Deep Learning: What’S All the Fuss About?

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Deep Learning: What’S All the Fuss About? Deep Learning: What’s All the Fuss About? Garrison W. Cottrell Gary's Unbelievable Research Unit (GURU) The Perceptual Expertise Network The Temporal Dynamics of Learning Center Computer Science and Engineering Department, UCSD 2/5/20 PACS-2017 1 Or, a Shallow Introduction to Deep Learning! Garrison W. Cottrell Gary's Unbelievable Research Unit (GURU) The Perceptual Expertise Network The Temporal Dynamics of Learning Center Computer Science and Engineering Department, UCSD 2/5/20 PACS-2017 2 Outline ! What is Deep Learning? ! Why is it interesting? ! What can it do? 2/5/20 PACS-2017 3 What is Deep Learning? ! Deep Learning refers to Deep Neural Networks – What’s a neural network? – What’s a deep neural network? 2/5/20 PACS-2017 4 What’s a neural network? ! A neural network is a kind of computational model inspired by the brain ! It consists of “units” connected by weighted links ! Here’s a simple neural network, called a perceptron: 2/5/20 PACS-2017 5 Perceptrons: A bit of history Frank Rosenblatt studied a simple version of a neural net called a perceptron: – A single layer of processing – Binary output – Can compute simple things like (some) boolean functions (OR, AND, etc.) PACS-2017 6 Perceptrons: A bit of history ! Computes the weighted sum of its inputs (the net input), compares it to a threshold, and “fires” if the net is greater than or equal than the threshold. ! Why is this a “neural” network? – Because we can think of the input units here as “neurons” that are spreading activation to the output unit via synaptic connections – like real neurons do. PACS-2017 7 The Perceptron Activation Rule output net input This is called a binary threshold unit PACS-2017 8 The Perceptron Activation Rule output net input This is called a binary threshold unit PACS-2017 9 Quiz!!! X1 X2 OR(X1,X2) 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 0 1 1 1 1 Assume: FALSE == 0, TRUE==1, so if X1 is false, it is 0. Can you come up with a set of weights and a threshold so that a two-input perceptron computes OR? PACS-2017 10 Quiz X1 X2 AND(X1,X2) 0 0 0 0 1 0 1 0 0 1 1 1 Assume: FALSE == 0, TRUE==1 Can you come up with a set of weights and a threshold so that a two-input perceptron computes AND? PACS-2017 11 Quiz X1 X2 XOR(X1,X2) 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 0 1 1 1 0 Assume: FALSE == 0, TRUE==1 Can you come up with a set of weights and a threshold so that a two-input perceptron computes XOR? PACS-2017 12 Perceptrons The goal was to make a neurally-inspired machine that could categorize inputs – and learn to do this from examples via supervised learning: The network is presented with many input-output examples, and the weights are adjusted by a learning rule: Wi(t+1) = Wi(t) + α*(target - output)*Xi (the target is given by the example, α is the learning rate) This rule is called the delta rule, because the weights are changed according to the delta – the difference between what the output and the target are. PACS-2017 13 How the delta rule works Wi(t+1) = Wi(t) + α*(target - output)*Xi So, the network is presented with an input, it computes the output, and this is compared with the target. Notice that if the output and the target are the same, nothing happens – there is no change to the weight. If the output is 0 and the target is 1, and Xi is 1, then the weight is raised – which will tend to make the output 1 next time. If the output is 1 and the target is 0, and Xi is 1, then the weight is lowered– which will tend to make the output 0 next time. PACS-2017 14 Problems with perceptrons ! The learning rule comes with a great guarantee: anything a perceptron can compute, it can learn to compute. ! Problem: Lots of things were not computable, e.g., XOR (Minsky & Papert, 1969) ! Minsky & Papert said: – if you had hidden units, you could compute any boolean function. – But no learning rule exists for such multilayer networks, and we don’t think one will ever be discovered. Back propagation, 25 years 15 later Problems with perceptrons Back propagation, 25 years 16 later XOR: The smallest “hard problem” Notice that the network could not do XOR. If there is another layer (here, just one unit!) between the input and the output, then the network can compute XOR: PACS-2017 17 XOR: The smallest “hard problem” Notice that the network could not do XOR. If there is another layer between the input and the output, then the network can compute XOR: Without the middle unit, the rest of the network just computes OR. OR PACS-2017 18 XOR: The smallest “hard problem” Notice that the