<<

THE INFLUENCE OF MOVIE ON AUDIENCE

REACTION TO

by

STEVEN DAVID GARZA, B.S.

A THESIS

IN

MASS COMMUNICATIONS

Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of Texas Tech University in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of

MASTER OF ARTS

Approved

Chairperson of the Committee

Accepted

Dean of the Graduate School

December, 2003 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Several individuals contributed greatly to this thesis. Without these individuals, I would not have been able to succeed in this endeavor.

First, I am forever indebted and sincerely grateful to Dr. Coy

Calllson for his support, guidance, and direction in the development and execution of this thesis. His research expertise guided the quality of this thesis. I would like to thank committee members, Dr. Ed Youngblood for jumping in whole-heartedly and for his meticulous attention to detail and

Dr. Jimmie Reeves for his encouraging words that did not "suck."

A special thanks goes to Craig Childre, for contributing his time and talent in developing my stimulus materials.

This work could not have been completed without the support, encouragement, advice, and patience of my family and adopted families

(Bates & Hales). Words cannot adequately express my gratitude for their emotional, financial, and physical (i.e., food) contribution to this effort.

I gratefully wish to thank Dr. Jerry Hudson and Dr. Liz Watts, for their encouragement but more importantly their patience. It's done! The support and encouragement of the staff of the School of Mass

Communications at Texas Tech University are also appreciated. Finally, thank you Tory, Jason, and Donna, for the many weekend getaways and much needed distractions.

This thesis is dedicated to the memory of my mother, Mary Frances

Garza, who instilled in me a purpose of and love of family. I will forever miss her optimism and words of wisdom. TABLE OF CONTENTS

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS ii

ABSTRACT vi

LIST OF TABLES vii

LIST OF FIGURES viii

CHAPTER

I INTRODUCTION 1 The Payne Fund Study 2

II BRAND PLACEMENT IN MOVIES 5 The Marketability of Tie-Ins 11 Brand Placement Research 13 Efficacy of Brand Placements in Movies 1 5 Brand Attitude Change 16 The Marketability of the Genre 21 Humor Advertising Research 23 Humor and Attention 24 Humor and Persuasion 26 Humor and Liking 28 Humor and Placement 32 Recap 33 Hypothesis and Research Question 34

III METHODOLOGY 39 Overview 39 Research Participants 39 Procedure 40 Measures 41 Stimulus Material 44 Measurement 48

IV IV RESULTS 50 Preliminary Analysis 50 Brand Recall and Recognition Hypothesis 51 Attitude Toward the Brand Hypothesis 52 Attitude Toward Product Placement Hypothesis 56 Research Question One 58

V DISCUSSION 65 First Hypothesis 65 Second Hypothesis 67 Third Hypothesis 71 Research Question One 74 Limitations 11 Considerations for Future Research 79

VI CONCLUSION 84

REFERENCES 86

APPENDIX: QUESTIONNAIRE 93

V ABSTRACT

The use of brand placements is quite evident in movies today. The success of some brands reporting a phenomenal sales increase may be partly dependant on the type of movie in which the brand is placed.

Employing 200 university students, this initial experimental study examined the effects of movie genre on brand placement. Students completed a questionnaire after watching and evaluating one 30-minute movie clip categorized by genre—comedy, , and science . The study compared the effects of genre on brand recall, brand liking, and consumer's reactions toward brand placement. Central to the practice of using brand placement in movies is the belief that the humor found in a comedy movie would help create a favorable response toward brand placement. It did not. The comedy genre did not outrank the other on brand recall/recognition, brand liking, and attitude toward brand placement. However, this research did confirm the findings of previous research that prominent brand placements promote a significantly higher level of brand awareness than subtle brand placements.

VI LIST OF TABLES

1. Movie, Classification, and Brand Placement List 49

2. Mean Results for Demographic Questions 61

3. Mean Results for Recall and Recognition 62

4. Mean Results for Attitude Toward the Product 63

5. Mean Results for Attitude Toward Product Placement 64

VII LIST OF FIGURES

1. Photo 1, There's Something About Mary 81

2. Photo 2, Mission to Mars 81

3. Photo 3, The Usual Suspects 82

4. Photo 4, Me, Myself, and Irene 82

5. Photo 5, The Philadelphia Experiment 83

6. Photo 6, Monster's Ball 83

Vlll CHAPTER I

INTRODUCTION

Storytelling was used in many early cultures as part of religious rituals (Bywater & Sobchack, 1989). The purpose of was seen as pure and good, and its effects on the participants uplifting and noble.

But somewhere along the way, storytelling entered secular life.

Storytelling became , and its power to affect emotions and motivate was socially utilized. Today, stories told in movies according to

Bywater and Sobchack (1989), are a more powerful stimulus than those told in other media. They said,

almost from the beginning, society at large expressed its concern about how the medium may affect a person's behavior for good or for ill. By the end of the first decade of the 20th century everyone was aware of the motion picture as part of the social fabric of even the smallest community, catering to the public's desire for , adventure, romance, comedy, and spectacle, (p. 109)

One of the first community concerns regarding media came from

Christian moralists during the earliest days of the 's depiction of immoral behavior (Bywater & Sobchack, 1989). By the late 1920s, the majority of the community called out for action, and a massive investigation of by social scientists was funded to find out just how "bad" (p. 109) the effects of movie viewing could be on innocent minds (Bywater & Sobchack, 1989). According to Jowett and

Linton (1989), during this period, movies received more attention and were the subject of more research than any other form of .

The investigation into movies accelerated the most significant milestone in the development of mass communication as a scientific field of study—the Payne Fund study.

The Pavne Fund Studv

In 1928, Reverend William H. Short, executive director of the

Motion Picture Research Council, received a $200,000 grant from a private charitable organization called the Payne Fund to support an investigation of what effects motion pictures had on children (Jowett &

Linton, 1989). To lead the investigation. Short organized a group of prominent educators, university sociologists, and psychologists into the

Committee on Educational Research of the Payne Fund. The effort, which extended from 1929 until 1935, produced a series of 12 studies. These

12 studies considered variables, such as: attendance; content; the relation of behavior in to normal social behavior; the effect of films on sleep and health; the emotional effect of film viewer ship; the relationship between film attendance and delinquency; and the method of

teaching children to distinguish between good and bad films. The results

of this investigation were not as convincing as many of the medium's

critics had hoped. Instead, the study fell short of admitting any evidence

of strong effects. The researchers concluded that factors other than

going to a movie influenced an individual's life (Jowett & Linton, 1989).

One of the more controversial of these studies. Movies and Conduct

(1933) by sociologist Herbert Blumer, investigated the influences of

motion pictures on general day-to-day behavior. Blumer used the

autobiographical method, in which respondents (college students, high

school youths, factory workers, and office personnel) recalled earlier

influences of movies on their lives and on specific activities such as

modes of dress, hairstyles, and forms of communication. The results

indicated that the content of movies served as a substantial influence on young students. The research participants reported that they had

imitated the movie characters openly in beautification, mannerisms, and attempts at lovemaking.

According to Lowery and DeFleur (1995), the Payne Study findings were correct—for that time. They said. America was not a media society except in a limited sense during the 1920s. The only mature medium was the newspaper. Radio was only a toy. The movies came with a rush—almost overnight—to delight, frighten, thrill and fascinate an audience of millions of children who had never seen anything like them. The idea that this new medium, suddenly thrust upon a society just emerging from the Victorian era, could have profound and visible effects upon such an audience does not seem far-fetched, (p. 41)

The study of movies and specific research aimed at understanding the nature and extent of movies' effects declined almost completely after the introduction of television in the mid-1940s (Bywater & Sobchack,

1989). The direction focused more on television and its effect on audience behavior. However, studies conducted by Glucksmann (1972),

Howitt (1977) and Cline (1974) acknowledged that the researchers findings could be inferred to film.

Even with the controversy over the social influence of motion pictures, a commercial phenomenon evolved. Today, the motion picture is being used as a mass communication medium attracting marketers and advertisers for its enormous promotional possibilities. These advertisers and marketers pay to place brands within the of movies for the purpose of promotional exposure. This practice is referred to as product placement or brand placement. CHAPTER II

BRAND PLACEMENT IN MOVIES

Brand placement is the promotional technique of placing consumer brand products or services in motion pictures in return for or promotional exposure of the films in a marketer's advertisement (Clark,

1991). The promotional technique of brand placement was used as early as 1945 when noticeably drank Jack Daniel's bourbon whiskey in Mildred Pierce (Reed, 1989). Yet, it was not until the tremendous success of 's E.T. in 1982, where sales of

Reese's Pieces' soared 66% in the three months following the movies release—reportedly as a result of the candy's exposure in the film—that brand placement in feature releases became an important element of consumer marketing programs (Reed, 1989). A more recent example of this form of promotion is seen in the film. Men in Black, where actors Will

Smith and Tommy Lee Jones wear Ray-Ban sunglasses.

According to Ray-Ban, sales of the sunglasses tripled to almost $5 million after the release of the movie in 1997 (Basset, 2000).

This promotional technique, now an advertising/marketing industry staple, continues to grow, as does academic research to understand it. Gillen (1995) stated that brand placements are now seen in many types of entertainment vehicles other than film or TV. Timex has agreed to pay

$1.3 million to have their products intertwined into the plot of a Tom

Clancy , a CD-ROM, game, and a movie (Benezra &

Ebenkamp, 1997). Product placement has even reached the industry (Nelson, 2000).

Nelson's (2000) study was the first to examine the effectiveness of product placements in a computer or video game. The video game used was. Gran Turismo 2 on a Playstation One, which contained background ads (billboards, sideboards) with products such as motor oil

(e.g., Pennzoil), cars (e.g., ), and tires (e.g., Goodyear). The study was conducted in a comfortable carpeted room, with couches, low lighting, and refreshments in attempt to emulate a game-player's living room. A total of 20 self-selected game-players (either casual or hard­ core) participated in the study having responded to flyers and ads. These participants completed a survey after 1 5-20 minutes of playing the game.

The survey measured their attitude toward the brand and attitude toward brand placement. Recall was measured using free and aided recall questions. The participants were able to recall about 25-30% of the brands displayed in the video game.

Today the practice is growing at an extraordinary rate including placement opportunities in the television market. Brand placements in television should become more prevalent and necessary with the mergence of new technology, such as personal video recorders (PVR-also known as TiVo or Replay TV). This technology finds and digitally records television shows in real time, allowing viewers to watch what they want, whenever they want. This technology also allows viewers to easily zip through commercials or eliminate them altogether. As a result, broadcasters are expected to lose $5.5 billion in television ad expenditures in 2007 (Curry, 2003). A Yankee Group study estimates that 19.1 million households, nearly 20% of the U.S. total, will have personal video recorders by the end of 2006. Currently, 3.8 million U.S. homes own a PVR, and the study predicts that figure will climb to 19.1 million by 2006. The study estimates that 80% of viewed prime-time programming currently is time-shifted in PVR households, and 65 to 70 percent of advertising is fast-forwarded during that programming (Curry, 2003). Undoubtedly, placing products in the context of programming will

become even more prevalent as ownership of PVRs climbs.

Broadcasters and advertisers will need to develop progressive

advertising techniques to hold or influence viewer's attitudes and

attention. They will "need to be planning now for the day when PVRs have

a penetration in households," said the Association of National Advertisers

President-CEO Bob Liodice, "Product placement is one avenue. Program

sponsorship is another opportunity" (Curry, 2003).

Now, more than a dozen companies have been organized to

expedite the placement of brands in films (McGill, 1989).

Similar to an actor's booking agent, these brand placement companies

find and review scripts to locate possible roles for their clients' products.

Spillman (1985) said, "Coke and each have their own full-time

employees who search scripts for potential placement opportunities" (p.

55).

These placement opportunities, according to Oliver (1986), come

in two types of brand placements in movies: creative and on-set placement. Creative placement involves developing ingenious ways to

embed the brand in the film. An example of this type of placement is the

8 use of the Zaie's logo on a park bench in Back to the Future. The other type of placement, on-set placement, occurs when the product on the set

is seen in its natural environment, such as the Tide detergent by the washing machine in Mr. Mom. However, the advertising industry categorizes placements into one of three types. The first category

includes a brand, logo, or brand name being clearly seen within the

background of the action or a character holding the brand. In the second category, a character is seen using a product while the brand name is clearly visible. And in the third category, a character, either with or without the actual brand being visible is depicted voicing the brand's name (Kinsley, 1990).

