Is Attachment Theory Consistent with Aboriginal Parenting Realities?

Is Attachment Theory Consistent with Aboriginal Parenting Realities?

First Peoples Child & Family Review A Journal on Innovation and Best Practices in Aboriginal Child Welfare Administration, Research, Policy & Practice Volume 3, Number 2, 2007, Special Issue, pp. 65-74 Is Attachment Theory Consistent with Aboriginal Parenting Realities? Raymond Neckowaya, Keith Brownleea, and Bruno Castellana a School of Social Work, Lakehead University, ThunderBay, ON, Canada Abstract Attachment theory has become one of the most influential mod- els guiding parent-child relationships in programs of prevention, Introduction treatment, and education, including programs for Aboriginal par- Attachment theory has become one of the most ents. However, whether the model can be reliably applied when working with Aboriginal peoples has not yet been established. important conceptual schemes for understanding the Studies on attachment security conducted with different cultural early socio-emotional development of children (Cas- groups provide a means of comparing naturally occurring differ- sidy & Shaver, 1999; Crittenden & Claussen, 2000). ences in parenting practices and socio-emotional environments It has also become one of the most influential mod- of children. These studies report inconsistencies of attachment security across cultures and suggest that consideration should els guiding parent-child relationships in key areas be given to cultural differences when applying attachment theory such as daycare, child welfare, head start programs, across cultures. In this article, we analyse the correspondence hospitals, schools, and parenting programs. Equally, between attachment theory and descriptions of Aboriginal par- attachment theory has a central role as a model that enting and question the relevance of attachment theory to Ab- original parents who do not adhere to the mother-infant dyad as informs social work practice with Aboriginal parents the sole contributor to the child’s sense of security. even though the applicability of the model for work- ing with Aboriginal peoples has not been established. Despite this widespread adoption of attachment the- This raises the question of whether Aboriginal parent- ory, research on the applicability of the model with ing practices are congruent with attachment theory. different cultural groups has been limited. By looking Since attachment theory is believed to have a at cultures that do not follow Western child rearing central role in child development, it has been widely practices, an opportunity emerges to examine natu- incorporated into programs dealing with parent-child rally occurring differences in parenting and socio- relationships. The role of attachment theory in guid- emotional environments of children that can clarify ing programming for parents is evident in the many the implications of these differences for attachment references to the theory given in the rationale and behaviours. The research that has been conducted, design of the programs (Aboriginal Head Start, 2006; which will be reviewed below, has suggested that McCain & Mustard, 1999; Rycus & Hughes, 1998). parenting practices which differ from Western norms Correspondence concerning this article may be addressed to: lead to inconsistent results in infant security. This sug- Raymond Neckoway gests that attachment security as a guiding concept for School of Social Work Aboriginal parents requires further analysis. Lakehead University In this paper we first provide a brief description 955 Oliver Road of the core ideas in attachment theory. Then, we ex- Thunder Bay, ON P7B 5E1 amine studies that have explored the consistencies (807) 343-8417 of attachment security across cultures, which have Electronic mail may be sent to: raymond.neckoway@ lakeheadu.ca raised questions about the cross-cultural applicability 65 Is Attachment Theory Consistent with Aboriginal Parenting Realities? of the model. Next, we analyse the apparent consis- and uncertainty (Bowlby, 1973; Bretherton, 1985; tencies or inconsistencies between attachment theory Main & Hesse, 1992). The internal working model and descriptions of Aboriginal parenting. Finally, we is seen as a relatively fixed and lasting schema of the offer some suggestions for applying the model with accessibility and quality of the relationship with the Aboriginal parents. attachment figure (Bowlby, 1973). It has further been described as an unconscious blueprint of emotional Attachment development that has the potential to impact future Attachment theory has long been recognized as relationships (Morton & Browne, 1998). an important model for understanding individual de- Working from Bowlby’s premise that initial velopment. Attachment is regarded as significant in primary relationships between infant and caregiver shaping our capacity for interpersonal