network could not do XOR. If there is another layer between the input and the output, then the network can compute XOR: OR is “right” in 3 out of four cases for XOR: X1 X2 OR XOR (X1,X2) (X1,X2) 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 1 0 1 1 1 1 1 0 OR PACS-2017 19 XOR: The smallest “hard problem” Notice that the network could not do XOR. If there is another layer between the input and the output, then the network can compute XOR: The middle unit computes “AND” of the inputs and turns the output off just for the one exception to OR: the 1,1 case! AND PACS-2017 20 Multi-layer neural networks Ok, we’ve established that: 1. The simplest neural networks have a single layer of processing 2. By adding one more layer, networks can compute harder problems. 3. The AND unit can be thought of as a feature that is useful for solving the task Now, backpropagation learning generalizes the delta rule to multi-layer networks – it can be used to learn the AND feature! PACS-2017 21 Aside about perceptrons ! They didn’t have hidden units - but Rosenblatt assumed nonlinear preprocessing! ! Hidden units compute features of the input ! The nonlinear preprocessing is a way to choose features by hand. ! Support Vector Machines essentially do this in a principled way, followed by a (highly sophisticated) perceptron learning algorithm. Back propagation, 25 years 22 later Enter Rumelhart, Hinton, & Williams (1985) ! (Re-)Discovered a learning rule for networks with hidden units. ! Works a lot like the perceptron algorithm: – Randomly choose an input-output pattern – present the input, let activation propagate through the network – give the teaching signal – propagate the error back through the network (hence the name back propagation) – change the connection strengths according to the error Back propagation, 25 years 23 later Enter Rumelhart, Hinton, & Williams (1985) OUTPUTS . Hidden Units Activation Error . INPUTS ! The actual algorithm uses the chain rule of calculus to go downhill in an error measure with respect to the weights Back propagation, 25 years ! The hidden units must learn features24 that solve the problem later XOR Back Propagation Learning OR AND Random Network XOR Network Here, the hidden units learned AND and OR - two features that when combined appropriately, can solve the problem Back propagation, 25 years 25 later XOR Back Propagation Learning OR AND Random Network XOR Network But, depending on initial conditions, there are an infinite number of ways to do XOR - backprop can surprise you with innovative solutions. Back propagation, 25 years 26 later Why is/was this wonderful? 1. Learns internal representations 2. Learns internal representations 3. Learns internal representations ! Generalizes to recurrent networks Back propagation, 25 years 27 later Hinton’s Family Trees example ! Idea: Learn to represent relationships between people that are encoded in a family tree: Back propagation, 25 years 28 later Hinton’s Family Trees example ! Idea 2: Learn distributed representations of concepts: localist outputs Learn: features of these entities useful for solving the task Input: localist people localist relations Localist: one unit “ON” to represent each item Back propagation, 25 years 29 later People hidden units: Hinton diagram ! What does the unit 1 encode? What is unit 1 encoding? Back propagation, 25 years 30 later People hidden units: Hinton diagram ! What does unit 2 encode? What is unit 2 encoding? Back propagation, 25 years 31 later People hidden units: Hinton diagram ! Unit 6? What is unit 6 encoding? Back propagation, 25 years 32 later People hidden units: Hinton diagram When all three are on, these units pick out Christopher and Penelope: Back propagation, 25 years Other combinations pick out other parts of33 the trees later Relation units What does the lower middle one code? Back propagation, 25 years 34 later Lessons ! The network learns features in the service of the task - i.e., it learns features on its own. ! This is useful if we don’t know what the features ought to be. ! The networks have been used in my lab for years to explain some human phenomena Back propagation, 25 years 35 later Switch to Demo ! This demo is downloadable from my website under “Resources” ! About the middle of the page: ! “A matlab neural net demo with face processing.” Back propagation, 25 years 36 later Multi-layer neural networks Why is this interesting? Because now we can train neural networks to do very interesting tasks – like face recognition, object recognition, handwriting recognition, etc. Thus there was a lot of excitement about neural nets when the backpropagation learning rule was (re-)discovered in 1985.
