Or, the Potential Rewards for Scholars Who Dialog Across Difference

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Or, the Potential Rewards for Scholars Who Dialog Across Difference Dewey, Women, and Weirdoes: or, the Potential Rewards for Scholars who Dialogue across Difference Craig A. Cunningham, David Granger, Jane Fowler Morse, Barbara Stengel, and Terri Wilson Editor’s note: These essays were presented as part of an alternative session at the Annual Meeting of the Philosophy of Education Society in March of 2005. The authors have refined their original presentations and added an introduction and conclusion to make their thoughts and questions available to a wider audience through Education and Culture. Abstract This symposium provides five case studies of the ways that John Dewey’s philoso- phy and practice were influenced by women or “weirdoes” (our choices include F. M. Alexander, Albert Barnes, Helen Bradford Thompson, Elsie Ripley Clapp, and Jane Addams) and presents some conclusions about the value of dialoging across difference for philosophers and other scholars. Introduction In The Education of John Dewey, Jay Martin (2003) suggests, “It was and remained a characteristic of Dewey that he was always receptive to alternative ideas. With pro- fessional philosophers, he generally held to his own positions, but with intelligent women, non-philosophers, odd thinkers, and ordinary folk, he was a student again” (167). Martin’s insight is the basis for a hypothesis that seems worth exploring: that John Dewey is able to speak eloquently to us today—as much as a century after he formulated his ideas—precisely because of his willingness to listen, to closely at- tend to those we might categorize as women and weirdoes. We use the provocative expression, “women and weirdoes,” to acknowledge E&C/Education and Culture 23 (2) (2007): 27–62 ◆ 27 28 ◆ Craig A. Cunningham et al. that John Dewey took seriously persons who often were not given credence in philosophical circles. During Dewey’s scholarly career, women constituted minor voices at best in philosophical discourse; in addition, philosophy had come to be professionalized in Dewey’s time in a way that excluded those without philosophical credentials. However, Dewey came to understand (and with Emerson, to proclaim) that philosophy and life were not separable domains. And Dewey’s biographers note the significant role played in his life by such women as Alice Dewey, Jane Addams, Ella Flagg Young, numerous graduate students, the Camp sisters, Anzia Yezier- ska, and by such “odd ducks” and nonphilosophers as Scudder Klyce, F. Matthias Alexander, Franklin Ford, and Albert Barnes. While these sources of inspiration we seek to study may be classified as “odd” or “socially subordinate” in the realm of professional philosophy, it is also true that each of the persons we identify were intelligent in Dewey’s sense of a mind in use—often in unusual ways. We main- tain that the interaction between Dewey’s lived experience and the shaping of his philosophical inquiry warrants greater scholarly attention. Our perspective is partly historical and partly philosophical. Dewey’s rela- tionships have, for the most part, been documented. A variety of well-researched biographies, the Collected Works and now the Collected Papers, lay out the basic historical narrative. However, some relationships have been mined extensively and others less well considered. And what has not been done in a systematic way is the kind of analysis that looks closely at Dewey’s thinking in juxtaposition with the thinking of nonphilosopher others in the context of those particular relation- ships. The work offered here is a first step that lays out some useful paths for more extensive historical inquiry and offers some preliminary examples of the kind of textual analysis that can prove our thesis. We are not suggesting that Dewey didn’t listen to or attend to other profes- sional philosophers. Obviously he did. Those influences are well documented in the Rockefeller (1991) and Ryan (1995) biographies, among others. Here we begin to explore the “value added” by Dewey’s willingness to take seriously—for philo- sophical purposes—those who moved outside the bounds of professional philoso- phy, and even, in some cases, outside of accepted social circles. We are particularly interested in the ways in which Dewey’s works seem remarkably contemporary, as if they were written to address present issues in education, the arts and public life. Is it possible that the contemporary power of Dewey’s thought rests, at least in part, in just his willingness to cultivate, appreciate and appropriate the thinking of in- teresting women and odd ducks? Two essential questions guided our inquiry: First, were Dewey’s theories in fact significantly influenced by women and those professionally excluded eccentric voices we are calling weirdoes? How was this influence transacted? Second, if such influence did impact Dewey’s own thinking, so what? What should we think, say, or do as a result