825 Hous- ton Mill Road, Atlanta, GA 30329 USA.
    [Show full text]