Recommended publications
  • Generative Linguistics and Neural Networks at 60: Foundation, Friction, and Fusion*
    Generative linguistics and neural networks at 60: foundation, friction, and fusion* Joe Pater, University of Massachusetts Amherst October 3, 2018. Abstract. The birthdate of both generative linguistics and neural networks can be taken as 1957, the year of the publication of foundational work by both Noam Chomsky and Frank Rosenblatt. This paper traces the development of these two approaches to cognitive science, from their largely autonomous early development in their first thirty years, through their collision in the 1980s around the past tense debate (Rumelhart and McClelland 1986, Pinker and Prince 1988), and their integration in much subsequent work up to the present. Although this integration has produced a considerable body of results, the continued general gulf between these two lines of research is likely impeding progress in both: on learning in generative linguistics, and on the representation of language in neural modeling. The paper concludes with a brief argument that generative linguistics is unlikely to fulfill its promise of accounting for language learning if it continues to maintain its distance from neural and statistical approaches to learning. 1. Introduction At the beginning of 1957, two men nearing their 29th birthdays published work that laid the foundation for two radically different approaches to cognitive science. One of these men, Noam Chomsky, continues to contribute sixty years later to the field that he founded, generative linguistics. The book he published in 1957, Syntactic Structures, has been ranked as the most influential work in cognitive science from the 20th century.1 The other one, Frank Rosenblatt, had by the late 1960s largely moved on from his research on perceptrons – now called neural networks – and died tragically young in 1971.
    [Show full text]
  • Neural Networks and the Distorted Automation of Intelligence As Statistical Inference Matteo Pasquinelli
    JOURNAL > SITE 1: LOGIC GATE, THE POLITICS OF THE ARTIFACTUAL MIND | 2017 Machines that Morph Logic: Neural Networks and the Distorted Automation of Intelligence as Statistical Inference Matteo Pasquinelli Perceptrons [artificial neural networks] are not intended to serve as detailed copies of any actual nervous system. They are simplified networks, designed to permit the study of lawful relationships between the organization of a nerve net, the organization of its environment, and the “psychological” performances of which the network is capable. Perceptrons might actually correspond to parts of more extended networks in biological systems… More likely, they represent extreme simplifications of the central nervous system, in which some properties are exaggerated, others suppressed. —Frank Rosenblatt1 No algorithm exists for the metaphor, nor can a metaphor be produced by means of a computer’s precise instructions, no matter what the volume of organized information to be fed in. —Umberto Eco2 The term Artificial Intelligence is often cited in popular press as well as in art and philosophy circles as an alchemic talisman whose functioning is rarely explained. The hegemonic paradigm to date (also crucial to the automation of labor) is not based on GOFAI (Good Old-Fashioned Artificial Intelligence that never succeeded at automating symbolic deduction), but on the neural networks designed by Frank Rosenblatt back in 1958 to automate statistical induction. The text highlights the role of logic gates in the Machines that Morph Logic: Neural Networks and the Distorted Automation of Intelligence as Statistical Inference | Matteo Pasquinelli distributed architecture of neural networks, in which a generalized control loop affects each node of computation to perform pattern recognition.