of establishing Dewey’s philosophically significant interaction with women and weirdoes, and their influence on him? As we pursued this work, it became clear that our initial questions would not E&C ◆ Education and Culture Dewey, Women, and Weirdoes ◆ 29 be easily answered—and would not be answered at all until we raised a range of other questions. Why was Dewey drawn to those we studied? What was the nature of his relationships with them? What kind of a teacher—and learner—was Dewey? What qualities of personal responsiveness permeated his teaching and his theoriz- ing? How did Dewey go about developing insights and theories? How did he share those insights and theories with the broader public? What emerges is a picture of the philosopher as “man thinking” (to use Em- erson’s idiom). Of course, for Dewey, thinking done well is an interactive process into real puzzles for actual human persons and marked by open-mindedness and responsiveness. These are elements that we find in each of the case studies described below. These qualities of thinking are the elements that opened Dewey to the influ- ences of all those with whom he came and remained in contact. Because we exam- ined only a few candidates, and because the constraints of time and space prohibited us from completing a thorough examination of even these initial examples, we see our work here as preliminary, as more of a pilot study than a comprehensive one. Our hope is that these beginnings may inspire others to conduct similar studies, either by joining with us in a longer-term project, or by working on their own to address similar questions. Included here are five case studies taken from Dewey’s biography. Among the many women and weirdoes we could have selected, we chose Jane Addams, Helen Bradford Thompson, Elsie Ripley Clapp (as an exemplar of Dewey’s interaction with female graduate students at Teachers College), F. M. Alexander and Albert Barnes. We suggest that Dewey was influenced intellectually in these instances where he experienced dialogue across difference, and that these relationships are not anoma- lies, but critical tokens in understanding the quality of Dewey’s thinking. Dewey’s thinking is enriched when he is a “learner,” undergoing the kind of questioning in the face of trouble or doubt or desire that he himself so famously claims is the starting point of thinking. We offer here sketches of Dewey not as eminent philosopher but as a thinker learning from different others Dewey’s Pragmatic Poet: Reconstructing Jane Addams’s Philosophical Impact Barbara Stengel John Dewey calls Ralph Waldo Emerson “The Philosopher of Democracy” in an es- say of the same title (1903/1977). In making his case that Emerson is a philosopher, Dewey acknowledges that some (including Emerson himself) might be inclined to see him as a poet rather than a philosopher. Dewey goes on to discuss the differ- ence between the poet and the philosopher. The poet is maker rather than reflec- tor. The poet discerns and uncovers rather than analyzes and classifies. The poet evidences a “natural attitude” where the philosopher relies on reasons for believing. However, the distinction is not hard and fast; in Emerson’s case at least, one can be Volume 23 (2) ◆ 2007 30 ◆ Craig A. Cunningham et al. both poet and philosopher. Dewey’s description of Emerson as poet and philosopher of democracy holds, I suggest, for Jane Addams as well, but it is, perhaps ironically, as poet that Add- ams impacted the philosophy of John Dewey. Addams is unquestionably a maker of democratic community and pragmatic education; Dewey is just as unquestion- ably a reflector. Through her work at Hull House, Addams discerned the shape of democracy as a mode of associated living and uncovered the outlines of an experi- mental approach to knowledge and understanding; Dewey analyzed and classified the social, psychological and educational processes Addams lived. As I will dem- onstrate below, Addams’s “natural attitude” brought Dewey up short in a situation in which he could, by his own admission, only rely on reason. In this essay, I claim that Dewey became Dewey in the last decade of the nine- teenth century and that Jane Addams was present as poet to his philosopher. When I say that Dewey became Dewey, I mean that he let go of religious practice and theo- logical language, focused a conception of democracy as a mode of associated liv- ing, shifted from Hegelian dialectic to pragmatic experimentalism, acknowledged the relational nature of the self and found a way to think about thinking rooted in human action, thus acknowledging the unity of human experience. Dewey’s inter- action with Addams, again by his own admission, forced a reconsideration of his thinking, a reconstruction that led to the very elements (noted above) that have rendered Deweyan thought useful to us in the early twenty-first century. I make my case by focusing here on just one significant instance documented in Dewey’s correspondence and described—in various ways—in contemporary Dewey biog- raphies.
