May 1995 Vol. 40, No. 2

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

May 1995 Vol. 40, No. 2 NEWSLETTER Vol. 40, No.2 Animal Behavior May, 1995 Society A quarterly publication Susan A. Foster, Secretary Harleigh E. Willmott, Editorial Assistant Dept. of Biological Sciences, University of Arkansas, Fayetteville, AR, 72701, USA TRAVEL FUNDS FOR THE INTERNATIONAL ETHOLOGICAl. ASS RESEARCH AWARDS CONFERENC~ HONOLULU This year we received 40 outstanding applications for TIle United States Ethological Conference Committee ABS Research Awards. Only eight could be funded. has applied for a travel grant from lhe National We congratulate lhose who were funded and thank all Science Foundation to provide partial travel support applicants for their efforts in applying. for more junior ethologists to attend lhe International Ethological Conference meeting in Honolulu in Allison Abell, University of Chicago. "Sexual August, 1995. If the funds arc received, they win be selecLioll in the striped plateau lizard (Sceloporus used to support elhologists who have received their virga/us): molecular evaluation of behavioral Ph.D. between 1990 and 1995 or who expect to cstimates mating success". or complete PhD.s in 1995 and who arc US eilizens or residents. To apply for a travel award, submn the Michael Armstrong. Binghamton University. following to Dr. Charles T. Snowdon, Dept. of "Population differences in rcd-willged blackbirds' Psychology, University of Wisconsin, 1202 W.. defense against brown-headed cowbirds". Johnson St., Madison, WI 53706-1696 to be received 110 later than June 1995: Marc S. Dantzker, Duke University. "Disputing the (a) 7 copies of a I to 2 pg. curriculum vitae lack of sexual selection on male mating calls in (b) 7 copies of lhe ahstraet you have submitted for the genus BOlllbina". consideration by lhe Conference (cl Two letters of nomination lhat address the " Todd M. Freeberg. Indiana University. "Experimental applicant's expected contri hution to and henel1l approaches to studying cultural transmission of from lhe Conference, eilher sent with the mate recognition system behaviors in brown­ application in sealed, signed envelopes or sent headed cowbirds". separately to arrive by June 1. We wilt notify applicants of lhe funding outcome as Julie C. Hagelin, University of New Mexico. soon as possible, although checks may not be sent "Sexual selection, plumage ornamentation. and until just before lhe meeting. Travel reservations parental care in two species of New World quail". should be made before llotification or fUllding. [f you have questions contact Charles Snowdon at (608) Susan M. Kain, Cornell University. "TIle adaptive 262-3974 or SNOWDON(g)MACC.WISC.EDU. significance of variable reproductive behavior in the water strider Limnopours dissonis". 1995 ASS ANNUAL MEETING Ken Otter, Queen's University. "Mixed reproductive strategies of female black-capped chickadees". The ABS annual meeting witt be held from the 8-13 July at lhe University of Nebraska in Lincoln. For Denise S. Pope, Duke University. "The function of information, contact Dr. Al Kamil, School of lhe fiddler crab claw waving display". Biological Sciences, Manter Hall, University of Nebraska, Lincoln, NE 68588-0118, USA. 1996 ABS ANNUAL MEETING DIRECTION OF CORRESPONDENCE The 1996 ABS annual meeting will be held from the ABS Newsletter and general correspondence 3-8 August at Northern Arizona University, Flagstaff, concerning the Society: Susan Foster, Dept. of AZ. For information contact Con Slobodchikoff, Biological Sciences, University of Arkansas, Dept. of Biological Sciences, Northern Arizona Fayetteville, AR, 72701, USA. Deadlines are the University, Flagstaff, AZ 86011, USA. 15th of the month preceding each Newsletter. The next deadline is July 15, 1995. IN SEARCH OF MEETING SITES Articles submitted by members of the Society and judged by the Secretary to be appropriate are Sites for ABS meetings are needed for 1997, 1998, occasionally published in the ABS newsletter. The 1999. Anyone wishing to host one of our meetings publication of such material does not imply should contact Hugh Dingle to discuss requirements endorsement by the ABS of the opinions expressed. and to receive further information. We are especially interested in meeting sites in the eastern USA and in Animal