    [Show full text]
  • Arxiv:2012.03642V1 [Cs.LG] 7 Dec 2020 Results for Learning Sparse Perceptrons and Compare the Results to Those Obtained from Subgradient- Based Methods
    OPT2020: 12th Annual Workshop on Optimization for Machine Learning Generalised Perceptron Learning Xiaoyu Wang [email protected] Department of Applied Mathematics and Theoretical Physics, University of Cambridge Martin Benning [email protected] School of Mathematical Sciences, Queen Mary University of London Abstract We present a generalisation of Rosenblatt’s traditional perceptron learning algorithm to the class of proximal activation functions and demonstrate how this generalisation can be interpreted as an incremental gradient method applied to a novel energy function. This novel energy function is based on a generalised Bregman distance, for which the gradient with respect to the weights and biases does not require the differentiation of the activation function. The interpretation as an energy minimisation algorithm paves the way for many new algorithms, of which we explore a novel variant of the iterative soft-thresholding algorithm for the learning of sparse perceptrons. Keywords: Perceptron, Bregman distance, Rosenblatt’s learning algorithm, Sparsity, ISTA 1. Introduction In this work, we consider the problem of training perceptrons with (potentially) non-smooth ac- tivation functions. Standard training procedures such as subgradient-based methods often have undesired properties such as potentially slow convergence [2,6, 18]. We revisit the training of an artificial neuron [14] and further show that Rosenblatt’s perceptron learning algorithm can be viewed as a special case of incremental gradient descent method (cf. [3]) with respect to a novel choice of energy. This new interpretation allows us to devise approaches that avoid the computa- tion of sub-differentials with respect to non-smooth activation functions and thus provides better handling for the non-smoothness in the overall minimisation objective during training.
    [Show full text]
  • Deep Learning: State of the Art (2020) Deep Learning Lecture Series
    Deep Learning: State of the Art (2020) Deep Learning Lecture Series For the full list of references visit: http://bit.ly/deeplearn-sota-2020 https://deeplearning.mit.edu 2020 Outline • Deep Learning Growth, Celebrations, and Limitations • Deep Learning and Deep RL Frameworks • Natural Language Processing • Deep RL and Self-Play • Science of Deep Learning and Interesting Directions • Autonomous Vehicles and AI-Assisted Driving • Government, Politics, Policy • Courses, Tutorials, Books • General Hopes for 2020 For the full list of references visit: http://bit.ly/deeplearn-sota-2020 https://deeplearning.mit.edu 2020 “AI began with an ancient wish to forge the gods.” - Pamela McCorduck, Machines Who Think, 1979 Frankenstein (1818) Ex Machina (2015) Visualized here are 3% of the neurons and 0.0001% of the synapses in the brain. Thalamocortical system visualization via DigiCortex Engine. For the full list of references visit: http://bit.ly/deeplearn-sota-2020 https://deeplearning.mit.edu 2020 Deep Learning & AI in Context of Human History We are here Perspective: • Universe created 13.8 billion years ago • Earth created 4.54 billion years ago • Modern humans 300,000 years ago 1700s and beyond: Industrial revolution, steam • Civilization engine, mechanized factory systems, machine tools 12,000 years ago • Written record 5,000 years ago For the full list of references visit: http://bit.ly/deeplearn-sota-2020 https://deeplearning.mit.edu 2020 Artificial Intelligence in Context of Human History We are here Perspective: • Universe created 13.8 billion years ago • Earth created 4.54 billion years ago • Modern humans Dreams, mathematical foundations, and engineering in reality. 300,000 years ago Alan Turing, 1951: “It seems probable that once the machine • Civilization thinking method had started, it would not take long to outstrip 12,000 years ago our feeble powers.
    [Show full text]
  • Neurons Spike Back: the Invention of Inductive Machines and the Artificial Intelligence Controversy Dominique Cardon, Jean-Philippe Cointet, Antoine Mazieres
    Neurons spike back: The Invention of Inductive Machines and the Artificial Intelligence Controversy Dominique Cardon, Jean-Philippe Cointet, Antoine Mazieres To cite this version: Dominique Cardon, Jean-Philippe Cointet, Antoine Mazieres. Neurons spike back: The Invention of Inductive Machines and the Artificial Intelligence Controversy. Réseaux, La Découverte, 2018, n°211 (5), pp.173. 10.3917/res.211.0173. hal-02190026 HAL Id: hal-02190026 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-02190026 Submitted on 21 Jul 2019 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. NEURONS SPIKE BACK The Invention of Inductive Machines and the Artificial Intelligence Controversy Dominique CARDON Jean-Philippe COINTET Antoine MAZIÈRES La Découverte | « Réseaux » 2018/5 n° 211 | pp. 173-220 ISSN 0751-7971 ISBN 9782348040689 To cite this article : Dominique Cardon, Jean-Philippe Cointet et Antoine Mazières, «Neurons spike back. The invention of inductive machines and the artificial intelligence controversy », Réseaux 2018/5 (n° 211), pp. 173-220. DOI 10.3917/res.211.0173 Neurons spike back - Réseaux n° 211/2018 ​ Abstract Since 2010, machine learning based predictive techniques, and more specifically deep learning neural networks, have achieved spectacular performances in the fields of image recognition or automatic translation, under the umbrella term of “Artificial Intelligence”.