Recommended publications
  • Posible Fundamentación Biológica De Las Principales Diferencias Según El Sexo Juan Fernández
    Posible fundamentación biológica de las principales diferencias según el sexo Juan Fernández «No hay quizá campo que aspire a ser científico donde los sesgos personales hayan sido más flagrantes, la lógica haya sido más martirizada al servicio y apoyo de prejuicios y donde afirmaciones infundadas e incluso tonterías y boberías sentimentales hayan cometido tantos excesos como sobre este tema.» HELEN THOMPSON WOOLLEY 1. LA EVOLUCION HISTORICA DEL PROBLEMA La variable «sexo» ha tenido y sigue teniendo una importancia capital den- tro de la realidad social a juzgar por los estudios sociológico-antropológicos (Mead, 1935; MurClock, 1937; Durkheim, 1947; Parsons y Bales, 1955), desde prácticamente el comienzo de la humanidad. La especialización de tareas o segregación de actividades de acuerdo con el rol sexual aparecen como cons- tantes a lo largo de la historia determinando en buena medida las estructuras sociales, independientemente de que se verifiquen o no los estudios actuales sobre la existencia de principios o modelos universales de segregación de ac- tividades económicas según el sexo (Aronoff y Grano, 1975) y también, in- dependientemente de la valoración que se quiera dar a estos principios en la construcción del edificio social actual (Yorburg, 1974; Rosaldo y Lamphere, 1974; Rosenblatt y Cunningham, 1976; Quinn, 1977; Kelly, 1981). Dentro de este contexto, la especulación en torno a las diferencias entre los sexos parece haber sido tema frecuente tanto por parte de filósofos, poe- tas, literatos y hombres de ciencia en general como por parte del hombre de la calle en todas las épocas de la historia (Ellis, 1904; Terman y Miles, 1963; Rocheblave-Spenlé, 1964; Bakan, 1966).
    [Show full text]
  • Outstanding Women of Color Awards
    UW–MADISON’S 13TH ANNUAL Outstanding Women of Color Awards SHENIKQUA BOUGES, M.D. MARY MUSE, R.N., M.S.N. JESSICA PEREZ-CHAVEZ LESLIE PETTY, ED.D. SAMI SCHALK, PH.D. MONICA WHITE, PH.D. WEDNESDAY, MARCH 3, 2021 HOSTED BY THE OFFICE OF THE VICE PROVOST AND CHIEF DIVERSITY OFFICER1 WEDNESDAY, MARCH 3, 2021 Mary Muse, R.N., M.S.N. Program State Director of Nursing, Wisconsin Department of Corrections, Certified Correctional Health Profes- sional (CCHP)-RN, CCHP-Advanced Introduced by Barbara Bowers Jessica Perez-Chavez Doctoral Candidate, Counseling Psychology, School of Education A Salute to Outstanding Introduced by Stephanie Budge CONGRATULATORYWomen of REMARKS Color Leslie Petty, Ed.D. The Honorable Mandela Barnes, Wisconsin Assistant Dean of Evening, Executive, & Professional Lieutenant Governor MBA Programs, Wisconsin School of Business Introduced by Enno Siemsen and Jean Sink WELCOME Sami Schalk, Ph.D. Cheryl B. Gittens, Interim Deputy Vice Chancellor Associate Professor, Department of Gender & for Diversity & Inclusion, Chief Diversity Officer Women’s Studies, College of Letters and Sciences Introduced by Janet Hyde REMARKS UW–Madison Chancellor Rebecca Blank Monica White, Ph.D. Associate Professor, Nelson Institute for Environmen- ABOUT THE AWARD tal Studies, Community and Environmental Sociology, College of Agriculture and Life Sciences Laura Minero-Meza, 2019–2020 OWoC Cohort Introduced by Michael Bell PRESENTATION OF THE 2020–2021 COHORT OF HONOREES CLOSING REMARKS Cheryl B. Gittens, Interim Deputy Vice Chancellor Shenikqua Bouges, M.D. for Diversity & Inclusion, Chief Diversity Officer Advanced Geriatric Fellow, School of Medicine and Public Health/Division of Geriatrics and Gerontology Introduced by Taryn James 2 Outstanding Women of Color Awards 3 research.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychology and Feminism: Can This Relationship Be Saved?