Behaviour: manuscripts and editorial Canada given the recent geographic locations of our matters: Animal Behaviour Editorial Office, meetings. University of Washington, 119 Guthrie Hall, Box 351525, Seattle, WA 98195-1525, USA. Change of Address: missing or defective issues of ABS NOMINATIONS FOR 1995 Animal Behaviour: Randall Breitwisch, Dept. of ELECTION OF OFl"ICERS Biology, University of Dayton, 300 College Park, Dayton, OH, 45469-2320, USA. Elections will be held this year for the following ABS offices: Second President Elect, Secretary, and Member-at-Large. The slate of nominees provided by ARS OFFIC"~RS the ABS Nominating Committee will appear in the August Newsletter. The election ballot will be President: Hugh Dingle, Dept. of Entomology, published in the November Newsletter. Additional University of California, Davis, CA, 95616, USA. nominations may be made by letter to the E-mail: [email protected] Nominating Committee Chair Zuleyma Tang­ First President-elect: Lee Drickamer, Dept. or Martinez, Department of Biology, University of Zoology, Southern lIlinois University, Carbondale, Missouri, St. Louis, MO, 63121, USA. IL, 62901, USA. E-mail: [email protected] Nominations must be signed by five or more ABS Second President elect: Susan Riechert, Dept. members in good standing (ABS Constitution, of Zoology, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN, Article 8, Section l) and must be received by June 37996-0810, USA. E-mail: pa34628@ 10, 1995. utkvm l.utk.edu Past President: Zuleyma Tang-Martinez, Dept. of Biology, University of Missouri, St. Louis, MO. 63121, USA. E-mail: szthalp@umslvma Treasurer: Randall Breitwisch, Dept. of Biology, University of Dayton, 300 College Park, Dayton OB, ABS IN THE NEWS 45469. USA. E-mail: breitwis@ udavxb.oca.udayton.edu Lee Dugatkin, winner of the ABS Outstanding Secretary: Susan Foster, Dept. of Biological New Investigator Award in 1994, has recently been Sciences, University of Arkansas, Fayetteville, AR, featured in articles in a number of newspapers for 72701, USA.: E-mail: [email protected],cdu research conducted in collaboration with R. Craig Program Officer: John Byers. Dept. of Biological Sargent at the University of Kentucky, Lexington. Sciences, University of Idaho, Moscow. ID, 83843, The ABS award was mentioned in both the Columbia USA. E-mail: [email protected] Daily Tribune and the St. Louis Post-Dispatch. The Parliamentarian: George Waring, Dept. of research that generated the attention was: Zoology, Southern Illinois University, Carbondale, Dugatkin, L.A. and R.C. Sargent 1994. Male-male IL, 62901, USA. E-mail: [email protected] association patterns and female proximity in the Editor: Michael Beecher, Dept. of Psychology NI­ guppy, Poecilia reticulata. Behav. Eco!. Sociobio!. 25, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195, 35:141-145. USA. E-mail: [email protected] 2 Members-at-Large: Patricia Gowaty, Institute of Revenue Received: Ecology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, 30602, Membership Dues USA. E-mail: gowaty@ zookeeper.zoo.uga.edu. Regular 67,759 Christine Boake, Dept. of Zoology, University of Student 19,784 Tennessee, Knoxville, lN, 37996-0810, USA. Emeritus 840 E-mail: [email protected]. Multiple (joint) Memher 1,449 Katherine Wynne-Edwards, Dept of Biology, Queen's Fellow 2,058 University, Kingston, Canada, K7L 3N6. E-mail Late Fees 2,641 [email protected]. Interest 6,073 Historian: Donald Dewsbury, Dept. of Psychology, Label List Sales 1,827 University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, 32611, USA. Professional Certi fication Fees 765 E-mail: [email protected]. Graduate Programs in Behavior Booklet 358 Newsletter Only Subscriptions 668 ABS Officers-elect: Program Advertising 490 Contrihutions to Unrestricted Fund 904 The following new offtcers will take office at the end Contributions to Research Award Fund 2,667 of the 1995 annual meeting: COl1trihutions to Ethnic Diversity Fund 740 David Duvall, Second President-elect. Department of Miscellaneous Income 42 Life Sciences, Arizona State University West, P.O. Box 37100, Phoenix, AZ 85069, USA. Total Revenue Received 109,065 Kimberly Sullivan, Parliamentarian, Department of Biology, Utah State University, Logan, UT 84322­ Total }'unds Available 272,367 5305, USA. Anne Clark, Program