    [Show full text]
  • Arxiv:2109.01517V1 [Cs.AI] 3 Sep 2021
    A BRIEF HISTORY OF AI: HOW TO PREVENT ANOTHER WINTER (A CRITICAL REVIEW) Amirhosein Toosi Andrea Bottino Department of Integrative Oncology Department of Computer and Control Eng. BC Cancer Research Institute Polytechnic University of Turin Vancouver, BC, V5Z 1L3 Turin, Italy 10129 [email protected] [email protected] Babak Saboury Eliot Siegel Department of Radiology and Imaging Sciences Department of Diagnostic Radiology and Nuclear Medicine National Institutes of Health University of Maryland School of Medicine Bethesda, MD 20892 Baltimore, MD 21201 [email protected] [email protected] Arman Rahmim Departments of Radiology and Physics University of British Columbia Vancouver, BC [email protected] September 9, 2021 ABSTRACT The field of artificial intelligence (AI), regarded as one of the most enigmatic areas of science, has witnessed exponential growth in the past decade including a remarkably wide array of applications, having already impacted our everyday lives. Advances in computing power and the design of sophisticated AI algorithms have enabled computers to outperform humans in a variety of tasks, especially in the areas of computer vision and speech recognition. Yet, AI’s path has never been smooth, having essentially fallen apart twice in its lifetime (‘winters’ of AI), both after periods of popular success (‘summers’ of AI). We provide a brief rundown of AI’s evolution over the course of arXiv:2109.01517v2 [cs.AI] 8 Sep 2021 decades, highlighting its crucial moments and major turning points from inception to the present. In doing so, we attempt to learn, anticipate the future, and discuss what steps may be taken to prevent another ‘winter’.
    [Show full text]
  • Curriculum Vitae
    Curriculum Vitae Geoffrey E. Hinton January 6, 2020 Citizenship: Canadian (also British) Address: Department of Computer Science University of Toronto 10 Kings College Road Toronto, Ontario, M5S 3G4 Canada Email: [email protected] Higher Education and Qualifications 1967 - 1970 Cambridge University, B.A. Hons (Experimental Psychology) 1972 - 1975 Edinburgh University, PhD. in Artificial Intelligence (awarded 1978) Professional Experience Jan 76 - Sept 78 Research Fellow Cognitive Studies Program, Sussex University, England Oct 78 - Sept 80 Visiting Scholar Program in Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego Oct 80 - Sept 82 Scientific Officer MRC Applied Psychology Unit, Cambridge, England Jan 82 - June 82 Visiting Assistant Professor Psychology Department, University of California, San Diego Oct 82 - June 87 Assistant Professor then Associate Professor Computer Science Department, Carnegie-Mellon University, Pittsburgh, USA Jul 87 - June 98 Professor Computer Science Department, University of Toronto, Canada Jul 98 - Sep 01 Founding Director of the Gatsby Computational Neuroscience Unit University College London, England Oct 01 - Professor Computer Science Department, University of Toronto, Canada (Became\University Professor" in 2006 then Emeritus in 2014) Mar 13 - Sep 16 Distinguished Researcher, Google (half-time). Oct 16 - VP and Engineering Fellow, Google (half-time). Jan 17 - Chief Scientific Adviser, Vector Institute (pro bono) 1 Professional Recognition Fellowships 2016 Honorary Foreign Member of the US National Academy of Engineering 2015 Honorary Foreign Member of the Spanish Real Academia de Ingenieria 2014 Distinguished Fellow, Canadian Institute for Advanced Research 2003 Honorary Foreign Member of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences 2003 Fellow of the Cognitive Science Society 1998 Fellow of the Royal Society 1996 Fellow of the Royal Society of Canada 1991 Fellow, Association for the Advancement of Artificial Intelligence 1987 Fellow, Canadian Institute for Advanced Research (1987-1998; 2004-2014) Awards 2019 ACM A.