    Swarthmore College Works Psychology Faculty Works Psychology 1995 Psychology And Feminism: Can This Relationship Be Saved? Jeanne Marecek Swarthmore College, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://works.swarthmore.edu/fac-psychology Part of the Psychology Commons Let us know how access to these works benefits ouy Recommended Citation Jeanne Marecek. (1995). "Psychology And Feminism: Can This Relationship Be Saved?". Feminisms In The Academy. 101-134. https://works.swarthmore.edu/fac-psychology/1036 This work is brought to you for free by Swarthmore College Libraries' Works. It has been accepted for inclusion in Psychology Faculty Works by an authorized administrator of Works. For more information, please contact [email protected]. chapter 4 Psychology and Feminism: Can This Relationship Be Saved? Jeanne Marecek Since its early days the field of psychology has issued pronouncements about women and set out prescriptions for their mental health and proper conduct. Women in the discipline often dissented from the pronouncements and prescrip­ tions of mainstream psychology, but for much of psychology’s one hundred- year history women’s voices were few and far between. The past quarter of a century has witnessed a dramatic increase in the overall number of women in psychology. Moreover, many women, committed to feminism in their personal lives, have been committed to feminist ideals in their work as researchers, thera­ pists, and teachers as well. Two organizations, one within the main professional association (the Division of the Psychology of Women of the American Psycho­ logical Association, established in 1973) and one outside (the Association for Women in Psychology, established in 1969), exist, providing a locus of colle- giality and institutional support for feminist scholarship and activism (Mednick and Urbanski 1991; Tiefer 1991).
    [Show full text]
  • Teaching Guide for the History of Psychology Instructor Compiled by Jennifer Bazar, Elissa Rodkey, and Jacy Young
    A Teaching Guide for History of Psychology www.feministvoices.com 1 Psychology’s Feminist Voices in the Classroom A Teaching Guide for the History of Psychology Instructor Compiled by Jennifer Bazar, Elissa Rodkey, and Jacy Young One of the goals of Psychology’s Feminist Voices is to serve as a teaching resource. To facilitate the process of incorporating PFV into the History of Psychology course, we have created this document. Below you will find two primary sections: (1) Lectures and (2) Assignments. (1) Lectures: In this section you will find subject headings of topics often covered in History of Psychology courses. Under each heading, we have provided an example of the relevant career, research, and/or life experiences of a woman featured on the Psychology’s Feminist Voices site that would augment a lecture on that particular topic. Below this description is a list of additional women whose histories would be well-suited to lectures on that topic. (2) Assignments: In this section you will find several suggestions for assignments that draw on the material and content available on Psychology’s Feminist Voices that you can use in your History of Psychology course. The material in this guide is intended only as a suggestion and should not be read as a “complete” list of all the ways Psychology’s Feminist Voices could be used in your courses. We would love to hear all of the different ideas you think of for how to include the site in your classroom - please share you thoughts with us by emailing Alexandra Rutherford, our project coordinator,
    [Show full text]
  • 1 25 November 2014 Presidents and Chancellors Association Of
    25 November 2014 Presidents and Chancellors Association of American Universities, Consortium on Financing Higher Education Dear Presidents and Chancellors: As a group of 45 researchers advocating for a rigorous scientific approach to assessment of sexual victimization and campus climate, we continue to recommend that decision makers defer participation in an AAU survey by the December 1 deadline to allow time for a thoughtful and transparent evaluation of the issues. Information provided in the November 20 AAU FAQ (that we just today saw) raises new concerns that suggest unanticipated negative consequences. 1. The survey is unlikely to be as strong as other models because AAU is neither waiting to benefit from the psychometric studies being undertaken by the Bureau of Justice Statistics nor using a process that will allow adequate input from experts. 2. If implemented universally across AAU and Consortium institutions, the survey would likely remove the 80 top universities from a potential national data base being developed through the Presidential Executive Order because they would have recently measured campus climate with non-comparable questionnaire components. 3. The AAU time frame for survey design, IRB approval, administration and data analysis is unrealistic for a scientifically sound result. The FAQ says IRBs cannot change anything; how will AAU override the usual independent process with IRBs? 4. The AAU is proposing a one-size fits all model whereas there is a strong argument for a uniform core across institutions and then individualized components that reflect campus specific concerns. The AAU contract with Westat forbids individual campuses from adding questions of local interest such as reactions to prevention programs or perceptions of residence life initiatives.