Officer, Department of Expenditures Biological Sciences, State University of New York, Editor of Animal Behaviour 50,991 Binghamton, NY 13901. USA. Secretary 14,186 Jean-Guy Godin, Member-at-Large, Department of Treasurer 9,965 Biology. Mount Allison University. Sackviile, N.B. President 918 EOA ::ICO. Canada. Second President Eject (Allee Award) 395 First President Elect (Poster Award) 100 Par! iamentarian 131 Memhcr-at-Large (Research Grants) 7,250 ABS TREASURER'S REPORT Program Committee 7,426 Professional Certi fication Committee 935 Memhership Committee 749 STATEMENT OF CASH RECEIPTS AND Education Committee 359 DISBURSEMENTS Film Committee 345 I JULY 1993 TO 30 JUNE I 993 Puhlic Affairs Committee 80 Animal Care Committee 1,125 Beginning Balance: AIBS Annual Affiliation Fec 780 Nations Bank, Athens, GA Total Expenditures 95,735 Checking Account -1,594 Money Market Account 40,499 Ending Balance University Employees Federal Credit Union, Athens, Nations Bank, Athens, GA GA Regular Checking Account -7,930 Certificate of Deposit
Recommended publications
  • NEWSLETTER Animal Behavior Society
    NEWSLETTER Vol. 41, No.1 Animal Behavior February, 1996 Society A quarterly publication Susan A. Foster, Secretary Cristin Hulslander, Editorial Assistant Department of Biology, Clark University, 950 Main St., Worcester, MA 01610 USA DO YOU KNOW SCIENCE WRITERS? RESULTS OF THE 1995 ABS ELECTIONS The ABS Public Affairs Committee is interested in making contacts with science writers, reporters, or 155 ballots were cast in the 1995 election. This is editors in the national or local news media (NPR, Science News, Discovery, NY Times, etc.) who we J less than 5% of the membership. The following officers were elected: can invite to Animal Behavior meetings and send press releases of exciting research in animal behavior. Second President-Elect: Meredith West The Committee promises to treat all contacts with Secretary: Susan A. Foster the utmost respect and will not abuse the Member-At-Large: Ken Yasukawa relationship. Please forward names, addresses, and other pertinent information, including special areas of Representative, USECC, and delegate, IEC interest to the writer to: Council: Deborah M. Gordon Dr. Linda S. Rayor, Dept. Entomology, Cornell Representative, USECC, and alternate, IEC University, Ithaca, NY 14853, email: Council: Jerry F. Downhower Isrl @comell.edu. Congratulations to the new officers and representatives and thanks to all who ran for office. SYMPOSIA AND INVITED PAPER The new officers will begin their terms at the end of SESSION PROPOSALS the Annual Meeting in August 1996. 1997 ANNUAL MEETING Proposals for symposia and invited paper sessions to be held at the 1997 annual ABS meeting in College CALL FOR RESOLUTIONS Park, Maryland are invited.
    [Show full text]
  • Decoding Calls of the Wild
    BRAINPOWER: GROUNDBREAKING THINKERS IN WISCONSIN Decoding calls of the wild Marquette's 'Dr. Dolittle' uses computers to unlock the meaning behind snorts, rumbles and cheeps By MARK JOHNSON [email protected] Posted: Oct. 20, 2007 In childhood, Dr. Dolittle never had a pet of his own. The family kept fish, but he did not name or talk to them, feed them or clean their tanks. His enduring memory is that once, when he was 4 or 5, he banged the aquarium with a wooden block until the glass broke, and a catfish spilled out and flopped about on the floor. By his own admission, Dr. Dolittle wasn't "a big animal person." In some respects, he still isn't. He's more of a computer person, one who talks to technology the way others talk to pets, with an occasional flash of tough love -"Better not freeze up on me, or I'll get mad." His real name is Michael Johnson, and he's a 40-year-old Marquette University engineering professor, founder of the ambitious but little-known Dr. Dolittle Project. In a small lab at the Olin Engineering Center, Johnson puts recordings of whales, birds and elephants through sophisticated computer analysis, leading to important advances in the understanding of animals. His work is changing the way we listen to our fellow creatures and might just help us discover a language in their songs and rumbles. If such a claim sounds outrageous, consider an animal much on Johnson's mind of late: the prairie dog, spurned by ranchers and farmers across the West.