Eddington's (1991) study on effectiveness tested the advertising industry's belief that the closer the product is in proximity to the celebrity in a movie the higher amount of brand awareness and brand liking elicited in viewers. She concluded that the audience's feelings toward the movie in general and the audience's feelings toward the movie star contribute to enhanced brand attitude but not brand awareness and recall, as the advertising industry believes. However, a study by Gupta and Lord (1998) supported the industry's standard by examining the impact of placement type on brand awareness. In this study of 274 undergraduate students, brand recall was assessed by asking the participants to list each product or company they recalled having seen or heard referenced in the movies. Big (products:

Pepsi, Pizza Hut), Prq/ectX (products/company: Pepsi, United Way) and

Ferris SueZ/er (product: Ferrari). In each of the 30-minute movie clips, the brand was prominently (placements that are highly visible due to their size, positioning or centrality to the action in the scene) and subtlety placed (placements that serve as background props). For example, in the clip they used of the movie Big, a Pepsi machine becomes part of the action when throws an object at it and then retrieves the can of Pepsi from the machine. When the camera zoomed in and filled the screen with the Pepsi Machine/can, this illustrated a prominent placement. Later in the movie, a subtle placement was used when a Pizza

Hut box and two slices of pizza on a plate were seen. The subtleness refers to the fact the pizza was not mentioned and/or consumed. The researchers found that prominent brand placements promote a significantly higher level of brand awareness than subtle brand

10 placements. These findings support the film studio and advertising

industries practice of charging higher prices for on-set prominent

placements (Darlin, 1995).

The cost of such a placement typically ranges from $25,000 to

$225,000, or even higher if the sponsor desires a highly visible placement

(Gupta & Lord, 1998). The costs "depend on the prominence of the

placement, who the producer is, expected distribution, and other

factors," said Caria Denham, senior account executive who is also

credited for the placement of Hershey's Reese's Pieces in E.T. (Higgins,

1985, p. 6).

The Marketability of Tie-ins

A successful movie can deliver millions of dollars of publicity for brands placed within them (Higgins, 1985). Big screen placements, say agents, provide more bang for the buck, than television," according to

Reed (1989, p. 103). Higgins (1985) stated that with international distribution, pay-TV, and videocassette rentals, the typical movie reaches more than 100 million consumers. However, with the emergence of new technologies and services such as the Internet, and the low cost of

VHS tapes, distribution and exposure more than likely exceeds billions of

11 consumers. While placement will generate millions of viewers' impressions nationwide, the goal of the advertiser is to generate sales. Therefore, advertisers build promotions around the movie's brand placement, referred to as tie-in promotions. It is estimated that corporate America paid about $200 million in the form of tie-ins to Hollywood for its 1995 summer movies (Bush, 1994). Denham said, "If you don't bring the product tie-in into the store level, you're not getting what you should get from placement" (Higgins, 1985, p. 6). Tomorrow Never Dies, the last

James Bond , garnered the largest product placement deal in history. An agreement worth more than $100 million dollars included

Pierce Brosnan () in a series of TV and magazine ads prior to the films release promoting products that are displayed in the movie—BMW roadsters and motorcycles, Heineken beer, Smirnoff vodka.

Omega watches, Ericsson cellular phones, Brioni and Visa credit cards (Bassett, 2000). Denham concluded, "The first step is to pick the right movie. Then you've got to get maximum exposure in the film and convert that exposure into store traffic" (Cited in Higgins, 1985).

But before a placement company or corporation can pick the right movie, there are many factors to consider: the producers; the expected

12 distribution; the prominence of the placement; the script itself; and/or possibly the movie's genre. With all these factors to consider picking the right movie for a brand placement becomes complicated. One factor placement companies and corporations regard as an inarguable truth is product placements are effective and recent research suggests the placement industry is built on solid ground.

Brand Placement Research

The practice may be established, but the and research relating to brand placement is still in its infancy. In fact, much of the academic research on brand placement began within the last two . Many of the first studies shed light on consumers' attitudes toward brand placements in films (Nebenzahl & Secunda, 1993; Ong &

Meri, 1994; Babin & Carder, 1996; Gupta & Gould, 1997). Nebenzahl and

Secunda (1993) concluded: (1) Overall, consumers do not object to brand placement; (2) consumers view it as an effective marketing communication medium which should be allowed; and (3) consumers are tired of traditional commercials and would rather be exposed to less obtrusive forms of marketing communications.

13 Another study addressed whether audience attitudes toward brand placement was consistent across other nations and cultures. Building upon prior research done by Gupta and Gould (1997), a study done by

Karrh, Frith and Callison (2001) compared consumer attitudes of and Eastern cultures with regard to brand placement. A total of 194 college students participated in the survey—97 participants from universities in the USA and 97 participants from a university in Singapore.

The study questioned whether American students were more attentive to brand placements, and if they were more likely to assume that a brand's appearance in a film is the result of a paid advertisement than their

Singaporean counterparts. The study also sought to determine whether ethical concerns about brand placement were greater among audiences in

Singapore, a traditionally more conservative culture. The researchers found that the American participants were more likely to perceive brand placements as paid advertisements. The Singaporean participants had a greater concern for the ethics of brand placements. However, both

American and Singaporean participants were equally attentive to brand placements in films and were likely to admit that their purchasing patterns were affected by the brands they saw in films. The researchers

14 found that building a brand image across cultural boundaries might not be successful in some cultures that have a less positive view of brand placement.

Although, these studies concentrated on the consumers' attitudes toward brand placement, acceptability, and ethics, they failed to assess

brand placement's effectiveness.

Efficacy of Brand Placements in Movies

The effectiveness of brand placement depends on whether or not

the movie viewer notices and recalls the brand placed in the film. There

have been several empirical examinations of recall effects and brand

placement (Vollmers & Mizerski, 1994; Karrh 1994; Babin & Carder,

1995, 1996; Gupta & Lord, 1998). Vollmers and Mizerski (1994) tested

effectiveness through recall in which 71 college students recalled brands

placed within movie clips. They found that 96% of the participants

specified that they were aware of a brand placement, and 93% of the

participants correctly identified brands that had appeared in movie scenes

they had viewed.

In a recent study by Brennan and Dubas (1999), 90 research

participants were asked to list all of the brands they recalled after they

15 watched the movies Rocky III and Rocky V. These movies contained various types of brand placements (e.g., audio brand name plus visual, subtle visual, or blatant visual). The study was done to determine the extent to which brand placement type and exposure time explained the rate at which participants recognized brand names affiliated with brand placements in films. They found that audio cues were more effective than subtle visuals without audio, but blatant visuals had the most effective means of instilling brand recall among viewers. Their finding suggests that the greater use of sensory cues may increase effectiveness of brand recall. Their findings also support the industry practice of classifying brand placements into creative and on-set, as product placement type has a significant effect on viewer recognition, even when the measure of viewer recognition was adjusted for the impacts of false recognition.

Brand Attitude Change

Karrh (1998b) suggested that brand placements are effective through positive positioning and/or linking with an attractive character.

Changing brand attitude is the goal of placement companies. It is finding a scene or event in a movie, which will connect a positive emotional experience to the brand. According to Mizerski and White (1986),

16 marketers will opt to use the positive emotion stimulated by a particular scene to produce an affected-based attitude toward the brand. They said

by pairing an emotional cue, such as a warm or positive scene in the movie, with a brand, the marketer can stimulate an affective response toward the brand. The association of emotional responses, such as happiness, with brand-related cues, should result in a positive image for the product. It is thought that this positive attitude would be later accessed by the consumer and lead to the purchase and consumption of the product, (p. 68)

Texas Instruments, for example, reported increased profits on its

"Speak and Spell" after E.T. 's emotional scene to "phone home"

(Bronson, 1984).

However, sometimes viewers are exposed to negative emotional scenes in a movie. Many image-conscious corporations feel these negative experiences can transfer over to the brand. It is believed that many placement agencies and their clients have been known to avoid placements in violent or horror themes, "R-rated" movies, and scenes which depict the brand in a negative light (King 1991; Mork, 1993). Bush

(1994) said, "a perception as anything less than squeaky clean is considered a serious problem" (p. 7). For example, when M&M/Mars turned down participation in E.T, it rushed to supply the makers of

Gremlins with their products before reading the script. An M&M/Mars

17 official reacted with bitter disappointment to the movie's violence: "We made a mistake. can't do anything positive for our image"

(Bronson, 1984, p. 44). In another example, Coca-Cola executives were upset over the use of their polar bear campaign, which was intercut, with images of violence, including a headless, bloody body, in Oliver Stone's violent drama Natural Born Killers (Bush, 1994). Although these charges made by corporations and advertising agencies may seem sound, there has been no research to confirm that negative cinema experiences can transfer to the brand.

With brand placements in movies, there is an opportunity for transformational advertising to occur. Transformational advertising involves the association of "the experience of using/consuming the advertised brand with a unique set of psychological characteristics" (Puto

& Wells, 1984, p. 638). Transformational advertisement makes the experience of using the brand more enjoyable by connecting the experience of the ad with that of using the brand in such an intimate fashion that "consumers cannot remember the brand without recalling the experience generated by the advertisement" (Puto & Wells, 1984, p.

638). Russell (1998) believes that transformational and affect transfer

18 procedures are in in the formation of linkages between a movie and the brand placement. He suggested that these findings of linkages could be important for buying behavior because the decision to purchase a product is likely to be the result of such linkages. In a two-part study done by Edell and Burke (1987), the role of feelings-based responses to ads was explored. A total of 61 students were recruited from a university campus to participate in both studies. The first study used 29 participants to measure attitude toward brands prior to viewing 10-video taped commercials. These participants were asked how they felt about the brand and the ad after viewing the commercials. The second study group watched six 30-second commercials embedded within the movies

The African Queen and Casablanca. These participants were asked for their overall evaluation of the ads, the extent to which the ads had elicited a list of 56 feelings, their judgments of the ads on a list of 26 different characteristics, their beliefs about the brands' attributes, and their evaluation of these attributes. From both studies researchers found that positive and negative feelings contributed to an ad's effectiveness and that feelings influence attitudes toward the brand, attitudes toward the ad, and beliefs about a brand's attributes. They also found that

19 transformational advertising connects the experience of the advertisement with the experience of using the brand. They said, "The emotions experienced during consumption should be the same emotions stimulated by use of the brand in the movies" (p. 430). Therefore, it can

be interpreted that brand placements can help the consumer expect

certain effects or emotions a brand can provide following purchase and

use.

As mentioned earlier, the goal of placement companies is to find a

scene or event in a movie, which will connect a positive emotional

experience to the brand. Achieving that goal of tying a product or brand

to a positive emotional experience has never been specific to just one

type of movie genre. In fact, brand placements have never been specific

to just one type of movie genre. For instance, Dr Pepper was creatively

scripted in the drama, ; Starbucks in the successful comedy,

Austin Powers: The Spy Who Shagged Me; Jiffy Pop in the horror, Scream;

and Taco Bell in the . Demolition Man. There has yet to be a

study done on whether a specific genre is more effective for brand

placement. However, there has been research done on the successful

20 marketability of the comedy genre-a genre that carries positive emotional experiences.

The Marketability nf the Comedy Genrp

The top 10 grossing movies of each year in the US are listed on the

InternetMovieDatabase website (http://us.imdb.eom/Top/1. This website also labels each movie by genre. By averaging the top 10 grossing movies of each year from 1990 to 2001, the comedy genre averaged 32.5% of the total top grossing movies of the past 11 years. In television, comedy accounts for 38% of the primetime programming on America's four major networks 2001-2002 schedule. followed at 36%, reality shows at

1 5%, news, sports at 7%, and finally movies/other at 4% (Pedder,

2002).