    [Show full text]
  • Gender and Emotion: Social Psychological Perspectives
    Gender and emotion: Social psychological perspectives Edited by Agneta H. Fischer & Editions de la Maison des Sciences de l’Homme Paris published by the press syndicate of the university of cambridge The Pitt Building, Trumpington Street, Cambridge, United Kingdom and editions de la maison des sciences de l’homme 54 Boulevard Raspail, 75270 Paris Cedex 06, France cambridge university press The Edinburgh Building, Cambridge CB2 2RU, UK http://www.cup.cam.ac.uk 40 West 20th Street, New York, NY 10011–4211, USA http://www.cup.org 10 Stamford Road, Oakleigh, Melbourne 3166, Australia © Agneta H. Fischer 2000 This book is in copyright. Subject to statutory exception and to the provisions of relevant collective licensing agreements, no reproduction of any part may take place without the written permission of Cambridge University Press. First published 2000 Printed in the United Kingdom at the University Press, Cambridge Typeset in 10/12 pt Palatino in QuarkXPress® [se] A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library Library of Congress cataloguing in Publication data Gender and emotion: social psychological perspectives / [edited by] Agneta H. Fischer. p. cm. – (Studies in emotion and social interaction. Second series) Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 0–521–63015–0 (hardback) – ISBN 0–521–63986–7 (paperback) 1. Expression – Sex differences. 2. Emotions – Sex differences. I. Fischer, Agneta, 1958– . II. Series. BF591.G45 2000 155.3′3–dc21 00-29140 CIP ISBN 0 521 63015 0 hardback ISBN 0 521 63986 7 paperback Contents Preface agneta h. fischer page x Contributors xi Part I Culture, gender, and emotional beliefs 1 Thinking about gender, thinking about theory: Gender and emotional experience stephanie a.
    [Show full text]
  • Personal Politics: the Rise of Personality Traits in The
    PERSONAL POLITICS: THE RISE OF PERSONALITY TRAITS IN THE CENTURY OF EUGENICS AND PSYCHOANALYSIS IAN J. DAVIDSON A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY GRADUATE PROGRAM IN PSYCHOLOGY YORK UNIVERSITY TORONTO, ONTARIO AUGUST 2020 © IAN J. DAVIDSON, 2020 ii Abstract This dissertation documents personality psychology’s development alongside psychoanalysis and eugenics, offering a disciplinary and cultural history of personality across the twentieth century. Using the psychological concepts of neurosis and introversion as an organizational framework, personality’s history is portrayed as one of “success:” a succession of hereditarianism and its politics of normativity; a successful demarcation of the science of personality from competing forms of expertise; and a successful cleansing of personality psychology’s interchanges with unethical researchers and research. Chapter 1 provides background for the dissertation, especially focusing on turn-of-the- century developments in the nascent fields of American psychology and the importation of psychoanalytic ideas. It ends with a look at Francis Galton’s eugenicist and statistical contributions that carved a key path for psychological testers to discipline psychoanalytic concepts. Part I details the rise of personality testing in the USA during the interwar years, while also considering the many sexual and gender norms at play. Chapter 2 tracks the varied places in the 1920s that personality tests were developed: from wartime military camps to university laboratories to the offices of corporate advertisers. Chapter 3 takes stock of popular psychoanalytic notions of personality alongside the further psychometric development of personality testing. These developments occurred at a time when American eugenicists— including psychologists—were transitioning to a “positive” form that emphasized marriage and mothering.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychological Bulletin
    Vol. XI. No. 10. October 15, 1914 THE PSYCHOLOGICAL BULLETIN GENERAL REVIEWS AND SUMMARIES THE PSYCHOLOGY OF SEX BY HELEN THOMPSON WOOLLEY Cincinnati, Ohio During the four years since my last review of the literature of the psychology of sex (PSYCHOL. BULL., October, 1910) the number of experimental investigations in the field has increased to such an extent that whereas it was difficult at that time to find anything to review, it is now impossible to review all I could find. The number of books and essays devoted to general discussions of the subject has also increased and their quality has improved very markedly. The emphasis placed on sex by the Freudian school and the interest in sex education, to say nothing