<<

First Peoples & Review A Journal on Innovation and Best Practices in Aboriginal Child Welfare Administration, Research, Policy & Practice

Volume 3, Number 2, 2007, Special Issue, pp. 65-74

Is Attachment Theory Consistent with Aboriginal Realities?

Raymond Neckowaya, Keith Brownleea, and Bruno Castellana

a School of , Lakehead University, ThunderBay, ON, Canada Abstract Attachment theory has become one of the most influential mod- els guiding -child relationships in programs of prevention, Introduction treatment, and education, including programs for Aboriginal par- Attachment theory has become one of the most ents. However, whether the model can be reliably applied when working with Aboriginal peoples has not yet been established. important conceptual schemes for understanding the Studies on attachment security conducted with different cultural early socio-emotional development of children (Cas- groups provide a means of comparing naturally occurring differ- sidy & Shaver, 1999; Crittenden & Claussen, 2000). ences in parenting practices and socio-emotional environments It has also become one of the most influential mod- of children. These studies report inconsistencies of attachment security across cultures and suggest that consideration should els guiding parent-child relationships in key areas be given to cultural differences when applying attachment theory such as daycare, child welfare, head start programs, across cultures. In this article, we analyse the correspondence hospitals, schools, and parenting programs. Equally, between attachment theory and descriptions of Aboriginal par- attachment theory has a central role as a model that enting and question the relevance of attachment theory to Ab- original who do not adhere to the mother- dyad as informs social work practice with Aboriginal parents the sole contributor to the child’s sense of security. even though the applicability of the model for work- ing with Aboriginal peoples has not been established. Despite this widespread of attachment the- This raises the question of whether Aboriginal parent- ory, research on the applicability of the model with ing practices are congruent with attachment theory. different cultural groups has been limited. By looking Since attachment theory is believed to have a at cultures that do not follow Western child rearing central role in , it has been widely practices, an opportunity emerges to examine natu- incorporated into programs dealing with parent-child rally occurring differences in parenting and socio- relationships. The role of attachment theory in guid- emotional environments of children that can clarify ing programming for parents is evident in the many the implications of these differences for attachment references to the theory given in the rationale and behaviours. The research that has been conducted, design of the programs (Aboriginal Head Start, 2006; which will be reviewed below, has suggested that McCain & Mustard, 1999; Rycus & Hughes, 1998). parenting practices which differ from Western norms Correspondence concerning this article may be addressed to: lead to inconsistent results in infant security. This sug- Raymond Neckoway gests that attachment security as a guiding concept for School of Social Work Aboriginal parents requires further analysis. Lakehead University In this paper we first provide a brief description 955 Oliver Road of the core ideas in attachment theory. Then, we ex- Thunder Bay, ON P7B 5E1 amine studies that have explored the consistencies (807) 343-8417 of attachment security across cultures, which have Electronic mail may be sent to: raymond.neckoway@ lakeheadu.ca raised questions about the cross-cultural applicability

65 Is Attachment Theory Consistent with Aboriginal Parenting Realities?

of the model. Next, we analyse the apparent consis- and uncertainty (Bowlby, 1973; Bretherton, 1985; tencies or inconsistencies between attachment theory Main & Hesse, 1992). The internal working model and descriptions of Aboriginal parenting. Finally, we is seen as a relatively fixed and lasting schema of the offer some suggestions for applying the model with accessibility and quality of the relationship with the Aboriginal parents. attachment figure (Bowlby, 1973). It has further been described as an unconscious blueprint of emotional Attachment development that has the potential to impact future Attachment theory has long been recognized as relationships (Morton & Browne, 1998). an important model for understanding individual de- Working from Bowlby’s premise that initial velopment. Attachment is regarded as significant in primary relationships between infant and shaping our capacity for interpersonal relationships, could provide insight into relational development, as well as, in the formulation of our view of the world and her colleagues (Ainsworth et al., and of others around us. (1969, 1973, 1978) laid the groundwork that extended the concept 1980), a British psychoanalyst, is credited with devel- of attachment into a phenomenon open to empirical oping attachment theory. Bowlby argued that attach- examination. In an attempt to further analyze and ment is biologically based and represents a child’s understand the intimate bonding exchange between instinctual need for a reliable, ongoing relationship mother and infant, Ainsworth began strategically with a primary caregiver and that if this attachment observing maternal responsiveness and sensitivity to was interrupted, lacking or lost, lasting emotional infant need, proposing that this was the crucial link damage could occur (Karen, 1994). Bowlby focused in the development of infant attachment. In a pivotal on the distress that tend to show when sepa- study, Ainsworth, Bell and Stayton (1974) demon- rated from their mother or the person with whom they strated that mothers’ responses to their children var- are emotionally bonded. Through his research, he ied widely, which in turn could be clearly shown to be identified a series of infant attachment behaviours, in- linked with the infants’ level of secure attachment. cluding crying, clinging, following and smiling, that he argued serve to keep the caregiver close at hand to The Procedure ensure the child’s safety and survival. He observed In her ongoing exploration of the fundamental that these attachment behaviours were invoked when components of attachment theory, Mary Ainsworth the distance from the mother (or attachment figure) et al. (1978) devised the strange situation procedure exceeded a certain threshold in time and/or space and (SSP) to test and observe individual differences in the infant sought to regain proximity. infant attachment behaviour. During this procedure, Bowlby argued that the responsive action or in- which optimally occurred with infants between action of the primary caretaker to these expressed twelve to twenty-four months of age, mothers and attachment needs formed the foundation of what babies were viewed in an unfamiliar but pleasant en- he termed the infant’s ‘internal working model’ – a vironment that invited exploration and . Through mental representation or belief about the ability and a series of brief episodic encounters, infant behaviour willingness of others around them to provide comfort was closely observed under varied conditions of and care (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978; : (a) when the mother was separated from the Bowlby, 1969). Thus, he proposed that attachment child (leaves the room) after a has entered figures who were able to promptly and consistently and begun to interact with the child and (b) when provide comfort and reassurance to an infant experi- the child was left alone (mother has left the room) encing distress laid the foundation for an optimal in- whereupon the stranger enters the room and interacts ternal working model that enabled the infant to view with the child. The infant’s responses to the reunion, the world as trusting and responsive and ultimately in upon the mother’s return on the two occasions of the terms of a sense of security. Conversely, procedure, were carefully assessed and soon became who were slow or unable to respond to an infant’s one of the focal points of the study of attachment expressed needs contributed a foundation for an in- behaviour. Based on the systematic observations of ternal working model that persuaded the infant to Ainsworth and her colleagues (1978), three catego- view the world through a lens clouded with mistrust ries of infant attachment behaviours were proposed.