The success of in attracting an audience to television is best revealed in Brooks and Marsh's chronological examination of prime time shows from 1950 to 1979 and Nielsen Media Research (1990) of the . In the 1950s, television programming with comedic skits, such as The Skelton Show and The Show and the emergence of the situation comedy, such as / Love Lucy and The Aldrich

Family accounted for 33% of the top rated programs (Zillmann & Bryant,

21 1991). But by the , 55% of the top rated shows were comedies.

Sitcoms were the most popular, but comedy-variety shows such as.

Rowan and Martin's Laugh-In and The Comedy Hour according to Zillmann and Bryant (1991) "added diversity to the comedy fare that Americans had come to love" (p. 264). With situational comedies like , , and Three's Company, comedies in the accounted for 46% of the highest rated programs. During the 1980s, comedies claimed 49% of the top television shows. The family comedy format in the 1980s dominated ratings as well with shows like. and (Zillmann & Bryant,

1991).

In 1989, Weiner classified Variety magazine's "Top 100 All-Time

Film Rental Champs" (1990) into specific genres. He found that 40 of the top 100 films were comedies. In a list created by Brooks and Marsh

(1988) of the top 100 television shows of all time, 46% were comedies.

Comedy has become and remains a very important today. Its ability to draw large audiences and garner a profitable share of the market illustrates why the comedy genre could prove to be an effective vehicle for brand placement, but academic research on comedy

22 movies specifically is limited, if nonexistent. However, parallels of a comedy's effectiveness can be made using advertising research done on humor.

Humor Advertising Research

"Humor is big business," remarks Marney (1995, p. 38), a media consultant in Toronto, Canada. Marney cited a 1992 study that found a definite increase in the use of humor in advertising, representing an investment of billions of dollars each year in campaigns. The study also reported that humor was used in over 30% of radio commercials, 24% of television commercials and 10% of magazine advertisements (Marney,

1995).

Advertising agencies, and many practitioners believe that humor possesses unique communicative powers. So much in fact that a study conducted among advertising research and creative executives in the top 1 50 U.S. advertising agencies found that 94% of the practitioners see humor as an effective way to attract attention

(Weinberger, Spotts, Campbell, & Parsons, 1995). A majority of the executives (88%) felt that humor worked best when it was related to a product. This survey also found that these executives felt radio (88%)

23 and television (84%) were best suited to the use of humor, followed by

outdoor (40%), magazines (39%), newspapers (29%), and direct mail

(22%) (Weinberger, Spotts, Campbell, & Parsons, 1995). The popularity

of using humor is quite certain; however, the success of its effect on

advertising is still uncertain. Researchers Spotts, Weinberger, and Parsons

(1997) summarized their reasons why understanding humor is so difficult.

Because of the many influences from the humorous message, the nature of the product, audience factors, communication goals, humor relatedness, humor style and humor placement, generalizations about the effects of humor have been rare. (p. 17)

The fact is humor is a complex topic that has found support for and

against its effectiveness as a selling tool. It is a diverse concept that is

affected by a wide variety of factors and finding a consistent result is

difficult. A humor research review, as it applies to product placement and

advertising research goals, necessarily examines the effects of humor in

its ability to attract attention, be persuasive, increase liking to the brand

and/or achieve greater success with a specific medium or placement.

Humor and Attention

There is no doubt that humor has been found to attract attention.

As mentioned earlier, 94% of advertising practitioners believe humor has

24 a positive effect on attention and research has supported this conclusion

(Madden & Weinberger, 1982; Speck, 1987; Stewart & Furse, 1986;

Weinberger & Campbell, 1991). In Stewart and Purse's (1986), data- based study of 1059 pre-tested ads for 356 brands, 11 5 product categories, and 63 firms, humor was found to be effective in increasing attention. In 1987, Speck used 182 undergraduates from Texas Tech

University and compared humorous ads with non-humorous controls. He used four attention measures: initial attention, sustained attention, projected attention, and overall attention. He found that humorous ads outperformed non-humorous ads on each of the attention measures. And finally, in another data-based study using magazine ads. Madden and

Weinberger (1982) found that there is a positive humor-attention relationship. The study used Starch's aided recall technique to establish scores indicating the amount of attention readers remember giving to a particular ad. The researchers compared the scores from each of the advertisements to whether or not the humorous advertisement was effective in capturing and holding the reader's attention. The study found a total of 148 recall scores of humorous ads and in each recall category the humorous ads outperformed the normal, non-humorous ads.

25 Most studies within the past 25 years have found that humor does affect attention positively (Weinberger & Gulas, 1992). A few studies like

Gruner (1970), Duncan (1979), and Madden (1982) found mixed results.

For instance, Gruner (1970) found that humor was not effective in gaining attention when the topic in a public speech was not interesting

(Weinberger & Gulas, 1992). Unlike the overwhelming support for humor's ability to gain attention the research on humor to persuade has had mixed results.

Humor and Persuasion

Research to enhance understanding of humor's ability to persuade has had mixed results. Several studies have found strong support for humor's ability to persuade (Chattopadhyay & Basu, 1989; Madden,

1982; McCollum & Speilman, 1982; and Scott, Klein, & Bryant, 1990).

For instance, in McCollum and Spieiman's (1982) study, 31% of humorous commercials exhibited above average scores on persuasiveness. In Chattopadhyay and Basu's (1989) study, participants with a prior positive brand attitude were persuaded more by humorous ads than participants with pre-existing negative brand attitudes. And lastly, Scott, Klein, and Bryant (1990) in a series of field studies

26 measured humor's effectiveness to promote a social and/or business event. This study also sought to provide insight on humor's ability to affect behavior. In each of the three field site studies, a social and business event was promoted, using humorous, non-humorous, and control fliers that were placed unfolded into residential mailboxes. The study found that attendance results were greater in all field sites of the social event by people who received a humorous promotional flier than those who received a non-humorous promotion. However, differences in frequency of attendance were not significant across promotion types at any of the business events (Scott, Klein, & Bryant, 1990).

This mixed result in Scott, Klein, and Bryant's (1990) study is also shared by a majority of the research on humor's effectiveness to persuade. In fact, Weinberger and Gulas (1992) concluded that of the 14 studies on the effects of humor on persuasion, five were positive, eight were neutral or mixed, and one (Bryant, Brown, Silberberg, & Elliott,

1981) was negative; therefore a precise conclusion could not be made.

Unlike the mixed results for humor's ability to persuade, research on humor's ability to increase ad and product liking, was overwhelmingly positive.

27 Humor and Liking

As early as 1967, Gruner measured research on humor's ability to increase liking. Gruner's experiment of 128 male undergraduates measured whether a public speaker that incorporates humor could damage their perceived authohtativeness. Although, the humorous speech failed to produce greater or less retention of information, including a no higher rating in interestingness than a serious speech, it did find that teachers who used humor in the classroom rated significantly higher on character scales. In another study, Bryant and Zillmann (1980) found that the use of humor by teachers positively influenced student attitudes toward educational programs.

In regards to advertising and marketing research, humor has been shown to increase both liking of the ad and liking of the brand. With regards to the goals of product placement particularly in increasing liking of the brand, various studies (Duncan & Nelson, 1985; Gelb & Zinkhan,

1986; Zhang & Zinkhan, 1991) have shown that humor increases brand liking.

In one study, Zhang and Zinkhan (1991) used 216 undergraduate business majors from the University of Houston to test whether

28 humorous ads increase ad information recall and positively affected brand attitude. Their study also sought to determine whether audience size influenced the degree of perceived humor. The three variables manipulated in their study were as follows: the humor in the ads, the number of ad exposures, and the size of the audience exposed to the ads.

In their experiment, half of the participants were exposed to a humorous

ad and the remaining half were exposed to a non-humorous ad. As part of

the guise, the participants were asked to indicate their musical preference

after they watched a series of videos. Embedded within these

videos were humorous or non-humorous ads. After watching the videos,

the students were then asked to complete a questionnaire that measured

brand attitude, perceived humor, and recall. Brand attitude and perceived

humor was measured using a six-point Likert-type scale. Recall was

measured by calculating the sum of correct answers to questions about

information contained in the ad. The study's findings indicated that humor

does have a positive effect on brand attitude and recall. The study also

found that social (audience size) positively influences perceived

humor. Zhang and Zinkhan's findings suggest that humorous ads tend to

29 produce higher levels of perceived humor, positive brand attitude, and brand information recall.

Duncan and Nelson's (1985) study, sought to determine whether humor affected attention, increased buying intention as well as attitude towards the ad and the product. A sample of 1 57 male undergraduate students participated in the radio programming experiment. For 75% of the participants, the transmitted program contained a humorous 60- second radio commercial for a new men's hair care product. The remaining 25% listened to a serious version of the commercial. After the participants listened to the programs, they completed a two-part survey.

The first-half of the survey measured the incidence of counter-arguing- behavior associated with the commercial. The second-half measured perceived humor of the ad and dependant variables such as: attention to the ad; liking the ad; positive beliefs about the product; liking the product; irritation (effects of a failed attempt at humor) instigated by the ad; distractions (effects of humor to disrupt counter argumentation of the discrepant message) solicited by the ad; intention to buy; and recall.

The researchers found that humor is a critical factor in influencing radio advertising effectiveness especially when the objective of the ad is to

30 generate awareness of the product. According to the study's findings,

participants attended to and appreciated the advertisement more and were less likely to be irritated by the ad if it was humorous. However, the

researchers found no differences for recall based on the presence or

absence of humor.

Gelb and Zinkman's (1986) study tested the relationship between

humor and brand attitude and/or between humor and purchase intention.

In their study 96 participants listened to six newscast excerpts and

completed a questionnaire after each one. Every newscast contained one

commercial of each product (fictional brands—Van Dean's Cola and

Coronado Corn Chips) at the beginning and at the end. Their study found

that humor was positively related to brand attitude but failed to show any

significant relationship between humor and purchase probability and/or

between humor and choice behavior. Their study suggested that

advertisers tracking the effects of humorous advertising are well advised

to monitor changes in brand attitude rather than changes in purchase

behavior.

In the 12 advertising studies conducted between 1970 and 1991,

10 studies reported a positive effect of humor on liking, while only two

31 studies reported neutral or mixed findings (Weinberger & Gulas, 1992).

Based on these conclusions it can be inferred that humor positively

effects liking and brand attitude.

Humor and Placement

As mentioned earlier, in the survey conducted by Madden and

Weinberger (1984) advertising executives felt that radio (88%) and

television (84%) were best suited to the use of humor. These types of

placement or medium by which an ad appears has been proven to some

degree in research to be true. For instance, in Weinberger and Spotts

(1992) study of U.S. ads and their humorous intent in each medium,

30.6% was in radio, 24.4% in television, and 9.9% in print ads. As

research would indicate, the medium most favored and employed for the

use of humor is broadcast-television or radio. However, there is no

comparative study to determine whether humor in broadcast media is

more effective than humor used in print to cement this belief.

Overall, humor has been found to be effective in brand or product

recall (Zhang & Zinkhan, 1991). Humor has been found to generate

awareness for a product (Duncan & Nelson, 1985). And humor, like

product placement, has been found to call attention to and increase liking

32 of a brand (Gelb & Zinkman's, 1986). However, it is important to note that even though humor was found to be quite effective in brand recall, awareness, attention, and likeability, there has not been research done in which a brand (or product placement) incurred the same successful results when placed in a humorous movie. Research is needed to determine whether humor through a comedy movie directly affects the recall, awareness, attention and or likeability of a brand placed within the humorous movie.

Recap

From the start movies were shown to strongly influence, affect, and motivate society. With this power came enormous promotional possibilities. Advertisers and marketers utilize the motion picture as a mass communication medium by placing brands within the plots of movies in return for money and/or exposure. The effectiveness of product placement through its ability to promote recall, increase brand awareness and brand likeability garnered an unprecedented ability to deliver millions of dollars of publicity for brands placed within a successful movie. Now as this promotional technique continues to grow and product placements are used in many other types of entertainment vehicles other than film and

33 television, the key is finding a vehicle to deliver the most successful and effective exposure through product placement.

Advertising and humor research suggest that the comedy genre may be the best genre/vehicle for a successful brand placement.