    [Show full text]
  • Prairie Dog Alarm Calls Encode Labels About Predator Colors
    Anim Cogn DOI 10.1007/s10071-008-0203-y ORIGINAL PAPER Prairie dog alarm calls encode labels about predator colors C. N. SlobodchikoV · Andrea Paseka · Jennifer L. Verdolin Received: 8 July 2008 / Revised: 29 November 2008 / Accepted: 9 December 2008 © Springer-Verlag 2008 Abstract Some animals have the cognitive capacity to Keywords Gunnison’s prairie dogs · diVerentiate between diVerent species of predators and gen- Referential communication · Alarm calls erate diVerent alarm calls in response. However, the pres- ence of any addition information that might be encoded into alarm calls has been largely unexplored. In the present Introduction study, three similar-sized human females walked through a Gunnison’s prairie dog (Cynomys gunnisoni) colony wear- An increasing body of evidence suggests that some animals ing each of three diVerent-colored shirts: blue, green, and have referential communication, in which they can incorpo- yellow. We recorded the alarm calls and used discriminant rate information into their vocalizations or signals about an function analysis to assess whether the calls for the diVer- object or event that is external to the animal (Evans 1997). ent-colored shirts were signiWcantly diVerent. The results This kind of communication has sometimes been termed showed that the alarm calls for the blue and the yellow functional reference (Evans et al. 1993; Evans 1997; Evans shirts were signiWcantly diVerent, but the green shirt calls and Evans 1999) because of the diYculty of determining were not signiWcantly diVerent from the calls for the yellow the meaning of the actual information that is encoded in the shirt. The colors that were detected, with corresponding signal.
    [Show full text]
  • NEWSLETTER Animal Behavior Society
    NEWSLETTER Vol. 40, No.4 Animal Behavior November, 1995 Society A quarterly publication Susan A. Foster, Secretary Cristin Hulslander, Editorial Assistant Department of Biology, Clark University, 950 Main St., Worcester, MA 01610 USA not at ABS. What I think we may be witnessing in VOTE, VOTE, VOTE!!!!! the next decade is a true integration of approaches 1995 ABS ELECTIONS involving proximate and ultimate causation of animaJ behavior. This should be an exciting time, for both Only a smaJl percentage of ABS members vote in the the younger scientists who are just entering our field annual election. This is not a good showing for an and those of us who have been a part of the discipline organization that takes pride in its democratic style. for 25 years or more. Election information and the baJlot are enclosed. At our 1995 Annual Meeting, the Executive Committee adopted a proposal establishing a new Service Award. Up to three such awards can be made in any given year. The proposaJ for these awards grew out of the need to have a form of recognition for MESSAGE FROM THE ABS PRESIDENT, those individuals who provide the ABS with LEE DRICKAMER outstanding and extended service, but who might not be considered for election as a Fellow. The latter It has become customary for the ABS President to honor is intended to recognize contributions in terms pen a few remarks to the membership at mid-year. I of research and scholarship. I hope that ABS members am happy to do so and prefer this vehicle over some will look for the announcement of these new ABS long-winded speech at our banquet at the Annual Service Awards in the Newsletter and that you will Meeting.
    [Show full text]
  • A DNA Fingerprint Analysis
    WellBeing International WBI Studies Repository Spring 1996 Social assemblages and mating relationships in prairie dogs: a DNA fingerprint analysis Steven E. Travis Northern Arizona University Con Slobodchikoff Northern Arizona University Paul Kefan Northern Arizona University Follow this and additional works at: https://www.wellbeingintlstudiesrepository.org/repbeh Part of the Animal Studies Commons, Behavior and Ethology Commons, and the Comparative Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Steven E. Travis, C. N. Slobodchikoff, Paul Kefan, Social assemblages and mating relationships in prairie dogs: a DNA fingerprint analysis, Behavioral Ecology, Volume 7, Issue 1, Spring 1996, Pages 95–100, https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/7.1.95 This material is brought to you for free and open access by WellBeing International. It has been accepted for inclusion by an authorized administrator of the WBI Studies Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Behavioral Ecology Vol. 7 No. 1: 95-100 Social assemblages and mating relationships in prairie dogs: a DNA fingerprint analysis Steven E. Travis, C. N. Slobodchikoff, and Paul Kefan Department of Biological Sciences, Box 5640, Northern Arizona University, Flagstaff, AZ 86011-5640, USA Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/beheco/article-abstract/7/1/95/222101 by guest on 23 September 2019 Mating system characterizations have been hindered by difficulties in accurately assigning parentage to offspring. We investigated the relationship between social assemblages and mating relationships in a territorial harem polygynous mammal, the Gunnison's prairie dog, using a combination of behavioral and molecular analyses. We demonstrate multiple paternity and an extraordinarily high incidence of extraterritorial fertilizations (i.e., 61% of all progeny), in combination with the existence of female kin groups.