66 First Peoples Child & Family Review, Volume 3, Number 2, 2007, Special Issue

Infant responses were labelled as Insecure – Anx- The classifications of infants’ secure and insecure ious/Avoidant (Type A), Secure (Type B), or Insecure attachment behaviours emerged as significant when – Anxious-Resistant or Ambivalent (Type C) infants. Ainsworth linked them to in home observations of the Secure – (Type B) infants displayed an optimal mother-infant pairs. From these initial observations level of exploration and caregiver affiliation during specific associations could be made between a moth- the pre-separation phase of the procedure, mild to er’s style of parenting and the infant’s attachment be- moderate wariness when the mother left the room and haviour (Karen, 1990). The infants that had received were easily comforted upon her return. It was noted a classification as securely attached had mothers that this group of infants protested or cried when that responded readily to their infant’s communica- separated from their mothers, but when their mother tion such as when they cried or otherwise expressed returned secure infants tended to greet her with plea- discomfort. These caregivers also reciprocated in- sure, often reaching out their arms to be picked up fants’ smiles with an affectionate response. These and were relatively easily consoled (Karen, 1990). observations regarding secure attachment confirmed Ainsworth’s central premise that a responsive or sen- Anxious-Avoidant – (Type A) infants were ob- sitive mother provides a secure base from which her served to be seemingly confident and independent, infant can explore the environment (Ainsworth et al., displayed a relative lack of distress when separated 1978; Ainsworth & Marvin, 1994) from their mother and avoidance behaviour upon her return. Avoidant infants seemed to be independent. In contrast, the mothers of infants labelled as inse- These infants would explore the new environment curely attached – avoidant (Type A) were found to be without seeming to rely on their mothers as a base, insensitive to their infant’s expressions of discomfort. and they did not engage in repeated checking on their These mothers also seemed to display a dislike for mother’s presence like the infants labelled secure. physical and showed little emotional respon- When the mother left the room, anxious-avoidant in- siveness towards their infant (Ainsworth et al., 1978; fants did not show any marked reaction and when she Bretherton & Waters, 1985). The mothers of infants returned they snubbed or avoided her (Karen, 1990). labelled as insecurely attached – resistant or ambiva- lent (Type C), on the other hand, demonstrated a clear Anxious-Resistant or Ambivalent – (Type C) inconsistency in responding to their children’s needs infants showed little interest in exploring their en- (Bretherton & Waters, 1985). Infants labelled as Dis- vironment; they became highly distressed when left organized (Type D), however, would appear to have alone or when in the presence of an unfamiliar experienced both inconsistent and abusive primary and could not be easily comforted by their mothers. relationships characterized by caregiver intrusive- Infants classified as ambivalent were mostly clingy ness and maltreatment, including physical abuse and from the beginning of the procedure and seemed psychological unavailability (Carlson, 1998; Sroufe, fearful about exploring the room on their own. These Egeland, Carlson, & Collins, 2005). infants showed a high level of agitation and became very tearful when separated from their caregiver. The patterns established by these attachment re- When the mother returned to the room ambivalent lationships are thought to become internalized by the infants sought contact with their mother, but also infant as an internal working model or set of beliefs arched away appearing to be angry and resisted ef- about what to expect of relationships and this internal forts at being soothed (Karen, 1990). model is regarded as stable and resistant to change (Cassidy & Shaver, 1999). Bowlby (1973) surmised A Disorganized/Disoriented – (Type D) category that the beliefs associated with the internal working was suggested by Main and Solomon (1986) to de- model persist throughout life. He hypothesized that scribe another group of infants who seemed to lack early attachment success provided a foundation for any coherence in their responses to the SSP. These healthy functioning in future relationships, whereas children were later found to be in abusive or trauma- failure to attach could hinder an individual’s abil- tizing mother-infant relationships that caused a mix- ity to form satisfactory relationships later in life ture of fearful and uncertain reactions that appeared and potentially lead to a variety of behavioural and disorganized and inconsistent (Bretherton, 1985; emotional difficulties. Much research has focused Main & Solomon, 1990). on this hypothesis that attachment security predicts