Research has shown that the comedy genre has the ability to draw large audiences and garner a profitable share of the market. And, by its very nature, the comedy genre carries positive emotional experiences, which work with the goal of placement companies, which is to find a scene or event in a movie that will connect a positive emotional experience to the brand. Research conducted on the effects of humor on advertising suggests the comedy genre may be the most fertile ground for product placement, but no evidence exists that can verify the assumption that comedy outranks other genres. Ultimately, research is needed to determine the best vehicles for eliciting positive reactions to product

placement.

Hypotheses and Research Question

With the marketability of the comedy genre and the effectiveness

of humorous advertising on increasing brand attitude and recall

established, the question becomes whether or not movie genre affects

34 the efficacy of brand placement in movies. The success of some brands reporting phenomenal sales increase (such as Reese's Pieces) may be

partly dependent on the genre of movie in which the brand is placed.

Based on the research indicating the value of humor in advertising, this

study examines the comedy movie genre specifically and attempts to

determine whether it promotes a higher recognition or stronger influence

of brand placement as compared to the drama and science fiction genres.

Because research seems to suggest that the comedy genre will serve as a

better vehicle for product placement than either the drama genre or the

science fiction genre, the following hypothesis will guide the research:

HI: Brand recall and/or recognition in the comedy genre will be

greater than brand recall and /or recognition in the drama or

science fiction genres.

With regards to the goals of product placement, particularly in

increasing liking of the brand, studies have shown that humor increases

consumers' affinity toward brands (Zhang & Zinkhan, 1991; Gelb &

Zinkhan, 1986; Duncan & Nelson, 1985). Research seems to suggest that

promotion through product placement in a comedy movie will increase

35 liking of the brand. As such, the following hypothesis will guide the research:

H2: Product placement within the comedy genre will favorably

increase viewers' perceptions of the brand.

In a study done by Mackenzie and Lutz (1989), attitudes toward advertising in general are thought to influence both attitude toward the ad and attitude toward the brand. Because product placement has advertising properties, it is likely that consumers who have a more favorable attitude toward product placement in general will have a more favorable attitude toward the product being promoted. This study also examines whether movie genre has an impact on consumers' reactions toward brand placement. For instance, a placement within the context of a drama may lead to negative reactions because viewers expect this genre to contain heavy emotional information, whereas brand placement in the context of a comedy movie may lead to positive reactions because viewers expect this type of genre to contain lighter fare. Zillmann's

(1994) management theory states that humor and comedy appear to play a special role for people who seek to change their mood for the better (i.e., terminate bad moods, change to good moods, or facilitate

36 and extend good moods). It could then be posited that because comedies tend to elicit good moods, that moviegoers would react more positively to products/brands placed within the comedy genre. The following

hypothesis guided the research:

H3: A comedy movie will lead to a more positive viewer reaction

toward brand placement in general than a drama or science

fiction movie.

Gupta and Lord's (1998) study revealed the impact of placement

type on brand awareness. They found that prominent brand placements

promote a significantly higher level of brand awareness than subtle brand

placements. The study by Brennan and Dubas (1999), found that audio

cues were more effective than were subtle visuals without audio but that

prominent visuals had the most effective means of instilling brand recall

among viewers. Their finding suggests that the greater use of sensory

cues may increase effectiveness of brand recall. With research supporting

the effectiveness of prominent placement and the use of humor in

increasing recall and/or recognition, the following research question will

guide the research:

37 RQ1: Does prominence of brand placement influence brand recal

and/or recognition independent of movie genre?

38 CHAPTER III

METHODOLOGY

Overview

Undergraduate research participants watched and evaluated movie clips from feature films released to video and DVD. These movie clips were categorized by genre—comedy, drama and science fiction. Each genre contained two movie clips. Each of these clips contained product placement. Hypotheses and research question were tested using a 20- item questionnaire. The questionnaire consisted primarily of Likert-type items using a five-point response scale, which allowed for the expression of an opinion toward the movie clip, genre classification, product recall, and attitude toward the use of brand placement in each movie.

Additionally the instrument measured demographic characteristics—age, gender, and academic classification

Research Participants

Two hundred students from a large southwestern university were recruited for the experiment. These participants were students of mass communication, history, English, biology, and mathematics. Students of

39 different classifications and majors provided a cross section of students for the study. The students received extra credit as an incentive for participating. College students served as a relatively representative sample because approximately one-third of all moviegoers are between the ages of 1 6 and 24 (Monush, 1991). Of the 200 participants, 1 29 were female, and 71 were male. The average age of the participants was

21. The typical classification was junior.

The 200 students were divided into three mixed-gender experimental groups-comedy, drama, and science fiction. Each genre group contained a minimum of 60 participants, with participants divided

between the two clips employed to represent each genre. For example in the comedy genre, 32 participants watched Me, Myself, and Irene whWe

another 34 watched There's Something About Mary. For drama, 34

participants watched The Usual Suspects while 34 watched Monster's Ball.

Finally, groups of 33 participants watched The Philadelphia Experiment or

Mission to Mars for the science fiction genre.

Procedure

When participants in each experimental group entered the research

room, the study administrator read a consent form, which the participants

40 were then asked to sign. The administrator then briefed the participants on the experiment and how the experiment would be conducted. To disguise the purpose of the study, participants were told the experiment dealt with common movie production practices as well as mass communication. After the participants viewed the movie clip, they were asked to complete the questionnaire. Upon completing the questionnaire and its collection, students were thanked for participating and dismissed.

Measures

Within the 20-item questionnaire, product placement was defined as, "the practice of placing consumer brand products or services by a placement agency or company in a motion picture," This was done so that participants had a precise idea as to the operational definition of the term and so that any confusion could be avoided.

The questionnaire was designed first to gauge the participants recall ability of the product placed in the movie clip. The questionnaire asked whether participants saw any product placement in the movie clip.

If the participants answered "yes" they were then asked to write the name of the product they saw (i,e., unaided recall). These questions were chosen using previous research as a guideline (Vollmers & Mizerski, 1994)

41 and used to test the first hypothesis—brand recall and/or recognition in a comedy movie will be higher than brand recall and/or recognition in a drama or science fiction movie, A sample of the questionnaire is provided in the appendix.

The second step was to assess the research participants' attitude toward product placement. Audience acceptability of product placement

may be in part tied to general ethical concerns about the practice and/or

ethical concerns about the specific product (Nebenzahl & Secunda,

1993). As stated earlier, researchers found that consumers felt that

product placement was acceptable, but those who objected tended to do

so for ethical reasons. The questions used in this study, similar to those

asked by Nebenzahl and Secunda (1993), asked whether the participants

minded if brand-name products appeared in movies and whether seeing

the brand-name product in a movie makes the experience more realistic

(Gupta, Gould, & Grabner-Krauter, 2000), Attitudinal questions on a five-

point scale with poles of strongly agree (1) and strongly disagree (5)

regarding the ethical use of product placements by movie producers were

also asked based on previous research done by Gupta and Gould (1997),

For instance, participants were asked whether they minded if movie

42 producers received money or other compensation from advertisers for placing their brands in movies. They were also asked whether they thought it was unethical for movie producers to attempt to influence the audience by including brand name products in movies. These questions were used to test the third hypothesis—a comedy movie will lead to a more positive viewer reaction toward brand placement in general than a drama or science fiction movie.

The third step in the design of the questionnaire was to gauge the research participant's recognition of the actual product placed in the move clip. From a list of 20 popular products, participants were asked to circle the one product they saw in the movie clip. This question followed the study done by Babin and Carder (1995), in which they exposed a treatment group to a film and measured recall of brands by giving respondents product category cues (i,e, aided recall). This question was used to again test the first hypothesis.

In the final phase of the questionnaire, research participants were asked attitudinal questions about the product they saw in the movie clip.

The questions were employed to gauge the effects of humor on brand liking as stated by Duncan and Nelson (1985), Similar to the attitudinal

43 questions posed in their study to indicate product opinion, these questions asked if participants first liked the product; whether the appearance of the product in the movie was great and/or realistic; and finally if the product's appearance in the movie increased their liking of the product itself. These questions were used to support the second hypothesis—a comedy movie will favorably increase the viewer's perception of the product placed in the movie.

Stimulus Material

There are many potential limitations to the generalization of a product placement study's findings. First, product placement's impact is likely to vary from movie to movie. The product placement's impact may be in part due to its manner and extent of an audience's exposure to the brand and/or audience familiarity to the brand itself; liking for the movie; a specific frame or scene in which the brand appeared; the timing of the placement (i.e., towards the beginning or end of movie); and or the setting in which an audience watched the movie (i.e., theater viewing versus home viewing on cable, television, or video).

In order to maintain some level of ecological validity, this study used as stimulus material previously released film clips. The stimulus

44 material or films used for this experiment could not be independently

created professionally due to monetary costs, time, and lack of expertise.

However, every product placement experimental research study to date

has employed actual movie clips or full-length movies. Therefore, an

intensive search of actual, released films in each gene was made with

several goals in mind for this experiment.

First, the films chosen did not have any promotional tie-ins or

announced sponsorships during the time of their release. Any previous

memorable marketing of the product or tie-in would have deliberately

drawn participants' attention to the brand placed in the film (Ong & Meri,

1994).

Second, the film needed to show a national brand, which is widely

distributed and advertised, and intent ally placed in the movie. The brand

placements in the movie clips were predominantly beverages. Product

categories such as beer, liquor, and soft drinks were expected to

represent familiar consumption experiences among students.

Third, due to constraints on time and the availability of the students, the movies could not be seen in their entirety. Films were selected so that brand placements were contained within an

45 uninterrupted segment of the movie (midpoint of the clip) that was comprehensive on its own. The goal was to have research participants engaged in some part of the segment's story line. The time before and after the brand placement served as a buffer. This was done to reduce primacy and recency effects. According to Wixted and McDowell (1989), when an individual is confronted with numerous stimuli in a short amount of time, the first and last items viewed will have the greatest memory trace.

It is also important to note that the films chosen were categorized by genre by the website, http://internetmoviedatabase.com. Because there are many types of comedy, drama, and/or science fiction movies, two films were used to represent each of the genres in the experiment.

This was done to alleviate any movie-specific effects regarding any

individual film labeled to represent a specific genre.

Also, each movie clip within a genre contained a different type of

placement. One movie contained a prominent placement where the

product was voiced, seen and used by the character. The other movie

contained a subtle placement where the product was only seen in the

background. According to Gupta and Lord (1998), prominent brand

46 placements—placements that are highly visible due to their size,

positioning, or centrality to the action in the scene—promote a significantly higher level of brand awareness than subtle brand

placements—placements which serve as background props. Every effort

was taken to ensure consistent placement patterns across genres.

Ultimately, no one genre was represented with more prominent placement

episodes than another. All shared seemingly consistent levels of

placement.

Finally, the total exposure time for the products in each genre

needed to be consistent. Total exposure time was calculated from the

total amount of time in each movie clip that the product was used, voiced

or visible. For instance, the exposure time for the product in Me, Myself,

and Irene was 1 5 seconds and 19 seconds in There's Something About

Mary. The product was exposed for 1 5 seconds in The Usual Suspects and

18 seconds in Monster's Ball. In the science fiction genre, the exposure

time was 18 seconds in The Philadelphia Experiment and 16 seconds in

Mission to Mars. The films used for this study, their classifications and

brand placements are outlined in Table 1.

47 Measurement

Data was analyzed using Analysis of Variance comparisons

(ANOVA) for each dependant variable. Frequencies were run in order to determine the percent of research participants who correctly identified the branded product they saw. Descriptive statistics were calculated to provide an overview of results. In cases where both independent variables and dependent variables were nominal, cross tabulations analyses were employed.

48 Table 1 Movie, Classification and Brand Placement List

Genre Movie Brand Placement & Time Comedy There's Something About Mary Pepsi (19 sec.)

Comedy Me, Myself, and Irene Coca Cola (15 sec.)

Drama The Usual Suspects Budweiser (15 sec.)

Drama Monster's Ball Jack Daniel's (18 sec.)

Science Fiction Mission to Mars Dr Pepper (16 sec.)

Science Fiction The Philadelphia Experlrjient Coca Cola (18 sec.)