    [Show full text]
  • Perceptual Specificity in the Alarm Calls of Gunnison's Prairie Dogs
    Behavioural Processes 73 (2006) 29–35 Perceptual specificity in the alarm calls of Gunnison’s prairie dogs Judith Kiriazis 1, C.N. Slobodchikoff ∗ Northern Arizona University, Flagstaff, AZ 86011, USA Received 20 August 2005; received in revised form 20 January 2006; accepted 30 January 2006 Abstract Gunnison’s prairie dogs have a complex alarm communication system. We show that the escape responses of prairie dogs to naturally occurring live predators differed depending upon the species of predator. We also show that playbacks of alarm calls that were elicited originally by the live predators produced the same escape responses as the live predators themselves. The escape responses fell into two qualitatively different categories: running to the burrow and diving inside for hawks and humans, and standing upright outside the burrow for coyotes and dogs. Within these two categories there were differences in response. For hawks, only the prairie dogs that were in the direct flight path of a stooping red-tailed hawk ran to their burrows and dove inside, while for humans and human alarm call playbacks there was a colony-wide running to the burrows and diving inside. For coyotes and coyote alarm call playbacks there was a colony-wide running to the burrows and standing alert at the burrow rims, while for domestic dogs and playbacks of alarm calls for domestic dogs the prairie dogs assumed an alert posture wherever they were feeding, but did not run to their burrows. These responses to both the live predators and to predator-elicited alarm calls suggest that the alarm calls of Gunnison’s prairie dogs contain meaningful referential information about the categories of predators that approach a colony of prairie dogs.
    [Show full text]
  • Awareness, Emotions, and Heart by Dr. Marc Bekoff
    \\server05\productn\L\LCA\9-1\LCA110.txt unknown Seq: 1 12-MAY-03 12:32 A REVIEW OF MINDING ANIMALS: AWARENESS, EMOTIONS, AND HEART BY DR. MARC BEKOFF By Michael Tobias* Dr. Marc Bekoff’s book, Minding Animals: Awareness, Emotions, and Heart,1 like much of his work, is a tantalizing and ambitious over- view of animal rights and liberation, approaching the subject through the lens of ethological data, anecdote, and philosophy. For more than three decades, Bekoff, who recently coined the expression “deep ethol- ogy,”2 has been observing a variety of species at close quarters, from Adelie penguins to coyotes to wolves. His tent sites have ranged from the Western Peninsula of the Antarctic to Yellowstone to Boulder, Col- orado, where he is a professor. Bekoff expects the best from humanity and asks scientists and the public to endorse a code of ethics that rec- ognizes the cognitive and emotional mysteries of all other life forms. It is a simple, if utopian, plea against global ecological mayhem, animal cruelty, and runaway consumerism—but what distinguishes his ap- proach in this book is the overwhelming evidence he elicits to support his goals. Bekoff left a graduate program in neurobiology at medical school because he refused to kill animals as part of the curriculum. Paul Ehr- lich refers to the dilemma as the “world of wounds” for students enter- ing the study of ecology with a dream of healing the world.3 In the years since then, Bekoff has campaigned to raise awareness in the sci- entific world to the possibility of healing—to understand there is a “deep science” merging traditional disciplines, induction, and the old style of intrusive experiments with a new paradigm of “aesthetic and sentient experiences.” His book is a feast of convincing arguments and analogies that leaves no doubt about the impending revolution in the scientific, legal, and consumer realms regarding our treatment of other species.