    [Show full text]
  • Marvin Lee Minsky
    Marvin Lee Minsky Born August 9, 1927, New York; with John McCarthy, established the basic concepts of artificial intelligence; with Seymour Papert in 1969, solidified the computational limits of Rosenblatt's Perceptron. Education: BA, physics, Harvard University, 1950; PhD, Princeton University, 1954. Professional Experience: Lincoln Laboratory, 1957-1958; MIT: professor, mathematics, 1958-1961; professor, electrical engineering, 1958-present; director, Project MAC AI group, 1968-1970; director, Artificial Intelligence Laboratory, 1970-present. Honors and Awards: ACM Turing Award, 1969. Marvin Minsky has probably had more influence, or been a colleague of more people who have had influence, on the development of the concepts of artificial intelligence than any other single person. Over a period of 35 years he has probed most of the classic conjectures of the means to implement or model artificial intelligence. The Perceptron, conceived by Frank Rosenblatt in the mid- 1950s, consisted of a set of photocells and random wiring which was capable of “learning” to recognize specific shapes, such as letters of the alphabet, possibly resembling the brain. Minsky and Papert wrote a book on Perceptrons and were able to define their mathematical properties, thereby allowing them to define the limits of the concept. Minsky gave up this field of study; however, in the form of neural nets, the concept is receiving attention again in the 1990s. In the 1960s Minsky developed new mathematical ideas about artificial intelligence and applied his efforts towards pattern recognition and, eventually, vision. This led to the concept of “frames of information” and to the emergence of the ideas of “knowledge engineering.” Throughout this period he worked on robotic devices endowed with vision in the early 1970s.
    [Show full text]
  • Perceptrons: an Associative Learning Network
    Perceptrons: An Associative Learning Network by Michele D. Estebon Virginia Tech CS 3604, Spring 1997 In the early days of artificial intelligence research, Frank Rosenblatt devised a machine called the perceptron that operated much in the same way as the human mind. Albeit, it did not have what could be called a "mental capacity", it could "learn" - and that was the breakthrough needed to pioneer todayís current neural network technologies. A perceptron is a connected network that simulates an associative memory. The most basic perceptron is composed of an input layer and output layer of nodes, each of which are fully connected to the other. Assigned to each connection is a weight which can be adjusted so that, given a set of inputs to the network, the associated connections will produce a desired output. The adjusting of weights to produce a particular output is called the "training" of the network which is the mechanism that allows the network to learn. Perceptrons are among the earliest and most basic models of artificial neural networks, yet they are at work in many of todayís complex neural net applications. An Associative Network Model In order to understand the operations of the human brain, many researchers have chosen to create models based on findings in biological neural research. Neurobiologists have shown that human nerve cells have no memory of their own, rather memory is made up of a series of connections, or associations, throughout the nervous system. A response in one cell triggers responses in others which triggers more in others until these responses eventually lead to a so-called ìrecognitionî of the input stimuli.