© Raymond Neckoway, Keith Brownlee, and Bruno Castellan 67 Is Attachment Theory Consistent with Aboriginal Parenting Realities?

subsequent behaviour and has tended to confirm an three core patterns that are identifiable which have a association between attachment types and behaviours bearing on attachment theory. First, parenting that is during infancy and , such as play and very involved and intensive in meeting infant needs exploration, autonomy and competence, peer relation- or what we have called ‘hypersensitive parenting.’ ships and psychopathology (Sroufe, Fox, & Pancake, Second, parenting that is less intensive in meeting 1983). infant’s and what we have called ‘selective parent- The idea that the internal working model is stable ing.’ Third, the involvement of multiple caregivers in and has an enduring effect arising from an individual’s a significant role in caring for the infant or what we secure or insecure level of attachment is a concept have called ‘.’ We will review stud- with far reaching implications. Thus, attachment pat- ies that have examined the association between these terns are measured by using the SSP and interpreted patterns of parenting in different cultures and attach- as secure or insecure. Lasting and stable internal ment security. working models are by definition a function of early parenting behaviours, with sensitive parenting leading Hypersensitive Parenting to the preferred outcome of the secure child. But what In Western culture the expectation is that a parent if the parenting does not follow attachment theory’s will respond sensitively to a child’s needs as a reaction ideal pattern, not because the mother or caregiver is to explicit signals from the child. In Japanese culture insensitive, but because the cultural context in which the expectation is different, a parent is expected to the child is raised promotes parenting practices that engage in a high level of emotional closeness and to are contrary, or at least, not consistent with the attach- anticipate a child’s needs rather than wait for a signal ment theory ideal. from the child (Rothbaum et al., 2000). The Japanese A number of researchers have pointed out that at- mother is encouraged to view the child as an exten- tachment theory makes assumptions, based on West- sion of herself (Bournstein et al., 1992), with close ern ideologies, regarding ideal dyadic relationships physical contact between the dyad, whereas American and preferred developmental outcomes based on the mothers “prefer more distal modes of interaction with mother-infant bond (Harwood, Miller, & Irizarry, their baby” (Vereijken, Riksen-Walraven, & Kondo- 1995; McShane & Hastings, 2004; Rothbaum, Weisz, Ikemura, 1997, p. 36). The aim in Japanese culture is Pott, Miyake, & Morelli, 2000). For instance, not to promote interdependence while in Western culture all cultures expect mothers to be the sole caregiver the aim is to promote independence of the child (Roth- (Bournstein et al., 1992) nor do all cultures inter- baum et al., 2000). Thus, Japanese parenting contrasts pret the child’s needs in the same way (Sagi, 1990) with what attachment theorists have described as sen- or have the same reactions to emotional expression, sitive responding (Ainsworth et al., 1974). Japanese such as the meaning of an infant’s cry (Harwood et mothers, according to the theory, could be labelled al., 1995). What must surely come into question then, hypersensitive (Gibson, Ungerer, McMahon, Leslie, is the universal applicability of attachment theory & Saunders, 2000). According to attachment theorists (van IJzendoorn, 1990). Although there are relatively this type of interaction could lead to insecurely at- few studies that have examined the consistency of tached infants, specifically anxious-resistant infants. attachment theory and attachment security across cul- Takahashi (1986) conducted a study with Japa- tures (van IJzendoorn & Sagi, 1999), there have been nese mothers and their infants using the SSP that studies that have allowed for comparisons between allowed this assumption to be examined. Takahashi cultures. In the next section we review studies that reported that 68% of the infants were assessed as hav- have examined the pattern of attachment security us- ing a secure attachment with their mother while 32% ing the SSP with parents and children from cultures were reported as having an anxious-resistant attach- where parenting practices differ from the normative ment. However, when Takahashi decided to classify sample from the USA. the infants on a modified SSP, where only the first five episodes were used and the infant was not left Thematic Analysis of Attachment alone in the room, the results were drastically altered. Although there are numerous cultural differ- She found that 83% of infants were rated securely ences in parenting that could be explored, there are attached and 17% were classified as having an anx-