    [Show full text]
  • Uncorrected Proof
    Behavioural Processes xxx (2006) xxx–xxx 3 Perceptual specificity in the alarm calls of Gunnison’s prairie dogs 1 ∗ 4 Judith Kiriazis , C.N. Slobodchikoff 5 Northern Arizona University, Flagstaff, AZ 86011, USA Received 20 August 2005; received in revised form 20 January 2006; accepted 30 January 2006 6 7 Abstract 8 Gunnison’s prairie dogs have a complex alarm communication system. We show that the escape responses of prairie dogs to naturally occurring 9 live predators differed depending upon the species of predator. We also show that playbacks of alarm calls that were elicited originally by the live 10 predators produced the same escape responses as the live predators themselves. The escape responses fell into two qualitatively different categories: 11 running to the burrow and diving inside for hawks and humans, and standing upright outside the burrow for coyotes and dogs. Within these two 12 categories there were differences in response. For hawks, only the prairie dogs that were in the direct flight path of a stooping red-tailed hawk ran 13 to their burrows and dove inside, while for humans and human alarm call playbacks there was a colony-wide running to the burrows and diving 14 inside. For coyotes and coyote alarm call playbacks there was a colony-wide running to the burrows and standing alert at the burrow rims, while 15 for domestic dogs and playbacks of alarm calls for domestic dogs the prairie dogs assumed an alert posture wherever they were feeding, but did 16 not run to their burrows. These responses to both the live predators and to predator-elicited alarm calls suggest that the alarm calls of Gunnison’s 17 prairie dogs contain meaningful referential information about the categories of predators that approach a colony of prairie dogs.
    [Show full text]
  • Report from the Burrow Forecast of the Prairie Dog
    Report from the Burrow Forecast of the Prairie Dog A Report from WildEarth Guardians By Taylor Jones February 2, 2011 MISSION STATEMENT WILDEARTH GUARDIANS protects and restores the wildlife, wild places and wild rivers of the American West. Inquiries about this report and WILDEARTH GUARDIANS’ work can be made directly to: Taylor Jones, WILDEARTH GUARDIANS 1536 Wynkoop St., Suite 301 Denver, CO, 80202 303-573-4898 [email protected] Cartography: Kurt Menke, Bird’s Eye View GIS Front and back cover photos: Rich Reading Thank you to the state and federal agencies and non-governmental organizations who provided information for this report and the reviewers who provided helpful feedback. Outside Reviewers: Kristy Bly, Lauren McCain, Richard P. Reading, Con Slobodchikoff (“Prairie Dog Communication” section only), Lindsey Sterling-Krank. Review does not constitute an endorsement of the 2011 Report from the Burrow. © WILDEARTH GUARDIANS. All rights reserved. No part of this report may be used in any manner whatsoever without written permission from the publisher, WILDEARTH GUARDIANS, except in the case of brief quotations. Report from the Burrow 2011 Report from the Burrow 2011: Forecast of the Prairie Dog An expansive redefinition of the human relationship to nature requires not only an open international, national, state, county, and community-wide dialog, it also requires each human being, in their own way, to reexamine and redefine their physical and emotional relationship to non-human entities. –Slobodchikoff et al. 2009, p. 209 The greatness of a nation and its moral progress can be judged by the way its animals are treated. –Mahatma Gandhi 2010 was a year of intense and often acrimonious political debates.