relationships, could provide insight into relational development, as well as, in the formulation of our view of the world Mary Ainsworth and her colleagues (Ainsworth et al., and of others around us. John Bowlby (1969, 1973, 1978) laid the groundwork that extended the concept 1980), a British psychoanalyst, is credited with devel- of attachment into a phenomenon open to empirical oping attachment theory. Bowlby argued that attach- examination. In an attempt to further analyze and ment is biologically based and represents a child’s understand the intimate bonding exchange between instinctual need for a reliable, ongoing relationship mother and infant, Ainsworth began strategically with a primary caregiver and that if this attachment observing maternal responsiveness and sensitivity to was interrupted, lacking or lost, lasting emotional infant need, proposing that this was the crucial link damage could occur (Karen, 1994). Bowlby focused in the development of infant attachment. In a pivotal on the distress that infants tend to show when sepa- study, Ainsworth, Bell and Stayton (1974) demon- rated from their mother or the person with whom they strated that mothers’ responses to their children var- are emotionally bonded. Through his research, he ied widely, which in turn could be clearly shown to be identified a series of infant attachment behaviours, in- linked with the infants’ level of secure attachment. cluding crying, clinging, following and smiling, that he argued serve to keep the caregiver close at hand to The Strange Situation Procedure ensure the child’s safety and survival. He observed In her ongoing exploration of the fundamental that these attachment behaviours were invoked when components of attachment theory, Mary Ainsworth the distance from the mother (or attachment figure) et al. (1978) devised the strange situation procedure exceeded a certain threshold in time and/or space and (SSP) to test and observe individual differences in the infant sought to regain proximity. infant attachment behaviour. During this procedure, Bowlby argued that the responsive action or in- which optimally occurred with infants between action of the primary caretaker to these expressed twelve to twenty-four months of age, mothers and attachment needs formed the foundation of what babies were viewed in an unfamiliar but pleasant en- he termed the infant’s ‘internal working model’ – a vironment that invited exploration and play. Through mental representation or belief about the ability and a series of brief episodic encounters, infant behaviour willingness of others around them to provide comfort was closely observed under varied conditions of and care (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978; stress: (a) when the mother was separated from the Bowlby, 1969). Thus, he proposed that attachment child (leaves the room) after a stranger has entered figures who were able to promptly and consistently and begun to interact with the child and (b) when provide comfort and reassurance to an infant experi- the child was left alone (mother has left the room) encing distress laid the foundation for an optimal in- whereupon the stranger enters the room and interacts ternal working model that enabled the infant to view with the child. The infant’s responses to the reunion, the world as trusting and responsive and ultimately in upon the mother’s return on the two occasions of the terms of a sense of security. Conversely, caregivers procedure, were carefully assessed and soon became who were slow or unable to respond to an infant’s one of the focal points of the study of attachment expressed needs contributed a foundation for an in- behaviour. Based on the systematic observations of ternal working model that persuaded the infant to Ainsworth and her colleagues (1978), three catego- view the world through a lens clouded with mistrust ries of infant attachment behaviours were proposed. 66 First Peoples Child & Family Review, Volume 3, Number 2, 2007, Special Issue Infant responses were labelled as Insecure – Anx- The classifications of infants’ secure and insecure ious/Avoidant (Type A), Secure (Type B), or Insecure attachment behaviours emerged as significant when – Anxious-Resistant or Ambivalent (Type C) infants. Ainsworth linked them to in home observations of the Secure – (Type B) infants displayed an optimal mother-infant pairs. From these initial observations level of exploration and caregiver affiliation during specific associations could be made between a moth- the pre-separation phase of the procedure, mild to er’s style of parenting and the infant’s attachment be- moderate wariness when the mother left the room and haviour (Karen, 1990). The infants that had received were easily comforted upon her return. It was noted a classification as securely attached

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    10 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us