of the whole feminist and woman's suffrage movement, have swelled the dimensions of the literature aside from experimental contributions to such an extent that no brief review could pretend to deal with it. Confronted by such a dilemma I have chosen the course of attempt- ing a summary in the field of experimental psychology as complete as time and library facilities would allow, and a very brief mention of what seem to me the most important contributions to the other phases of the subject.1 I. EXPERIMENTAL AND STATISTICAL STUDIES There have been two extended series of tests applied for the purpose of measuring sex differences, one by Burt and Moore (19), 11 am indebted to Mr. Charles A. Reed, librarian of the University of Cincinnati, for special library privileges and for assistance in borrowing books and periodicals from libraries at a distance, without which it would have been impossible to prepare this review in Cincinnati.
    [Show full text]
  • NOBA History of Psychology
    NOBA History of Psychology David B. Baker & Heather Sperry This module provides an introduction and overview of the historical development of the science and practice of psychology in America. Ever-increasing specialization within the field often makes it difficult to discern the common roots from which the field of psychology has evolved. By exploring this shared past, students will be better able to understand how psychology has developed into the discipline we know today. Learning Objectives • Describe the precursors to the establishment of the science of psychology. • Identify key individuals and events in the history of American psychology. • Describe the rise of professional psychology in America. • Develop a basic understanding of the processes of scientific development and change. • Recognize the role of women and people of color in the history of American psychology. Introduction It is always a difficult question to ask, where to begin to tell the story of the history of psychology. Some would start with ancient Greece; others would look to a demarcation in the late 19th century when the science of psychology was formally proposed and instituted. These two perspectives, and all that is in between, are appropriate for describing a history of psychology. The interested student will have no trouble finding an abundance of resources History of Psychology 2 on all of these time frames and perspectives (Goodwin, 2011; Leahey, 2012; Schultz & Schultz, 2007). For the purposes of this module, we will examine the development of psychology in America and use the mid-19th century as our starting point. For the sake of convenience, we refer to this as a history of modern psychology.
    [Show full text]
  • Women's Place in the History of Sport Psychology By
    Women's Place in the History of Sport Psychology By: Diane L. Gill Gill, D.L. (1995). Women's place in the history of sport psychology. The Sport Psychologist, 9, 418-433. Made available courtesy of Human Kinetics: http://hk.humankinetics.com/TSP/journalAbout.cfm ***Note: Figures may be missing from this format of the document Abstract: Women and women's issues have a place in sport psychology today, but women have no place in most histories of the field. Some women sport psychologists, particularly Dorothy Harris, were instrumental in the development of sport psychology as a subdiscipline in the 1960s and 1970s. Re-searching the historical founda- tions reveals that the notable contributions of Harris and other women sport psychologists have roots in earlier work in both psychology and physical education. Pioneering women psychologists conducted research and challenged sexist assumptions and practices since psychology's earliest days. At the same time, prominent women leaders developed women-oriented programs and contributed to the professional literature in physical education. Women and women's issues have a place in sport psychology today, but too often that is an "other" or special interest place. By taking steps to re-place women in history and by engendering current research and practice, sport and exercise psychology will be a stronger science and profession. Article: Do women have a place in the history of sport psychology? Clearly women have a place in sport and exercise psychology today. The latest Association for the Advancement of Applied Sport Psychology (AAASP) newsletter indicated that the 1994 membership is 41% female, and my unofficial count from the 1993 North American Society for the Psychology of Sport and Physical Activity (NASPSPA) directory indicates a similar 46% female membership.