    [Show full text]
  • The Emergence of Fuzzy Sets in the Decade of the Perceptron—Lotfi A
    mathematics Article The Emergence of Fuzzy Sets in the Decade of the Perceptron—Lotfi A. Zadeh’s and Frank Rosenblatt’s Research Work on Pattern Classification Rudolf Seising The Research Institute for the History of Science and Technology, Deutsches Museum, 80538 Munich, Germany; [email protected]; Tel.: +49-(0)-89-2179-298 Received: 25 May 2018; Accepted: 19 June 2018; Published: 26 June 2018 Abstract: In the 1950s, the mathematically oriented electrical engineer, Lotfi A. Zadeh, investigated system theory, and in the mid-1960s, he established the theory of Fuzzy sets and systems based on the mathematical theorem of linear separability and the pattern classification problem. Contemporaneously, the psychologist, Frank Rosenblatt, developed the theory of the perceptron as a pattern recognition machine based on the starting research in so-called artificial intelligence, and especially in research on artificial neural networks, until the book of Marvin L. Minsky and Seymour Papert disrupted this research program. In the 1980s, the Parallel Distributed Processing research group requickened the artificial neural network technology. In this paper, we present the interwoven historical developments of the two mathematical theories which opened up into fuzzy pattern classification and fuzzy clustering. Keywords: pattern classification; fuzzy sets; perceptron; artificial neural networks; Lotfi A. Zadeh; Frank Rosenblatt 1. Introduction “Man’s pattern recognition process—that is, his ability to select, classify, and abstract significant information from the sea of sensory information in which he is immersed—is a vital part of his intelligent behavior.” Charles Rosen [1] (p. 38) In the 1960s, capabilities for classification, discrimination, and recognition of patterns were demands concerning systems deserving of the label “intelligent”.
    [Show full text]
  • Connectionism: Past, Present, and Future Jordan B
    Connectionism: Past, Present, and Future Jordan B. Pollack Computer & Information Science Department The Ohio State University 1. Introduction Research efforts to study computation and cognitive modeling on neurally-inspired mechanisms have come to be called Connectionism. Rather than being brand-new, it is actually the rebirth of a research pro- gram which thrived from the 40's through the 60's and then was severely retrenched in the 70's. Connec- tionism is often posed as a paradigmatic competitor to the Symbolic Processing tradition of Arti®cial Intel- ligence (Dreyfus & Dreyfus, 1988), and, indeed, the counterpoint in the timing of their intellectual and commercial fortunes may lead one to believe that research in cognition is merely a zero-sum game. This paper surveys the history of the ®eld, often in relation to AI, discusses its current successes and failures, and makes some predictions for where it might lead in the future. 2. Early Endeavors: High Hopes and Hubris Before the explosion of symbolic arti®cial intelligence, there were many researchers working on mathematical models of intelligence inspired by what was known about the architecture of the brain. Under an assumption that the mind arises out of the brain, a reasonable research path to the arti®cial mind was by simulating the brain to see what kind of mind could be created. At the basis of this program was the assumption that neurons were the information processing primitives of the brain, and that reasonable models of neurons connected into networks would suf®ce. 2.1. McCullogh & Pitts The opening shot in neural network research was the 1943 paper by Warren S.
    [Show full text]
  • Machine Learning
    Graham Capital Management Research Note, September 2017 Machine Learning Erik Forseth1, Ed Tricker2 Abstract Machine learning is more fashionable than ever for problems in data science, predictive modeling, and quantitative asset management. Developments in the field have revolutionized many aspects of modern life. Nevertheless, it is sometimes difficult to understand where real value ends and speculative hype begins. Here we attempt to demystify the topic. We provide a historical perspective on artificial intelligence and give a light, semi-technical overview of prevailing tools and techniques. We conclude with a brief discussion of the implications for investment management. Keywords Machine learning, AI 1Quantitative Research Analyst 2Head of Research and Development 1. Introduction by the data they are fed—which attempt to find a solution to some mathematical optimization problem. There remains a Machine Learning is the study of computer algorithms that vast gulf between the space of tasks which can be formulated improve automatically through experience. in this way, and the space of tasks that require, for example, – Tom Mitchell (1997) reasoning or abstract planning. There is a fundamental divide Machine Learning (ML) has emerged as one of the most between the capability of a computer model to map inputs to exciting areas of research across nearly every dimension of outputs, versus our own human intelligence [Chollet (2017)]. contemporary digital life. From medical diagnostics to recom- In grouping these methods under the moniker “artificial in- mendation systems—such as those employed by Amazon or telligence,” we risk imbuing them with faculties they do not Netflix—adaptive computer algorithms have radically altered have.
    [Show full text]