49 CHAPTER IV

RESULTS

Preliminary Analysis

Although the films chosen were categorized into genres by the website lnternetMovieDatabase,com, this experiment used a 5-point response scale in which 1 equaled strongly agree and 5 equaled strongly

disagree to test whether participants percieved the movie clips differently

than the website's categorization. An ANOVA revealed that participants

percieved the comedy clips (M= 1,33, SD = ,75) used in the experiment

to be more representative of the comedy genre than those who saw the

drama clips (M = 4,54, SD = ,72) or science fiction clips (M = 4,70, SD =

.70), F(2, 197) = 455,29, p < .05 as suggested by Student-Neuman

Keuls post hoc tests.

An ANOVA also revealed that participants who watched the drama

clips (M = 1.66, SD = .13) percieved these clips to be more

representative of the drama genre than those who saw the comedy clips

(M = 4.11, SD = .1 3) or science fiction clips (M = 2.71, SD = .13), F(2,

197) = 93.90, p < ,05 as suggested by Student-Neuman Keuls post hoc

tests,

50 Finally, participants percieved the science fiction clips (M= 2,21,

SD = 1.06) to be more representative of the science fiction genre than

those who saw the comedy (M = 4.70, SD = .76) or drama clips (M =

4.66, SD = .11), F(2, 197) = 1 76,53, p < ,05 as suggested by Student-

Neuman Keuls post hoc tests.

Mean results for demographic questions are illustrated in Table 2,

Brand Recall and Recognition Hypothesis

HI, which stated brand recall and/or recognition in a comedy genre

would be higher than brand recall and/or recognition in a drama or science

fiction genres, was not supported. When participants were asked if

product placement was used, participants viewing different genres

responded differently, x^(2, N= 200) = 67,74, p < ,05, A cross tabs

analysis revealed that 63.6% of the participants watching comedy clips

said they saw product placement while only 35,3% of the respondents

watching drama clips said the same. However, 100% of the participants watching science fiction clips reported seeing product placement.

In order to test unaided recall, participants were asked to actually write in the name of the product placed in the movie clip. Again participants viewing different genres responded differently, x^(2, N =

51 200) = 86,02, p < ,05, A cross tabs analysis revealed that 45,5% of the participants watching comedy clips correctly wrote down the name of the product placed in the clips while only 1 6,2% of the respondents watching drama clips did the same. However, 95,5% of the participants watching science fiction clips correctly supplied the name of the product placed in the movies.

When testing for recognition, participants were asked to circle the one product they saw in the movie clip from a list of 20 popular products, participants viewing different genres responded with varying levels of success, x^(2, N= 200) = 86,02, p < .05. A cross tabs analysis revealed that 57.6% of the participants watching comedy clips correctly circled the name of the product placed in the clip while a higher 63.2% of the respondents watching drama clips did the same. However, 98.5% of the participants watching science fiction clips correctly circled the name of the product.

Mean results for recall and recognition are illustrated in Table 3,

Attitude Toward the Brand Hypothesis

H2, which stated product placement within the comedy genre will favorably increase viewers' perceptions of the brand, was not supported,

52 H2 was tested using five questions. These five questions targeted participants' attitude toward the brand, its use as a brand placement, and if its appearance was great, realistic and/or influencial in improving their liking of the product. Each question used a Likert-based scale in which (1) equaled strongly agree and (5) equaled strongly disagree, A conglomerate score of these five questions was created to provide one

overall conclusive score of the results. An ANOVA revealed there was no

significant difference between genres. Although no statistical significant

difference emerged, the mean score of the science fiction (M= 2,55, SD

= ,63) genre was more favorable than the scores of comedy (M= 2,69,

SD = .62) and the drama (M = 2.71, SD = .58) genres, F(2, 197) = 1.53,

p=.22.

Examining the questions individually that composed the

conglomerate score, the first question asked participants if they minded if

featured product was a product placement in the movie. An ANOVA

revealed there was no significant difference among genres. Although no

statistical significant difference emerged, the mean score of the science

fiction (M = 1.62, SD = 1.05) genre was more favorable than those of

53 comedy (M = 1.70, SD = 1.01) and the drama (M = 1.71, SD = 1.01) genres, F(2, 197) = .138, p = .87.

Through the second question, participants were asked if they

agreed that the appearance of the featured product in the movie was

great. An ANOVA revealed that participants viewing the science fiction

genre (M = 2.53, SD = .98) agreed more than did participants watching

the drama genre (M = 2.94, SD = .93). The comedy genre (M = 2.82, SD

= .80) was in the middle and not significantly different from either of the

others, F(2, 197) = 3.61, p < .05. A post hoc test using Student-Neuman

Keuls revealed that perceptions of comedy mimicked those of science

fiction and drama, but that science fiction and drama were significantly

different from each other.

Through the third question, participants were asked if they agreed

that the appearance of the featured product in the movie was realistic.

An ANOVA revealed there was no significant difference among genres.

Mean scores suggested that the drama (M = 1.97, SD = ,85) genre was viewed more favorably, but not significantly so, than the comedy (M =

2.06, SD = ,98) and the science fiction (M = 2,29, SD = 1.13) genres,

F(2, 197) = 1,82, p=,17,

54 Through the fourth question, participants were asked if they agreed that the featured product was a great product. An ANOVA revealed that participants viewing the science fiction genre (M = 2,05, SD= 1,13) agreed more than did participants watching the drama (M = 2,62, SD =

1,25), The comedy genre (M = 2,55, SD = 1,24) was in the middle and not significantly different from either of the others, F(2, 197) = 4,43, p <

.05, A post hoc test using Student-Neuman Keuls revealed that

perceptions of comedy mimicked those of drama, but that science fiction

was viewed significantly more positvely.

Finally, through the fifth question, participants were asked if they

agreed that the appearance of the featured product in the movie

increased their liking of that product. An ANOVA revealed there was no

significant difference among genres. Mean scores suggested that the

science fiction (M = 4,24, SD = 1,90) genre was more favorable but not

significantly more favorable than the comedy (M = 4,35, SD = ,90) and

the drama (M = 4.34, SD = .87) genres, F(2, 197) = .286, p = ,75,

Mean results for attitude towards the product are illustrated in

Table 4.

55 Attitude Toward Product Placement Hypothesis

H3, which stated a comedy movie will lead to a more positive viewer reaction toward brand placement in general than a drama and science fiction movie, was not supported,

H3 was tested using four questions. These four questions targeted participant's attitude toward product placement itself, whether movie producers should receive compensation, its ethical use in movies, and/or product placement's influence in making the movie experience realistic.

Each question used a Likert based scale in which one equaled strongly agree and five equaled strongly disagree. Once again, a conglomerate score of these four questions was created to provide one overall conclusive score of the results. An ANOVA revealed the drama genre (M =

3,38, SD = ,68) to be more favorable than the comedy (M = 3,68, SD =

.61) and the science fiction (M = 3.77, SD = ,57) genres, F(2, 197) =

7,20, p < ,05, A post hoc test using Student-Neuman Keuls revealed that perceptions of comedy mimicked those of science fiction, but drama was viewed significantly more positively.

The first question asked participants if they minded if brand-name products appear in movies. An ANOVA revealed that participants viewing

56 the drama clips (M = 3,97, SD = 1,02) minded less than did participants watching the science fiction genres (M = 4,39, SD = .78), The comedy

genre (M = 4,27, SD = ,97) was in the middle and not significantly

different than either of the others, F(2, 197) = 3,69, p < ,05. A post hoc

test using Student-Neuman Keuls revealed that perceptions of comedy

mimicked those of science fiction and drama, but science fiction and

drama were perceived in a significantly different manner in comparison.

In the second question, participants were asked if they minded if

movie producers recieved money or other compensation from advertisers

for placing their brands in movies. An ANOVA revealed there was no

significant difference among genres. Mean scores suggested that the

drama (M = 3.78, SD= 1.13) genre was more favorable, but not

significantly more so, than the comedy (M = 4.06, SD = 1.05) and the

science fiction (M = 4.20, SD = 1,00) genres, F(2, 197) = 2,70, p = ,07,

In the third question, participants were asked if they agreed that it

is unethical for movie producers to attempt to influence the audience by

including brand-name products in their movies. An ANOVA revealed that

participants viewing the drama clips (M = 3.59, SD= 1.16) agreed less

than did participants watching the science fiction genre (M= 4.23, SD =

57 ,94). The comedy genre (M = 4.1 5, SD = .90) was in the middle and not significatly different than either of the others, F(2, 197) = 8.04, p < .05.

A post hoc test using Student-Neuman Keuls revealed that perceptions of

comedy mimicked those of science fiction, but drama was perceived

significantly more positive.

In the last question, participants were asked if they agreed that

seeing the brand-name product in a movie makes the experience more

realistic. An ANOVA revealed there was no significant difference among

genres. Mean scores suggested that the drama genre (M = 2.18, SD =

1.05) was more favorable, but not significantly more so, than the comedy

(M = 2.24, SD = 1.12) and the science fiction (M = 2.24, SD = 1.10)

genres, F(2, 197) = .082, p = .92.

Mean results for attitude towards product placement are illustrated

in Table 5.

Research Question One

The research question asked if the prominence of brand placement

influenced brand recall/recognition independent of movie genre. The

research question was tested using three questions. These three

questions tested whether participants saw a product placement, could

58 correctly write down the name of the product (unaided), and if they could successfully circle the name of the product from a list of brand names.

Movies were divided into placement types—prominent versus subtle. The

movies containing prominent placements were, Me, Myself, & Irene,

Monster's Ball, and The Philadelphia Experiment. Movies containing subtle

placements were. There's Something About Mary, The Usual Suspects,

and Mission to Mars.

In the first question participants were asked if product placement

was used, participants viewing different placement types responded

differently, x^l, N= 200) = 21.86, p < .05. A cross tabs analysis

revealed that 81.8% of the participants recalled a product placement in

the prominent placement clips while 18.2% did not. When participants

were asked if there was a product placement in subtle placement clips,

50.5% participants recalled a product while 49.5% did not.

The second question, which tested unaided recall, asked

participants to actually write in the name of the product placed in the

movie clip. Again, participants viewing different placement types

responded differently, xKh N = 200) = 27.49, p < ,05, A cross tabs

analysis revealed that 70,7% of the participants watching prominent

59 placement clips correctly wrote down the name of the product while only

29.3% of the participants did not. In subtle placement clips a cross tabs analysis revealed that 33.7% of the participants correctly wrote down the name of the product while 66.3% did not.

Through the final question, which tested for recognition,

participants were asked to circle the one product they saw in the movie

clip from a list of 20 popular products, participants viewing different

placement types responded differently, x^(l, N= 200) = 55.44, p < .05,

A cross tabs analysis revealed that 97,0% of the participants watching

prominent placement clips correctly circled the name of the product while

only 3.0% of the participants did not. In subtle placement clips, a cross

tabs analysis revealed that 50.5% of the participants correctly circle the

name of the product while 49.5% did not.