68 First Peoples Child & Family Review, Volume 3, Number 2, 2007, Special Issue

ious-resistant attachment. Durett et al. (1984) studied tably surface about the reliability of the attachment 39 intact middle class living in Tokyo with concept across cultures. one-year-old infants and reported an attachment dis- tribution of 13% anxious-avoidant, 61% secure, 18% Shared Parenting anxious-resistant and 8% unclassifiable (cited by van The central focus of attachment theory has been IJzendoorn & Sagi, 1999). Durett et al’s distribution on the dyadic relationship between the infant and the of infant classifications is more consistent with global mother or primary caregiver. But since many cultures averages. What is noteworthy from the research with involve other family members or even community Japanese infants is the variety of attachment clas- members in significant parenting roles, these cultures sification distributions within the culture, the fact offer an opportunity to explore the implications of that none of the studies reported high levels of anx- shared parenting for attachment security. ious-avoidant attachment, and that all of the studies reported average or above average levels of securely The kibbutzim in Israel were collective farms attached infants. founded upon socialist principles of an equal sharing of responsibilities and rewards among community Selective Parenting members with no individual having greater hierarchal (social or economic) importance. These communities Ahnert and colleagues (Ahnert & Lamb, 2000, are unique in that they are the only cultural group that 2001; Ahnert, Lamb, & Seltenheim, 2000) have con- has adopted an arrangement where children sleep in a ducted a series of studies with German infants and separate location from their parents while being tend- parents which offered a unique opportunity to study ed at night by non-family members (van IJzendoorn cultural differences. These researchers were able to & Sagi, 1999). The intention of this arrangement was study parent child relationships before, during, and to socialize children for communal life and to create a after the reunification of East and West Germany. sense of group cohesion and, thus, people who could East Germany was known for its rigid child rearing socially and emotionally function within the commu- practices and valued child independence at an early nity. This would mean that if a secure attachment was age (Uhlendorff, 2004), which included children’s formed it would have been secondary to the core goal introduction to socially run daycare facilities. West of the community. Germany, in contrast, fostered a more nurturing and sensitive role on the part of mothers, with maternity Sagi, van IJzendoorn, Aviezer, Donnell, & May- leave being granted from employment for up to three seless (1994) conducted a study that compared 25 years to care for their children. Yet when the pattern family-based sleeping infants with 23 communal- of attachment security between East and West German based sleeping infants from a kibbutz. These authors infants was compared, the rates of secure attachment concluded that home-based infants had a higher rate were virtually identical at 49% and 50% respectively of secure attachment. The distribution of attachment (Ahnert & Lamb, 2001). Another result of interest was security among the home-based infants was 0% anx- the high rate of infants identified within the avoidant ious-avoidant, 60% secure, 8% anxious-resistant, and category from East Germany during all three time pe- 32% disorganized, whereas among the communal riods (before, during and after reunification), whereas infants it was 0% anxious-avoidant, 26% secure, and the West German infants assessed before and after 30% anxious-resistant, and 44% disorganized. Fur- reunification showed a higher than average classifi- thermore, the average rate of disorganized attachment cation in the disorganized category. The results from was 37%, almost reaching the rate of secure attach- the studies of post reunified Germany suggest that the ment of 44% with the kibbutz. These findings coin- culture is associated with a higher than average level cide with Sagi et al’s earlier study in 1985 concluding of infants classified with avoidant attachment (Ahnert that “41% of kibbutz infants were insecurely attached & Lamb, 2001). Given that anxious-avoidant attach- to their mothers” (Oppenheim, 1998, p. 80). Thus, the ment has typically been regarded as a rare form of form of shared parenting adopted by the kibbutzim attachment (True, Pisani, & Oumar, 2001) and that appeared to be associated with an overrepresentation German culture would appear to emphasize nurturing of infants classified as anxious-resistant and an under parenting with a Western orientation, questions inevi- representation of infants in the anxious-avoidant cat- egory (van IJzendoorn & Sagi, 1999).

© Raymond Neckoway, Keith Brownlee, and Bruno Castellan 69 Is Attachment Theory Consistent with Aboriginal Parenting Realities?