    [Show full text]
  • Prairie Dog” and “Gunnison's”
    GUNNISON’S PRAIRIE DOG Cynomys gunnisoni In the Office of Endangered Species U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service United States Department of Interior Petition to the U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service to List the Gunnison’s Prairie Dog as an Endangered or Threatened Species Under the Endangered Species Act, 16 U.S.C. § 1531 et Seq. (1973 as amended), and to Designate Critical Habitat Primary Petitioner Forest Guardians 312 Montezuma Ave. Suite A Santa Fe, New Mexico 87501 (505) 988-9126 23 February 2004 Gunnison’s prairie dog photo © Jess Alford Petition Prepared by: Nicole J. Rosmarino, Ph.D. With Assistance from: Paula Martin Additional Petitioners: Scientists & Academics 1. Dr. Con Slobodchikoff (biologist with expertise in Gunnison’s prairie dog, professor) 2. Ana Davidson (biologist with expertise in Gunnison’s prairie dog) 3. Jennifer Verdolin (biologist with expertise in Gunnison’s prairie dog) 4. Dr. Jean-Luc Cartron (biologist, with expertise in Gunnison’s prairie dog ecosystem) 5. Dr. Paul J. Polechla Jr. (biologist, with expertise in Gunnison’s prairie dog ecosystem) 6. Paula Martin (biologist) 7. Glenn Harper (biologist) 8. Dr. Wayne Bundy (geologist; president of Santa Fe Geological Society) 9. Dr. Linda Wiener (entomologist) 10. David Lamkin (retired mechanical engineering professor) 11. Dr. Charles Ervin (psychologist; animal behaviorist) 12. Greg Litus (horticulturalist) 13. Hollace L. Bristol (Ph.D. candidate in Education and Sustainability, Northern Arizona University) 14. Marcus Hamilton (Ph.D. candidate in Anthropology, University of New Mexico) Different Walks of Life Realtors & Homebuilders 15. Bennett Hammer (Realtor in New Mexico) 16. Tish Bogan-Ozmun (Realtor in Arizona) 17.
    [Show full text]
  • Rhyme, Reason, and Animal Rights: Elizabeth Costello’S Regressive View of Animal Consciousness and Its Implications for Animal Liberation Norm Phelps1
    Journal for Critical Animal Studies, Volume VI, Issue 1, 2008 Rhyme, Reason, and Animal Rights: Elizabeth Costello’s Regressive View of Animal Consciousness and its Implications for Animal Liberation Norm Phelps1 Abstract Novelist J. M. Coetzee is widely credited with “shifting the species boundary” (Mason 129) in his fiction to acknowledge the personhood of at least some nonhuman animals, thereby advancing the cause of animal protection, which he is known to support in his personal life (Satya). In this article, I will argue that in Elizabeth Costello Coetzee treats animals as alien and unknowable because he believes, contrary to a rapidly growing body of evidence, that they lack language. Building on this belief, he uses animals instrumentally to argue for the superiority of imagination to reason. And finally, because of his insistence on the priority of the imagination over reason, he would place animals solely at the mercy of human good will and leave them unprotected by the kinds of laws that protect human beings. Background From antiquity to the present, our most acclaimed thinkers and artists have endeavored to erect and defend what John Wesley called “the barrier between men and brutes, the line which they cannot pass.” (Wesley) This “great wall of humanity”—as it might be dubbed—is designed to maintain the uniqueness of human beings and our superiority to all other species. Over the years, the wall has assumed various forms, as successive scientific discoveries made earlier versions impossible to sustain. “[F]irst, it was possession of a soul, then „reason,‟ then tool use, then tool making, then altruism, then language, then the production of linguistic novelty, and so on .
    [Show full text]
  • Lessons from Animal Sentience: Towards a New Humanity Jonathan Balcombe
    Volume 1 Nature's Humans Article 5 2016 Lessons from Animal Sentience: Towards a New Humanity Jonathan Balcombe Follow this and additional works at: https://encompass.eku.edu/tcj Part of the Animal Studies Commons, and the Philosophy Commons Recommended Citation Balcombe, Jonathan (2016) "Lessons from Animal Sentience: Towards a New Humanity," The Chautauqua Journal: Vol. 1 , Article 5. Available at: https://encompass.eku.edu/tcj/vol1/iss1/5 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by Encompass. It has been accepted for inclusion in The hC autauqua Journal by an authorized editor of Encompass. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Balcombe: Lessons from Animal Sentience JONATHAN BALCOMBE LESSONS FROM ANIMAL SENTIENCE: TOWARDS A NEW HUMANITY Introduction – Moral Progress, and a Paradox Many years before I became a professional biologist, as a boy of about 9, I used to explore the wet ditches on each side of the railroad tracks that bordered the lakes and bays at the summer camp I went to north of Toronto, Canada. I was drawn to the many frogs and dragonflies there, and if it was a particularly good day I’d find a garter snake. One day, a scruffy-looking man ambled along the tracks. In one hand he held a heavy cloth sack and a dipping net on the end of a pole. In the other he held a leopard frog. He asked me to hold the frog. I took the frog while he waded into the ditch to catch another one. I didn’t know why he was catching frogs, but I didn’t like their prospects.
    [Show full text]