60 Table 2 Mean Results for Demographic Questions

Comedy Drama Science Fiction Clipl Clip 2 Clip 3 Clip 4 Clips Clip 6 Questions Me Myself Something The Usual Monster's Philadelphia Mission to & Irene About Mary Suspects Ball Experiment Mars Number of Subjects 32 34 34 34 33 33 (Total = 200) Total 66 68 66

Age 21.19 20.62 21.06 20.35 20.21 21.48 (1.51) (1.26) (1.82) (1.37) (1.67) (2.21) Total 20.89(1.40) 20.71 (1.64) 20.85 (2.05)

Gender Male = 16(50%) 12(35.3%) 13(38.2%) 9 (26.5%) 10(30.3%) 11(33.3%) Female = \6{507c) 22(64.7%) 21(61.8%) 25(73.5%) 23(69.7%) 22(66.7%) Total M = 28 (42.4%) M = 22 (32.4%) M = 21 (31.8%) F = 38 (57.6%) F =46(67.6%) F = 45 (68.2%)

Classification* 3.12 (.91) 2.96 (.78) 3.15 (.86) 2.65 (.69) 2.52 (.76) 3.33 (.74) Total 2.94 (.86) 2.90 (.81) 2.92 (.85) 1. Have You Seen Movie Before® Yes = 24(75%) 33(97.1%) 12(35.3%) 10(29.4%) 1 (3%) 4(12.1%) No = 8 (25%) 1 (2.9%) 22(64.7%) 24(70.6%) 32 (97%) 29(87.9%) Total Y = 57 (86.4%) Y = 22 (32.4%) Y = 5 (7.6%) N = 9(13.6%) N = 46 (67.6%) N = 61 (92.4%)

2. This Clip Was A 1.41 1.26 4.53 4.56 4.64 4.76 Good Example Of A (.95) (.51) (.79) (.66) (.82) (.56) Comedy® Total 1.33 (.75) 4.54 (.72) 4.70 (.70)

3. This Clip Was A 4.16 4.06 1.88 1.44 2.94 2.48 Good Example Of A (1.25) (1.15) (.81) (.75) (.90) (1.20) Drama® Total 4.11 (1.19) 1.66 (.80) 2.71 (1.08)

4. This Clip Was A 4.53 4.85 4.47 4.85 2.03 2.39 Good Example Of A (.98) (.44) (.93) (.50) (.92) (1.17) Science Fiction* Total 4.70 (.76) 4.66 (.77) 2.21 (1.06) Note. Values enclosed in parentheses represent standard deviations or percentages of the whole. O = Mean values are on a four-point scale (1 = freshman, 2 = sophomore, 3 = junior, 4 = senior). ® = Values are on a two-point scale (1 = yes, 0 = no). ® = Mean values are on a five-point scale (1 = strongly agree, 5 = strongly disagree).

61 Table 3 Mean Results for Recall and Recognition

Comedy Drama Science Fiction Questions to test Clip 1 Clip 2 Clip 3 Clip 4 Clips Clip 6 Hypothesis 1 Me Myself Something The Usual Monster's Philadelphia Mission to & Irene About Mary Suspects Ball Experiment Mars l.Was There PP In The Clip You Saw Yes = 29(90.6%) 13(38.%) 5(14.7%) 19(55.9%) 33(100%) 33(100%) No = 3(9.4%) 21(61.8%) 29(85.3%) 15(44.1%) 0 0 Total Y= 42 (63.6%) Y = 24 (35.3%) Y = 66(100%) N = 24 (36.4%) N = 46 (64.7%) 2.Write In The PP You Saw In Clip Yes = 28(90.6%) 2 (38.%) 2(14.7%) 9(55.9%) 33(100%) 30(100%) No = 4(9.4%) 32(61.8%) 32(85.3%) 25(44.1%) 0 3 Total Y = 30 (45.5%) Y= 11 (16.2%) Y = 63 (95.5%) N = 36 (54.5%) N = 57 (83.8%) N = 3 (45%) 3. From List Of Products Select The PP You Saw In Clip Yes = 31(90.6%) 7 (38.%) 12(14.7%) 32(55.9%) 33(100%) 32(100%) 1(9.4%) No = 27(61.8%) 22(85.3%) 2(44.1%) 0 1 Total Y = 38 (57.6%) Y = 44 (64.7%) Y = 65 (98.5%) N = 28 (42.4%) N = 24 (35.3%) N= 1 (1.5%) Note. All values are on a two-point scale (1 = yes, 0 = no). Values enclosed in parentheses are percentages of the whole. PP = product placement

62 Table 4 Mean Results for Attitude Toward the Product

Comedy Drama Science Fiction Questions to test Clipl Clip 2 Clip 3 Clip 4 Clips Clip 6 Hypothesis 2 Me Myself Something The Usual Monster's Philadelphia Mission to & Irene About Mary Suspects Ball Experiment Mars

1. Do You Mind If 1.14 1.97 1.79 1.62 1.61 1.64 Said Product Was (.71) (1.17) (1.09) (.92) (1.14) (.96) In Movie Total 1.70(1.01) 1.71(1.01) 1.62(1.05)

2. Do You Think 2.56 3.06 2.97 2.91 2.39 2.67 The Appearance Of (.84) (.69) (.97) (.90) (.83) (1.11) Said Product Was Great In Movie Total 2.82 (.80) 2.94 (.93) 2.53 (.98)

3. Appearance Of 1.47 (.72) 2.62 2.21 1.74 2.55 2.03 Said Product Was (.85) (.91) (.71) (1.20) (1.02) Real In Movie Total 2.06 (.97) 1.97 (.85) 2.29(1.13)

4. Is Said Product 1.94 3.12 2.96 2.47 1.76 2.33 A Great Product (1.01) (1.17) (1.23) (1.26) (.94) (1.24) Total 2.55(1.24) 2.62(1.25) 2.05(1.13)

5. Did The 4.47 4.24 4.29 4.38 3.97 4.52 Appearance Of Said (.76) (1.02) (.97) (.78) (.95) (.76) Product Increase My Liking of Product Total 4.35 (.90) 4.34 (.87) 4.24 (.90) Note. All mean values are on a five-point scale (1 = strongly agree, 5 = strongly disagree). Values enclosed in parentheses represent standard deviations.

63 Table 5 Mean Results for Attitude Toward Product Placement

Comedy Drama Science Fiction Questions to test Clipl Clip 2 Clip 3 Clip 4 Clips Clip 6 Hypothesis 3 Me Myself Something The Usual Monster's Philadelphia Mission to & Irene About Mary Suspects Ball Experiment Mars

l.MindlfPP 4.50 4.06 4.06 3.88 4.45 4.33 Appear In Movies (.84) (1.06) (1.07) (.98) (.75) (.82) Total 4.27 (.97) 3.97(1.02) 4.39 (.78)

2. Mind If Producers 4.09 4.03 4.09 3.47 4.12 4.27 Get Compensation (1.06) (1.06) (.90) (1.26) (1.08) (.91) ForPP Total 4.06(1.05) 3.78(1.13) 4.20(1.00)

3. It is Unethical 4.25 4.06 3.65 3.53 4.18 4.27 To Influence (.84) (.95) (1.20) (1.13) (.98) (.91) Audience With PP Total 4.15 (.90) 3.59(1.16) 4.23 (.94)

4. Seeing PP Makes 2.19 2.29 2.21 2.15 2.30 2.18 Movie Experience (1.33) (.91) (1.07) (1.05) (1.16) (1.04) More Realistic Total 2.24(1.12) 2.18(1.05) 2.24(1.10) Note. All mean values are on a five-point scale (1 = strongly agree, 5 = strongly disagree). Values enclosed in parentheses represent standard deviations. PP = product placement.

64 CHAPTER V

DISCUSSION

This study was conducted to address whether the comedy movie genre favorably affects product placement, specifically in brand recall/recognition, attitude towards the brand, and attitude towards product placement itself. The results of this initial experiment suggest that product placement is not more effective in the comedy genre and that prominence of placement may be more important than genre of placement vehicle.

First Hvpothesis

In examining the first hypothesis, brand recall was high across two genres—the comedy and science fiction. Approximately 63% of respondents from the comedy genre indicated they saw a product placement, and 46% of the participants viewing that genre correctly wrote down the brand that appeared in the movie clip. The most significant recall occurred in the science fiction genre, where 100% of the participants reported seeing a product placement and 96% of those participants correctly wrote down the brand placed in the movie clip.

65 Data suggests that the science fiction genre has a more successful recall andXor recognition rate than either drama or comedy movies for product placement. These results also appear to go against Zhang and

Zinkhan's (1991) research that humor tends to produce higher levels of brand recall. More research is needed to fully determine whether this study's initial results are truly conclusive. A possible factor that may have influenced the high recall and recognition in the science fiction genre was its type of placement. In retrospect, the placements used in the science fiction movies were intertwined with the story's plot more so than the products appearing in the other two genres. Not only were the products an integral part of the story used by the characters, but also their placement was obviously noticeable in both movie clips. The placement in

Mission to Mars was judged in coding to be a subtle placement, however post-study examinations revealed the placement was blatant, in that it was highly visible and centered across the screen. The placement was stronger than the subtle placements shown in There's Something About

Mary and The Usual Suspects whose placements were as background props.

66 Second Hvpothesis

With regards to the goals of product placement particularly in increasing liking of the brand these studies by Zhang and Zinkhan (1991),

Gelb and Zinkhan (1986), and Duncan and Nelson (1985) found that humor was shown to increase brand liking. The use of humor through the comedy genre in this study did not support their findings or hypothesis two. In fact, the science fiction genre overall had a more favorable increase than either the drama or comedy movies.

As mentioned before, the second hypothesis was tested using five questions. The conglomerate score stated that the science fiction genre

provided the most favorable perceptions of a brand when used in product

placement.

In the first question used to build the conglomerate score,

participants were asked if they minded if the featured product was in the

movie clip they just watched. Unlike the study conducted by Gruner

(1967) in which teachers that used humor in the classroom rated

significantly higher on character scales than those who did not, the use of

humor in a comedy movie in this study did not increase brand liking. In

fact, this question revealed that there was no significant difference

67 among genres and that the use of humor was not influential. Participants were not influenced by the dramatic, the science fiction, or comedic content of the movie's genre to increase or decrease their liking for a

specific product. A possible reason could be that the product was shown

immediately after and during a comedic scene in only one (Me, Myself,

and Irene) of the comedy clips. In the other comedy clip. There's

Something About Mary the product placement did not occur immediately

before or after a comedic situation and/or involve the product itself in

the storyline.

In the second question, participants were asked if the appearance

of the featured product was great. Participants considered the product's

appearance good in all genres but the significant difference was between

the science fiction and drama genres. The results of this question

revealed that the use of humor in the comedy genre did not increase viewer's brand liking or participant's perception that the product was

great. Again, comedy genre did not outshine the others.

In the third question, participants were asked whether the appearance of the featured product was realistic. Once again, participants considered the product's appearance very realistic across all genres;

68 however, there was no significant difference between them. One possible

reason could be that the product was placed between highly emotional

scenes in the drama genre. The emotional scenes before the placement

included a tragic emergency in which a child character died, tensions of

racism, and acts of discrimination. Emotional scenes immediately after the

placement were of inebriated depression and sensuality. Participants may

have felt this heavy montage of realism stimulated their perception that

the product was just as realistic as the scenes in the movie clip. An

interesting side note was that even though participants viewing the

science fiction genres had the highest recall and/or recognition of the

product placement, the products (Coke and Dr Pepper) did not impact the

fictitious qualities of the science fiction genre. Participants still considered these popular and well-marketed sodas and the movie genre the least realistic in mean scores.

The fourth question asked participants if they thought the featured product was a great product. Once again, participants considered the product equally great in all genres but significant difference existed between the science fiction and drama genres. The results of this question revealed that the use of humor in the comedy genre did not

69 increase viewer's brand liking or participant's perception that the product was great. The results of this question also suggest that the science fiction genre appears to increase brand liking and/or the participant's

perception that the product was great. Prior testing of the participant's

attitude toward the brand would provide more definitive results. Again it

is believed that the increase in brand liking in the science fiction genre

may be due in part to the product's prominence and use in the movie.

Finally, in the fifth question, participants were asked if they agreed

that the appearance of the featured product in the movie increased their

liking of the product. Even though prior research suggests that humor

positively effects liking and brand attitude, data from this study revealed

that there was no significant difference among genres and that the use of

humor was not influential in increasing brand liking.

In summary, the results for the second hypothesis and its

favorablity of the brand in the science fiction genre may be due to several

factors. First the products placed in the science fiction genre were

intertwined with the story line. The products were blatantly used and

seen across the middle of the screen. The product (Pepsi) in the subtle

placement category of the comedy clip served only as a background prop

70 and was never cropped dead center of the screen. Please refer to figures

1 through 6 as they illustrate the type of placement. A second possible reason that drama scored so low was that an ethically-charged product such as alcohol may be better received in an optimistic movie than in a dramatic one (Kamins, Marks, & Skinner, 1991). For instance, some brands may be seen as more objectionable because of their being targeted to, or offensive to, certain groups (Gupta & Gould, 1997).