African cultures such as the Dogon, Efe, and Gu- Therefore, in this section we will review some of the sii also rely on multiple caregivers to maintain and descriptions of Aboriginal parenting that have ap- ensure child subsistence, although the degree and role peared in the literature and which may have a bearing of the caregiver is diverse among each culture. The on attachment theory. African cultures are known for feeding infants on de- Aboriginal cultures in Canada are similar to other mand, and keeping infants in close proximity. True’s cultures in that they cannot be viewed as homoge- (1994) study on the Dogon of Mali showed a high neous (Isajiw, 1999), rather they have characteristics percentage of disorganized infants (23%), a high rate specific to their geographic locations and social net- of secure infant attachment (69%), an absence of the works (Preston, 2002). Although there are differences anxious-avoidant classification, and an under-repre- between Aboriginal Peoples and differences within sentation of infants in the anxious-resistant category each People group in terms of culture, there are nev- (8%)(cited by van IJzendoorn & Sage, 1999). These ertheless some consistently reported generalizations, results were supported by another study conducted based upon observations and shared experience, that by True et al. (2001) with a sample of 42 infants in suggest Aboriginal parenting is often characterized by which they found the attachment distribution to be shared parenting (Red Horse, Lewis, Feit, & Decker, 67% secure, 8% anxious-resistant, 25% disorganized 1978) and selective parenting. and again an absent of the anxious-avoidant category. Similarly, Kermoian & Leiderman (1986) studied 26 Aboriginal families do not adhere to the linear Gusii infants ranging from 8 to 27 months in age, and sequence of the mother as the sole contributor to the reported 61% of the infants being classified as se- child’s physical and emotional well-being (Weaver & curely attached. Unfortunately these authors did not White, 1997). There is no pressure put on the sole identify the type of insecure attachment these infant relationship between mother and infant in most Ab- possess. Thus, when the SSP is used to assess attach- original cultures (Report of the Aboriginal Commit- ment security among children in these cultures, the tee, 1992). The ‘nuclear’ family of mother, father, and category distribution has similar outcomes, having children is considered a household within the family an over representation in one of the insecure groups (Red Horse, 1980). Aboriginal concepts of the fam- despite the fact that the cultures pride themselves on ily range from the concept, where sensitive parenting and instant responses to infant lineage and bloodlines are important, to the wider cues. view where clans, kin, and totems can include elders, leaders, and communities (Okpik, 2005; Red Horse, In summary, attachment theory argues that sensi- 1980). Hallowell (1955) observed this centripetal ten- tive caregiving leads to securely attached children. dency of Saulteaux (Ojibwe) structure where Yet in the above cross-cultural studies, where mater- people were continually included as part of the fam- nal sensitivity is thought to be high and the caregiving ily, regardless of bloodline. These members all share is nurturing, the rate of security is inconsistent with a collective responsibility for the caring and nurturing the sensitivity hypothesis. of the child (McShane & Hastings, 2004) and keep a watchful eye on young children in the community Aboriginal Parenting (Lame Deer & Erdoes, 1994). The bond between the Many descriptions and assertions of Aboriginal child and the parent and other caregivers in Aborigi- parenting exists in the literature (RCAP, 1996; Report nal culture, therefore, is multi-layered rather than dy- of the Aboriginal Committee, 1992; Report of the adic. The effect of these diverse, overlapping bonds First Nation’s Child and Family Task Force, 1993), is to create a dense network of relationships within however, there has been relatively little research that which sharing and obligations of mutual aid ensure has been conducted with Aboriginal Peoples analyz- that an effective safety net is in place (Brendtro & ing parenting practices (Gfellner, 1990) and even less Brokenleg, 1993). Attachment theory, in contrast, research related to attachment theory (Christensen concentrates on the linear relationship between the & Manson, 2001; McShane & Hastings, 2004). The mother and the infant and does not include in the the- authors could find no research that has examined the ory wider social relationships except to suggest that pattern of attachment security using the SSP with the mother infant relationship becomes a template parents and children from Aboriginal communities. for all future relationships (Lewis, 2005). Attachment