    [Show full text]
  • Inequity for Women in Psychology: How Much Have We Progressed and What Work Still Needs to Be Done? Caitlin Martin-Wagar [email protected]
    The University of Akron IdeaExchange@UAkron Psychology from the Margins April 2018 Inequity for Women in Psychology: How Much Have We Progressed and What Work Still Needs to Be Done? Caitlin Martin-Wagar [email protected] Please take a moment to share how this work helps you through this survey. Your feedback will be important as we plan further development of our repository. Follow this and additional works at: http://ideaexchange.uakron.edu/psychologyfromthemargins Part of the History Commons, Multicultural Psychology Commons, and the Social Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Martin-Wagar, Caitlin (2018) "Inequity for Women in Psychology: How Much Have We Progressed and What Work Still Needs to Be Done?," Psychology from the Margins: Vol. 1 : Iss. 1 , Article 3. Available at: http://ideaexchange.uakron.edu/psychologyfromthemargins/vol1/iss1/3 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by IdeaExchange@UAkron, the institutional repository of The nivU ersity of Akron in Akron, Ohio, USA. It has been accepted for inclusion in Psychology from the Margins by an authorized administrator of IdeaExchange@UAkron. For more information, please contact [email protected], [email protected]. Martin-Wagar: Inequity for Women in Psychology All along the question of marriage interferes with the woman's assured planning. Can a woman become a fanatic in her profession and still remain marriageable? Yes, she can, for I know some, but I think a woman must be abnormally bright to combine charm with concentration. These women make the synthesis by being charmingly enthusiastic. The Woman Problem comes up again after the professional woman has acquired a husband and a couple of children, with the culture pressing to give her a heavy responsibility in the home, with her husband noting, perhaps, that his own success demands his own job-concentration.
    [Show full text]
  • A Review of the Recent Literature on the Psychology of Sex
    A Review of the Recent Literature on the Psychology of Sex Helen Thompson Woolley (1910) Classics in the History of Psychology An internet resource developed by Christopher D. Green York University, Toronto, Ontario ISSN 1492-3713 (Return to C lassics index ) A Review of the Recent Literature on the Psychology of Sex Helen Thompson Woolley (1910) First published in Psychological Bulletin, 7, 335-342. Posted March 2001 Although the past few years have witnessed the appearance of a number of comprehensive and even encyclopedic works on various aspects of the problem of sex, there has been comparatively little advance in knowledge of the mental characteristics of sex. The few positive contributions in the literature of anatomy and experimental psychology I will review first, and will then deal briefly with the more pretentious treatises which are summaries and theoretical discussions. The most important single contribution to our knowledge of the facts of the case is to be found in Dr. Franklin P. Mall's paper 'On Several Anatomical Characters of the Human Brain Said to Vary According to Race and Sex, with Especial Reference to the Weight of the Frontal Lobe' (Am. J. of Anat., IX., p. 1, 1909). Dr. Mall's general conclusion is that there is as yet no reliable evidence for the variation of anatomical characters with either race or sex. The belief that the brains of females differ from those of males has been widely accepted, and has been thought to be conclusive evidence of the permanent inferiority of the female mind. The points in which the female brain has been said to be inferior to the male are: (1) total weight; (2) proportionate weight of the frontal lobe indicated by (a) actual weighings, and (b) determinations of the position of the central sulcus; (3) the area of the corpus callosum; (4) the complexity of gyri and sulci; (5) the conformation of gyri and sulci; and (6) the rate of development of the cortex in the foetus.
    [Show full text]