Third Hvpothesis

Much like the first studies that shed light on consumers' attitudes toward brand placements in films (Nebenzahl & Secunda, 1993; Ong &

Meri, 1994; Babin & Carder, 1996; Gupta & Gould, 1997) this study's third hypothesis tested whether a comedy genre favorably increased consumers' attitudes toward brand placement. As stated earlier,

Nebenzahl and Secunda (1993) concluded that overall consumers do not object to brand placement and that they view it as an effective marketing communication medium which should be allowed. This study supported their findings with participants across all genres expressing a positive attitudinal score toward brand placement; however, this study's conglomerate overall score revealed that the drama genre had a higher

71 positive viewer reaction toward brand placement in general than a science fiction and comedy movie.

The third hypothesis was tested using four questions, in addition to the conglomerate score. In the first question, participants were asked if they minded if brand placements appeared in the movie clip. Participants positively acknowledged the use of product placements in all genres, but there was significant difference between the science fiction and drama

genres. The use of humor in the comedy genre did not positively increase viewer's attitude toward brand placement above the other two genres.

The results of this question suggest that the science fiction genre

appears to increase viewer's attitude toward brand placement.

In the second question, participants were asked if they minded if

movie producers received money or other compensation from advertisers

for placing their brands in movies. Participants positively agreed across all

genres, however there was no significant difference between them. Again,

the use of humor did not positively increase a favorable attitude toward

brand placements above the other two genres.

In the third question, participants were asked whether they agreed

that it was unethical for movie producers to attempt to influence the

72 audience by including brand name products in their movies. Participants

considered the unethical influence attempted by movie producers with

product placement very minimal in all genres but the significant difference

was between the drama and science fiction genres. This question revealed

that the use of humor in the comedy genre did not increase viewers'

attitude toward product placement or that its use was unethical.

The final question asked participants if they thought that seeing

the brand-name product in a movie makes the experience more realistic.

Overall participants agreed that product placement made the movie

experience more realistic, but the only significant difference was between

the drama and science fiction genres. Results of this question did suggest

that viewers' perceptions were positive toward the practice of brand

placements and that viewers believed product placement made the movie

experience more realistic. Possible reasons could be that the realistic

images and scenes stimulated their perception that the product was just as realistic as the scenes in the movie clip.

In summary, the results for the third hypothesis and the overall favorable increase in viewer reaction toward product placement in a drama rather than a comedy may be due to a combination of factors.

73 Both Jack Daniels in Monster's Ball and Budweiser in The Usual Suspects were not intertwined in the storyline or centered across the screen as were the products in the science fiction genre. Placements in science

fiction were blatent—highly visible and centered across the screen.

Second, the drama genre had the lowest recall scores of all three genres.

A combination of these two factors may have effected the outcome. With the lack of extreme prominence and blatent placement as well as a low

recall score, participants may not have objected to something they did

not see or hardly recognized.

Research Ouestion One

The results of this study mimicked the conclusions made by Gupta and Lord (1998), in which prominent brand placements—placements that were highly visible due to their size, positioning, and/or centrality to the action in the scene—promoted a significantly higher level of brand awareness than subtle brand placements—placements which serve as background props. The study also supported Brennan and Dubas (1999), conclusions that audio cues were more effective than were subtle visuals without audio but that prominent visuals had the most effective means of instilling brand recall among viewers.

74 As stated earlier, each movie was divided into two placement type categories. One category contained a prominent placement where the product was voiced, seen and used by the character. The other category contained a subtle placement where the product was just seen in the background.

In the first question participants were asked if product placement was used in the movie clip they just viewed. A significant difference between placement types was revealed. In fact, most participants

(81.2%) in the prominent placement category recalled seeing a product placement, while only 50.5% of the subtle placement category recalled seeing a product. This seems to suggest that prominant placements are more effective in cueing movie goers to recall product placements than are subtle placements. A possible reason could be the added exposure of being voiced and used by the main characters.

In the second question, which tested unaided recall, participants were asked to actually write in the name of the product placed in the movie clip. Again a significant difference between placement types was revealed. An impressive 70.7% of the participants in the prominent placement category correctly wrote in the name of the product. Only

75 33.7% of the participants in the subtle placement category did the same.

Once again, this study seems to suggest that prominent placements can

effectively influence unaided recall more so than subtle placements. One

reason could be the proximity of the product to the celebrity in a movie

may have resulted in a higher amount of brand awareness and recall as

suggested in Eddington's (1991) study.

In the final question, which tested for recognition, participants were

asked to circle the one product they saw in the movie clip from a list of

20 popular products. An equally impressive 97% of the participants in the

prominent placement category correctly circled the brand name they saw

from a list of products. In the subtle placement category only 50.5% of

the participants correctly circled the name of the product. This question

revealed that the use of prominent placements are more effective in

product recognition than subtle placements. Again, possible reasons could

be the added exposure of being voiced and/or used by the main

characters.

In summary, results seem to support both the research of Gupta and Lord (1998) and Brennan and Dubas (1999), which found that prominent brand placements promote a significantly higher level of brand

76 awareness than subtle brand placements and that the greater use of sensory cues may increase effectiveness of brand recall. Although the subtle placement category received lower percentage scores in all three test questions, these scores might have been even lower if the subtle placement in the movie. Mission to Mars was not blatant—highly visible and centered across the screen—which may have influenced a higher score in the subtle placement category than it would have recieved.

Limitations

There are many potential limitations to the generalization of a product placement study's findings. This study is a first in terms of assessing the effectiveness of product placement on a movie genre basis.

While this study may be used as an initial benchmark, extensions of its approach should eliminate current limitations.

First, the stimulus material or films used in the experiment should be independently created if possible. This would allow the researcher to solve several problems plaguing product placement research. Created materials would allow the researcher to control the type of placement-subtle or prominent. This would allow for a cleaner investigation into genre effects. Additionally, by using stimulus materials

11 created specifically for research purposes, the types of products placed in clips could be controlled. When using actual products, as was the case in this study, participants possess pre-existing cognitions related to the

brand. For instance, a participant may already have a set of beliefs

concerning Pepsi or Jack Daniels. Ultimately, product placement studies

that employ fictitious products would allow researchers to more

accurately gauge how placement alone affects perceptions of products.

Second, the sample should not be limited to college students.

Although 18-24-year-olds are an appropriate target for product

placement, their opinions and reactions may differ from others. The

participants of this study were students who perhaps were conscious and

cautious of the classroom experiment and more sensitive to the use of

placements than the general public. Therefore, more research is

necessary with other groups. For example, an older sample might not be

familiar with product placement practices or be an avid user(s) of the

products used in the study. Also, this sample was limited geographically

and ethnically.

Third, an attitudinal pretest should be given to the subjects several

days prior to the experiment to determine whether there are existing

78 prejudices, favorable or not, to the products used in the experiment. An

attitudinal pretest should provide a statistical benchmark of a

participant's attitude prior to the experiment in order to compare

changes, if any, after the movie genre's influence or exposure.

Finally, researchers should attempt to use the same product in all

genres to help alleviate potential contaminates. Also using a product with

moderate, prejudices, undertones and popularity may provide a more

realistic account of subjects' attitudinal and recall scores.

Considerations for Future Research

There is much more research needed in this area of product

placement. The present study positioned brand placement as being

present in feature films screened in movie theaters. It did not assess

attitudes toward brand placement, recall, or brand liking in television

programs-dramas and/or sitcoms-or other home video markets.

Researchers should consider the possibility of divergent attitudes

between medias as well as new technologies such as TiVo, which could

catapult the use of brand placement in coming years due to PVRs allowing consumers to zap advertisements during commercial breaks. Researchers should consider using fictitious products in their experiments to eliminate

79 any possible pre-existing preceptions respondants may have toward popular and often used products. And lastly, researchers should consider whether the type of movie rating (e.g., G, PG, PG-13, R-rated) affects product placement.

80 Figure 1. Photo I, There's Sor?iethlng About Mary • This photo illustrates subtle placement in the movie clip. Note. From the movie. There's Sor?iethlng About Mary, by 20'" Century Fox. Copyright 1998.

Figure 2. Photo 2, Mission to Mars. This photo illustrates subtle placement in the movie clip. Note. From the movie, Mission to Mars, by The Jacobson Company and Touchstone Pictures. Copyright 2000.

81 Figure 3. Photo 3, The Usual Suspects. This photo illustrates subtle placement in the movie clip. Note. From the movie. The Usual Suspects, by PolyGram Filmed Entertainment. Copyright 1995.

Figure 4. Photo 4, Me, Myself, and Irene. This photo illustrates prominent placement in the movie clip. Note. From the movie. Me, Myself, and Irene, by 20"" Century Fox. Copyright 2000.

82 Figure 5. Photo 5, The Philadelphia £.Kperir?ient This photo illustrates prominent placement in the movie clip. Note. From the movie, The Philadelphia Experiment by Cinema Group Ventures and New Pictures. Copyright 1984.

MiMliliHMItHiiM^' .mil iTiifWWBw » • "I'-i-i ilWMMHpHHMlMii II. ,.IJIl| II I I illllllli

gUiHg •M ii ''

Figure 6. Photo 6, Monster's Ball This photo illustrates prominent placement in the movie clip. Note. From the movie. Monster's Ball by Lee Daniels Entertainment. Copyright 2001.

83 CHAPTER VI

CONCLUSION

In conclusion, the use of brand placements is quite evident in movies today. This study should be of interest to those companies or services considering or presently using brand placement in movies. This study attempted to determine whether the success of some brands reporting a phenomenal sales increase might be partly dependant on the type of movie in which the brand was placed. The results of this study suggest that the comedy genre does not outrank the other genres on brand recall/recognition, brand liking, and attitude toward brand placement. The results of this initial experiment did suggest though that the science fiction genre seemed to be more successful than the other two genres tested. Additionally, this research did confirm the findings gathered by Gupta and Lord (1998) that prominent brand placements promote a significantly higher level of brand awareness than subtle brand placements. Ultimately, this study suggests questions to the placement industry and to advertising researchers.

For the advertising and product placement industry, the question remains, is there some movie feature—be it genre, subject matter, or

84 cast—that can predictably result in positive and effective product

placement? Specifically, can the industry discover an ideal vehicle that will

provide the best bang for the buck in promoting a consistently higher

brand recall, and/or brand liking? And, if a specific genre is determined as

most effective, are all movies in that genre appropriate for the type of

product the advertiser is selling.

Finally, for the researcher, the question remains, is the practice of

product placement ethical? Social implications need to be addressed with

the explosive growth of product placement and the emergence of new

technology, like TiVo. Researchers will need to determine whether the

industry is taking advantage of its daily foray into our homes by

potentially subjecting viewers to 30-minute sitcom commercials and 90-

minute product promotions?

85 REFERENCES

Babin, L. A. & Carder, S. T. (1995). Advertising via the box office: Is product placement effective. Journal of Promotion Management, 3(1/2), 31-51.

Babin, L. A. & Carder, S. T. (1996). Viewers recognition of brands placed within a film. International Journal of Advertising, 75(2), 140-151.

Basset, M. (2000). Tomorrow Never Dies, but integrity might. Retrieved October 1 5, 2001 from http:// www.mediaawareness.ca/eng/med/class/teamedia/bond2.htm

Benezra, K. & Ebenkamp, K. (1997). Clancy's latest : Placement games? Brandweek, 38, 14.

Blumer, H. (1933). Movies and Conduct. New York: Macmillan.

Brennan, I. & Dubas, K. M. (1999). The influence of product placement type and exposure time on product placement recognition. International Journal of Advertising, 18(3), 323-338.

Bronson, G. (1984, August 20). Ads in movies? You're already watching them. US News & World Report, 97, 43-44.

Brooks, T., & Marsh, E. (1988). The complete directory to prime time network TV shows, 1946-present. New York: Ballantine.

Bush, A. M. (1994). 'Killers' doesn't go better with Coke. Variety, 356(5), 7.

Bywater, T. & Sobchack, T. (1989). Introduction to Film Criticism. White Plains, NY: Longman.

Campbell, D. T. & J. C. Stanley (1963). Experimental and Quasi- Experimental Designs for Research. : Randy McNally & Co.

86 Cantor, J. & Venus, P. (1980). The effect of humor on recall of a radio advertisement. Journal of Broadcasting, 24, 13-22.