70 First Peoples Child & Family Review, Volume 3, Number 2, 2007, Special Issue

theory, therefore, does not fully reflect the reality of less vigilant and can have an expectation that some- an Aboriginal infant’s life and socialization experi- one will be available to attend to the infant’s signals ences. Additionally, the qualities that emerge from the and needs. The implication in terms of attachment mother-infant relationship do not necessarily transfer theory is that such practices by a mother would be to other relationships because the roles others play in considered insensitive and when assessed in the SSP the child’s life take on a different meaning. it is possible that the child would reflect an anxious- Children hold a special place in Aboriginal cul- avoidant pattern, when in fact the child’s behaviour tures. According to many Aboriginal traditions, chil- would be consistent with his or her social context. dren are gifts from the Creator (RCAP, 1996). This is a spiritually based view of the world and contains Summary and Conclusion a belief that everything will work out in the end, that The ability to capture an infant’s quality of rela- momentary struggles are no more than a temporary tionship to a caregiver using the SSP has been chal- tribulation or lessons in life that need to be learned. lenging because of the variety of contexts in which As a result, parents take a long term view of the child, families live and the roles play within a child’s which includes a sense of destiny, and which means life. The SSP has been modified to compensate over- that the parent’s role is not to shape and create be- stressed infants and those whose proximity seeking haviour but to provide a context for its expression. behaviours were not normally activated by the proce- This can also be seen in Aboriginal parents preferring dure. This raises the question of the extent to which non-verbal teaching and learning styles where they the SSP can be modified before it is unable to measure observe their children’s behaviours rather than inter- what was originally intended. vene (Letourneau, Hungler, & Fisher, 2005). Children van IJzendoorn (1993) acknowledges that infants are allowed to make many decisions because they are in multiple caregiving cultures can establish a network considered a person and free to explore their own of attachment relationships but the primary relation- environment (McPherson & Rabb, 2001). In com- ship is still with their mothers. Questions arise as to parison to Canadian mainstream parenting practices, what the distribution of attachment types should look Aboriginal values and parenting practices would be like when the measure is applied to many family con- interpreted as passive, permissive, and lacking control texts across numerous cultural groups. The larger is- of children’s behaviour (Hamilton & Sinclair, 1991). sue is whether attachment classification matters if the Kelso & Attneave (1981), for instance, comment- family in question sees the infant developing along ed on the role of emotional restraint in the parenting culturally expected goals. If families follow culturally practices of Aboriginal parents, which was considered congruent approaches to parenting, these may not be a traditional parenting style associated with the de- in accord with what attachment theory suggests. For mands of a nomadic life. Even though the traditional instance, in cultures with shared parenting practices, context has disappeared, the child rearing practice has such as Aboriginal families, it is less clear who should persisted. Hallowell’s (1955) experiences with living be included in parenting skills training. Therefore, with the Saulteaux on the east side of Lake Winni- the relevancy of attachment theory may apply only peg bear out this common practice. Dr. Claire Brant to those parents who are intent on developing certain (1990), a Mohawk psychiatrist, observed in his expe- characteristics within their children. riences with the Cree of James Bay, that the practice The larger social and historical context of Ab- of inhibiting aggression was a prevalent parenting original realities in Canada, such as colonization, strategy. The behaviour has been misinterpreted as residential schools and their lingering affects, racism, psychopathology and/or conflict suppression by clini- poverty, high rates of suicide, high rate of child wel- cians unaware of the cultural values that have shaped fare involvement, school dropout rates, etc. renders this behaviour. concerns about as potentially One of the cornerstones of attachment theory is trivial. Many of these social-historical forces have the emphasis on the mother’s ability to be sensitive to destroyed relationships that many Aboriginal families her infant’s signals or cues and responsive to the in- have tried to develop with their children. In these situ- fants needs. In a context of multiple caregivers living ations, it is important that attachment theory does not in the same household, the mother can afford to be get over-extended in application by addressing mater-

© Raymond Neckoway, Keith Brownlee, and Bruno Castellan 71 Is Attachment Theory Consistent with Aboriginal Parenting Realities?

nal-infant relationships while ignoring social forces fication. Infant Behavior and Development, 23(2), acting upon the family. Waters, Corcoran, & Anafarta 197-209. (2005) acknowledges the limited domain which at- Ainsworth, M. D. S., Bell, S., & Stayton, D. (1974). In- tachment theory addresses, that is, the secure base fant-mother attachment and social development: so- facet of specific relationships, usually the mother. cialization as a product of reciprocal responsiveness to Therefore, a broader context of attachment theory as signals. In M. P. Richards (Ed.), The integration of the it relates to different contexts is not only desirable but child into the social world (pp. 99-135). Cambridge: clearly necessary in order to promote understanding UK: Cambridge University Press. and avoid misperceptions. Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. Instead of continuing the focus of research on (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study universality, which some researchers consider moot of the strange situation. Hillsdale: NJ: Lawrence Erl- (Bretherton & Waters, 1985), others consider it un- baum Associates. resolved (LeVine & Miller, 1990), and yet others consider it inaccessible (Grossmann & Grossmann, Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Marvin, R. S. (1994). On the shap- 1990), researchers are now considering the issue ing of attachment theory and research: An interview of conditional strategies in parenting (Main, 1990). with Mary D. S. Ainsworth (Fall 1994). Monographs Recognizing that the selection of parenting strate- of the Society for Research in Child Development, 60 gies reflects cultural norms, conditional strategies (2/3), 2-21. are considered to be those parenting strategies that Bournstein, M. H., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Tal, J., Lude- are most useful given the mores and expectations of mann, P., Toda, S., Rahn, C. W., Pecheux, M.-G., Azu- a society. Accordingly, Bretherton (1995) has noted ma, H., & Vardi, D. (1992). Maternal responsiveness that to better explore cultural variations in attachment to infants in three societies: The United States, France, organization, attachment researchers need to develop and Japan. Child Development, 63(4), 808-821. ecologically valid, theory driven observational and Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Attachment (Vol. interview measures that are tailored to specific cul- 1). London: Pimlico. tures and based on deeper knowledge of parents’ and children’s culture-specific folk theories about family Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Separation: Anger relationships and attachment. In Aboriginal cultures and anxiety (Vol. 2). London: Pimlico. this would imply exploring extended family connec- Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Loss (Vol. 3). tions, clans and kinship systems and their influence London: Pimlico. and role in parenting. Brant, C. (1990). Native ethics and rules of behaviour. Ca- nadian Journal of Psychiatry, 35(August), 534-539. References Brendtro, L. K., & Brokenleg, M. (1993). Beyond the cur- Aboriginal Head Start. (2006). Aboriginal Head Start riculum of control. Journal of Emotional and Behav- (AHS) in Urban and Northern Communities, Program ioural Problems, 1(4), 5-11. overview: Public Health Agency of Canada. Bretherton, I. (1985). Attachment theory: Retrospect and Ahnert, L., & Lamb, M. E. (2000). Infant-care provider at- prospect. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Growing tachments in contrasting care settings II: Individual-ori- points of attachment theory and research (Vol. 50 (1/2), ented care after German reunification. Infant Behavior pp. 3-35): Monographs of the Society for Research in and Development, 23(2), 211-222. Child Development. Ahnert, L., & Lamb, M. E. (2001). The East German child Bretherton, I. (1995). A communication perspective on at- care system: Associations with caretaking and care- tachment relationships and internal working models. taking beliefs, and children’s early attachment and Monographs of the Society for Research in Child De- adjustment. American Behavioral Scientist, 44(11), velopment, 60 (2/3), 310-329. 1843-1863. Bretherton, I., & Waters, E. (Eds.). (1985). Growing points Ahnert, L., Lamb, M. E., & Seltenheim, K. (2000). Infant- of attachment theory and research (Vol. 50 (1-2, Serial care provider attachments in contrasting No. 209)): Monographs of the Society for Research in settings I: Group oriented care before German reuni- Child Development.