Chattopadhyay, A. & Basu, K. (1989). Prior brand evaluation as a moderator of the effects of humor in advertising. Journal of Marketing Research, 26(4), 466-476.

Cho, H. (1994). Antecedents of perceived humorousness and their relationships to humorous devices in advertising. Working paper. Department of Advertising, The University of Texas, Austin.

Clark, K. R. (1991, June 10). Group goes after brand-name film props. , p. B3.

Cline, V. B. (1974). Where do you draw the line? Prowo, UT: Brigham Young University Press.

Curry, S. R. (2003). PVR threat growing: Study says advertisers big losers if networks don't adapt to commercial skipping. Retrieved from http://www.tvweek.com/advertise/090103pvr.html

Darlin, D. (1995, November 6). Junior mints, I'm gonna make you a star. Forbes, 90-94.

Duncan, C. P. (1979). Humor in advertising: A behavioral perspective. Journal of the Academy of marketing Science, 7(4), 285-306.

Duncan, C. P. & Nelson, J. E. (1985). Effects of humor in a radio advertising experiment. Journal of Advertising, 14, 33-40.

Eddington, J. A. (1991). Exploring the issue of product placement in movies. Madison, Wl: University Press.

Edell, J. A. & Burke, M. C. (1987). The power of feelings in understanding advertising effects. Journal of Consumer Research, 14, 421-433.

Finn, S. & Hickson, T. M. (1986). Impact of arousing commercials on perceptions of TV news. Journalism Quarterly, 63, 369-371.

87 Gelb, B. D. & Pickett, C. M. (1983). Attitude toward the ad: Links to humor and to advertising effectiveness. Journal of Advertising, 72(2), 34-42.

Gillen, M. A. (1995). Ads begin to pop up in CD-roms, games. Billboard, 707(12), 58.

Glucksmann, A. (1972). Violence On The Screen, London: .

Gruner, C. (1970). The effect of humor in dull and interesting informative speeches. Central State Speeches-Journal, 21(3), 160-166.

Gupta, P. B. & Gould, S. J. (1997). Consumers' perceptions of ethics and acceptability of product placements in movies: Product category and individual differences. Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 79(1), 37-50.

Gupta, P. B. & Lord, K. R. (1998). Product placement in movies: The effect of prominence and on audience recall. Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 20(1), 47-59.

Higgins, K. T. (1985). There's gold in silver screen product plugs. Advertising Age, 79(12), 6.

Jowett, G. & Linton, J. M. (1989), Movies as Mass Communication, (2nd. ed.), Newbury, CA: Sage Publications.

Karrh, J. A. (1994). Effects of brand placements in motion pictures. Proceedings of the American Academy of Advertising, Athens, GA, 182-188.

Karrh, J. A. (1998). Brand placement: A review. Journal of Current Issues and Research on Advertising, 20(2), 31-49.

Karrh, J. A., Frith, K. T., & Callison, C. (2001). Audience attitudes towards brand (product) placement: Singapore and the . International Journal of Advertising, 20(1), 3-24.

88 Kinsley, M. (1990, June 11). These foolish things remind me of Diet Coke. Time, 735, 88.

Lowery, S. A. & DeFleur, M. L. (1995), Milestones in Mass Communication Research: Media Effects (3rd. ed.). White Plains, NY: Longman.

Madden, T. (1982). Humor in advertising: Applications of a hierarchy of effects paradigm. Unpublished octoral isertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.

Madden, T. & Weinberger, M. G. (1982). The effects of humor on attention in magazine advertising. Journal of Advertising, 17(3), 8- 14.

Madden, T. & Weinberger, M. G. (1984). Humor in advertising: A practitioner view. Journal of Advertising Research, 24(4), 23-29.

Marney, J. (1995, December 4). Last laughs: Serious research on humor in ads. Marketing Magazine, 100, 38.

McGill, D. C. (1 989, April 13). Questions Raised On Product Placements. , p. Dl 8.

McCollum & Speilman & Co. Inc. (1982). Focus on funny. Topline, 3(3), 1- 6.

Miller, M. C. (1990, April). Hollywood the ad. The Atlantic Monthly, 41-54.

Mizerski, R. W. & White, J. D. (1986). Understanding and using emotions in advertising. The Journal of Consumer Marketing, 3(4), 54-69.

Monush, B. (1991) International Picture Almanac 1991, New York: Quigley Publishing Company, Inc.

Nebenzahl, I. D. & Secunda, E. (1993). Consumer's attitudes toward product placement in movies. International Journal of Advertising, 12, 1-11.

89 Nelson, M. (2001, September 5). Advertisers got game: Examining effectiveness of product placements in new media. Submitted to AEJMC Conference Papers.

Critics seek F.T.C. action on products as movie stars. (1991, May 31). The New York Times, p. Dl.

Nielsen Media Research (1990, February 1 5). Top 25 prime time programs of the 1980s by year. Unpublished data.

Osterhouse, R. & Brock, T. (1970). Distraction increases yielding to by inhibiting counterarguing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75,344-358.

Oliver, B. (1986, March). The latest screen stars. Marketing, 38-39.

Ong, B. S. & Meri, D. (1994). Should product placement in movies be banned? Journal of Promotion Management, 2(3/4), 1 59-175.

Pedder, S. (2002). Up the tube. The Economist, 363, survey 14 & 1 6.

Perry, S. D., Jenzowsky, S. A., King, C. M., Yi, H., Hester, J. B., & Gartenschlaeger, J. (1997). Using humorous programs as a vehicle for humorous commercials. Journal of Communication, 47, 20-39.

Perry, S. D., Jenzowsky, S. A., Hester, J. B., King, C. M., & Yi, H. (1997). The Influence of commercial humor on program enjoyment and evaluation. Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly, 74, 388- 399.

Puto, C. P. & Wells, W. D. (1984). Informational and transformational ' advertising: The Differential Effects of Time. Advances in Consumer Research, 7 7(1), 638-643.

Reed, J. D. (1989, January 2). Plugging away in Hollywood. Time, 103.

90 Russell, C. A. (1998). Toward a framework of product placement: Theoretical propositions. Advances in Consumer Research, 25, 357- 362.

Scott, C, Klein, D. M. & Bryant, J. (1990). Consumer response to humor advertising: A series of field studies using behavioral observation. Journal of Consumer Research, 16, 498-501.

Solomon, R. L. (1949). An extension of control group design. Psychological Bulletin, 46, 137-150.

Speck, P. S. (1987). On humor & humor in advertising. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Texas Tech University, Lubbock.

Spillman, S. (1985). Marketers race to leave their brands on films. Advertising Age, 56(3), 55.

Stewart, D. M. & Furse, D. H. (1986). Effective television advertisng. Lexington, MA: D.C. Heath and Company.

Top 100 all-time film rental champs. (1990, January 24). Variety, p. 46.

Vollmers, S. & Mizerski, R. (1994). A review and investigation into the effectiveness of product placements in films. , FL: Florida State University Press.

Wixted, J. T. & McDowell, J. J. (1989). Contributions to the functional analysis of single-trial free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 75(4), 685-697.

Weinberger, M. G. & Campbell, L (1991). The use and impact of humor in radio advertising. Journal of Advertising Research, 31, 44-52.

Weinberger, M. G. & Gulas, C. S. (1992). The impact of humor in advertising: A review. Journal of Advertising, 21, 35-59.

91 Weinberger, M. G., Spotts, H. E., Leiand, C, and Parsons, A. L. (1995). The use of humor in different advertising media. Journal of Advertising Research, 35(3), 44-56.

Weiner, T. (1989). The Book of Video Lists. Lanham, MD: Madison Books.

Zhang, Y. & Zinkhan, G. M. (1991). Humor in television advertising. Advances in Consumer Research, 78, 813-818.

Zillmann, D. & Jennings, B. (1991). Responding to the screen: Reception and reaction processes. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Eribaum Associates, Inc.

92 APPENDIX

QUESTIONNAIRE

93 MOVIE VIEWING STUDY

We are doing research on movie viewing and what parts of movies are best used to market upcoming films. Now that you have seen a movie clip, please comment on aspects of the clip.

1. How many times during a month do you go to a movie?

2. How many times during a month do you rent a movie?

3. Have you seen this entire movie before? (please circle) A. Yes B. No C. Not Sure 4. Please evaluate the movie clip you saw on the following scales: (please circle number)

Likable 1 2 3 4 5 Unlikable Funny 1 2 3 4 5 Not Funny Entertaining 1 2 3 4 5 Unentertaining Dramatic 1 2 3 4 5 Not Dramatic Good 1 2 3 4 5 Bad Futuristic I 2 3 4 5 Not Futuristic Pleasant I 2 3 4 5 Unpleasant Worth Watching 1 2 3 4 5 Not Worth Watching

Please continue

94 5. This movie clip represents a good example of a science fiction. (please circle number)

Strongly Agree 12 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree

6. This movie clip represents a good example of a drama. (please circle number)

Strongly Agree 12 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree

7. This movie clip represents a good example of a comedy. (please circle number)

Strongly Agree 12 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree

Product placement is the practice of placing consumer brand products or services by a placement agency or company in a motion picture.

8. Was there product placement in the movie clip you saw? (please circle)

A. Yes B. No

9. Name the product placed in this movie clip?

Please continue

95 Please answer the following questions by indicating your opinion on the number scale.

10. 1 mind if brand-name products appear in movies? (please circle number)

Strongly Agree 12 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree

11.1 mind if movie producers receive money or other compensation from advertisers for placing their brands in movies? (please circle number)

Strongly Agree 12 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree

12. 1 think it is unethical for movie producers to attempt to influence the audience by including brand-name products in their movies? (please circle number)

Strongly Agree 12 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree

13.1 think that seeing the brand-name product in a movie makes the experience more realistic? (please circle number)

Strongly Agree 12 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree

Please continue...

96 14. The movie clip did contain a product placement. From the products listed below, please select the one product placed in the movie clip? (please circle the ONE product)

Coke Snickers Fritos Miller Lite Jack Daniel's Budweiser Starbucks Cheetos Corvette Bacardi Pepsi Heineken Mountain Dew BMW Dr. Pepper Twix Crown Royal Apple Compute

15. Please write in your age

16. Gender (please check one)

Male Female

17. Classification (please check one)

Freshman Graduate Student Sophomore Non Student Junior Senior

Please continue,

97 (Coke, Pepsi. Dr Pepper, Budweiser, Jack Daniels)* was the product placed in the movie clip you just saw. In the following questions please indicate your opinion regarding the product on the number scale.

18. 1 did not mind that (Coke, Pepsi, Dr Pepper, Budweiser, Jack Daniels) was a product placement in the movie? (please circle number)

Strongly Agree 12 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree

19. 1 thought the appearance of (Coke, Pepsi, Dr Pepper, Budweiser, Jack Daniels) in the movie was great? (please circle number)

Strongly Agree 12 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree

20. 1 thought the appearance of (Coke, Pepsi, Dr Pepper, Budweiser, Jack Daniels) in the movie was realistic? (please circle number)

Strongly Agree 12 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree

21.1 think that (Coke, Pepsi, Dr Pepper, Budweiser, Jack Daniels) is a great product? (please circle number)

Strongly Agree 12 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree

22. The appearance of (Coke, Pepsi, Dr Pepper, Budweiser, Jack Daniels) in this movie has increased my liking of (Coke, Pepsi, Dr Pepper, Budweiser, Jack Daniels)? (please circle number) Strongly Agree 12 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree THE END Thank You For Your Time!

Note: One product choice was given in reference of the movie clip shown.

98 PERMISSION TO COPY

In presenting this thesis in partial fulfillment of the requirements for a

master's degree at Texas Tech University or Texas Tech University Health Sciences

Center, I agree that the Library and my major department shall make it freely

available for research purposes. Permission to copy this thesis for scholarly purposes

may be granted by the Director of the Library or my major professor. It is

understood that any copying or publication of this thesis for financial gain shall not be allowed without my fiirther written permission and that any user may be liable for

copyright infringement.

Agree (Permission is granted.)

Student Signature Date

Disagree (Permission is not granted.)

Student Signature Date