72 First Peoples Child & Family Review, Volume 3, Number 2, 2007, Special Issue

Carlson, E. A. (1998). A prospective longitudinal study of Kermoian, R., & Leiderman, P. H. (1986). Infant attach- attachment disorganization/disorientation. Child De- ment to mother and child caretaker in an East African velopment, 69(4), 1107-1128. community. International Journal of Behavioral De- Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.). (1999). Handbook of at- velopment, 9(4), 455-469. tachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications. Lame Deer, J. F., & Erdoes, R. (1994). Lame Deer: Seeker New York: The Guilford Press. of visions. New York: Washington Square Press. Christensen, M., & Manson, S. (2001). Adult attachment Letourneau, N. L., Hungler, K. M., & Fisher, K. (2005). as a framework for understanding mental health and Low-income Canadian Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal American Indian families. American Behavioral Sci- parent-child interactions. Child: Care, Health and De- entist, 44(9), 1447-1465. velopment, 31(5), 545-554. Crittenden, P. M., & Claussen, A. H. (Eds.). (2000). The LeVine, R. A., & Miller, P. M. (1990). Commentary. Hu- organization of attachment relationships: Maturation, man Development, 33, 73-80. culture, and context. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Uni- Lewis, M. (2005). The child and its family: The social net- versity Press. work model. Development, 48(1-2), 8-27. Gfellner, B. M. (1990). Culture and consistency in ideal Main, M. (1990). Cross-cultural studies of attachment or- and actual child-rearing practices: A study of Cana- ganization: Recent studies, changing methodologies, dian Indian and white parents. Journal of Comparative and the concept of conditional strategies. Human De- Family Studies, XXI(3), 413-423. velopment, 33, 48-61. Gibson, F. L., Ungerer, J. A., McMahon, C. A., Leslie, G. Main, M., & Hesse, E. (1992). Disorganized/disoriented I., & Saunders, D. M. (2000). The mother-child rela- infant behaviour in the strange situation: Lapses in tionship following in vitro fertilisation (IVF): Infant the monitoring of reasoning and discourse during the attachment, responsivity, and maternal sensitivity. parent’s adult attachment interview and dissociative Journal of Child & Psychiatry, 41(8), states. In J. Stevenson-Hinde, C. M. Parkes & S. D. 1015-1023. (Eds.), Attachment and . Rome: Gius, Grossmann, K. E., & Grossmann, K. (1990). The wider Laterza and Figli. concept of attachment in cross-cultural research. Hu- Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1986). Discovery of a new, inse- man Development, 33, 31-47. cure disorganized/disoriented attachment pattern. In T. Hallowell, A. I. (1955). Culture and experience. Philadel- B. Brazelton & M. Yogman (Eds.), Affective Develop- phia: University of Pennsylvania Press. ment in Infancy. Norwood: NJ: Ablex. Hamilton, A. C., & Sinclair, C. M. (1991). Report of the Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for identi- Aboriginal justice inquiry of Manitoba: The justice fying infants as disorganized/disoriented during the system and Aboriginal people. Ainsworth strange situation. In D. Greenberg, D. Cic- Harwood, R. L., Miller, J. G., & Irizarry, N. L. (1995). chetti & E. M. Cummings (Eds.), Attachment in the Culture and attachment: Perceptions of the child in preschool years (pp. 121-160). Chicago: University of context. New York, NY: The Guilford Press. Chicago Press. Isajiw, W. W. (1999). Understanding diversity: Ethnicity McCain, M. N., & Mustard, J. F. (1999). The early years and race in the Canadian context. Toronto: Thompson study: Reversing the real brain drain. Toronto, ON: Press. Children’s Secretariat. Karen, R. (1990). Becoming attached. The Atlantic Month- McPherson, D., H., & Rabb, J. D. (2001). Indigeneity in ly, 35-70. Canada: Spirituality, the sacred and survival. Inter- national Journal of Canadian Studies, 23(Spring), Karen, R. (1994). Becoming attached: Unfolding the mys- 57-79. tery of the infant-mother bond and its impact on later life. New York: Warner Books. McShane, K. E., & Hastings, P. D. (2004). Culturally sensi- tive approaches to research on child development and Kelso, D., & Attneave, C. (1981). Bibliography of North family practices in first peoples communities. First American Indian mental health. Westport: CT: Green- Peoples Child and Family Review, 1(1), 38-44. wood Press.

© Raymond Neckoway, Keith Brownlee, and Bruno Castellan 73 Is Attachment Theory Consistent with Aboriginal Parenting Realities?

Morton, N., & Browne, K. D. (1998). Theory and observa- study of risk and from birth to adulthood. tion of attachment and its relation to child maltreat- New York, NY: Guilford Press. ment: A review. and Neglect, 22(11), Sroufe, L. A., Fox, N., & Pancake, V. (1983). Attachment 1093-1104. and dependency in developmental perspective. Child Okpik, A. (2005). We call it survival: Nunavut Arctic Development, 54(6), 1615-1627. College. Takahashi, K. (1986). Examining the strange situation Oppenheim, D. (1998). Perspectives on infant mental procedure with Japanese mothers and 12-month-old health from Isreal: The case of changes in collective infants. , 22, 265-270. sleeping on the kibbutz. Infant Mental Health Journal, True, M. M., Pisani, L., & Oumar, F. (2001). Infant-mother 19(1), 76-86. attachment among the Dogon of Mali. Child Develop- Preston, R. (2002). Cree Narrative (2nd ed.). McGill: ment, 72(5), 1451-1466. Queen’s University Press. Uhlendorff, H. (2004). After the wall: Parental attitudes to RCAP. (1996). The Royal Commission on Aboriginal child rearing in East and West Germany. International Peoples. Ottawa: Canada. Journal of Behavioral Development, 28(1), 71-82. Red Horse, J. G. (1980). Family structure and value ori- van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1990). Developments in Cross- entation in American Indians. Social Casework, 61(8), Cultural Research on Attachment: Some Methodologi- 462-467. cal Notes. Human Development, 33, 3-9. Red Horse, J. G., Lewis, R., Feit, M., & Decker, J. (1978). van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1993). Commentary. Human De- Family behaviour of urban American Indians. Social velopment, 33, 3-9. Casework, 59(2), 67-72. van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Sagi, A. (1999). Cross-cultural Report of the Aboriginal Committee. (1992). Liberating patterns of attachment: Universal and contextual di- our children: Liberating our nations. Vancouver, BC: mensions. In J. Cassidy & S. P. R. (Eds.), Handbook of Community Panel Family and Children’s Services attachment: Theory, research and clinical applications Legislation Review in British Columbia. (pp. 713-734). New York: The Guilford Press. Report of the First Nation’s Child and Family Task Force. Vereijken, C. M. J. L., Riksen-Walraven, J. M., & Kondo- (1993). Children first: Our responsibility: Assembly of Ikemura, K. (1997). Maternal sensitivity and infant Manitoba Chiefs, Department of Indian and Northern attachment security in Japan: A longitudinal study. Affairs, Manitoba Minister of Family. International Journal of Behavioral Development, Rothbaum, F., Weisz, J., Pott, M., Miyake, K., & Mo- 21(1), 35-49. relli, G. (2000). Attachment and culture: Security in Waters, E., Corcoran, D., & Anafarta, M. (2005). Attach- the United States and Japan. American Psychologist, ment, other relationships, and the theory that all good 55(10), 1093-1104. things go together. Human Development, 48, 80-84. Rycus, J. S., & Hughes, R. C. (1998). Field guide to chld Weaver, H., & White, B. J. (1997). The Native American welfare, Volume 3: Child development and child family circle: Roots of resiliency. Journal of Family welfare. Washington, DC: Child Welfare League of Social Work, 2(1), 67-80. America. Sagi, A. (1990). Attachment theory and research from a cross-cultural perspective. Human Development, 33, 10-22. Sagi, A., van Ijendoorn, M. V., Aviezer, O., Donnell, F., & Mayseless, O. (1994). Sleeping out of home in a kib- butz communal arrangement: It makes a difference for infant-mother attachment. Child Development, 65(4), 992-1004. Sroufe, A. L., Egeland, B., Carlson, E. A., & Collins, W. A. (2005). The development of the person: The Minnesota 74