Reflexivity, Indexicality and Names

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Reflexivity, Indexicality and Names Reflexivity, Indexicality and Names John Perry [In Direct Reference, Indexicality and Proposition Attitudes edited by Wofgang Kunne, Martin Anduschus, and Albert Newen. Stanford, CA: CSLI-Cambridge University Press, 1997.] 1 Introduction Consider: (1) “I am a computer scientist.” (said by David Israel) (2) “David is a computer scientist.” (said by someone who is referring to David Israel with “David”) It has been persuasively argued by David Kaplan and others that the proposition ex- pressed by statements like (1) is a singular proposition, true in just those worlds in which a certain person, David Israel, is a computer scientist. Call this proposition P . The truth of this proposition does not require that the utterance (1) occur, or even that Israel has ever said anything at all. Marcus, Donnellan, Kripke and others have persua- sively argued for a view of proper names that, put in Kaplan’s terms and applied to this example, implies that the proposition expressed by (2) is also simply P .1 The thesis that expressions of a certain category (names, indexicals, demonstra- tives, pronouns, descriptions, etc.) are referential 2holds that these expressions con- tribute the object to which they refer, rather than a mode of presentation of that object, to the propositions expressed by statements containing them. The thesis that indexicals and names are referential creates the challenge of ex- plaining the difference in cognitive significance between statements like (1) and (2), that express the same proposition[Wettstein, 1986]. The problem has two parts, which 1[Marcus, 1961], [Donnellan, 1970],[Kripke, 1980] 2David Kaplan’s term is directly referential. Kaplan has a precise concept of “directness” in mind, but unless one is focusing on his exact words, the term “directly” is likely to suggest that there is no semantic mechanism intervening between the expression and its referent. This is pretty clearly not the case with indexicals, as Kaplan’s own analysis shows; it may be more plausible for proper names. Using terminology introduces in Section 3 we can say: Kaplan’s language suggests that directly referential terms name,butwhat he really says is simply that they refer. 1 I’ll call “cognitive motivation” and “cognitive impact”. The problem of cognitive mo- tivation is that in many circumstances, the beliefs and desires that might lead a com- petent user of English to assert (1) differ from those that might lead one to assert (2). The problem of cognitive impact is that in many circumstances the beliefs and desires a competent listener would acquire (and would be expected to acquire) from hearing (1) differ from those she would acquire from hearing (2). If (1) and (2) express exactly the same proposition, why should this be so? Let’s look at our example in some detail. First note that differences in David’s beliefs might lead him to assert (1) rather than (2) or vice versa. Suppose David has amnesia. He might have figured out what his profession was, but not have remembered his name. Then he would be in a position to assert (1) but not (2). Or he might have fig- ured out that a certain person, David Israel, was a computer scientist, without realizing that he is that person. Then he would be in a position to assert (2) but not (1). In the normal case, in which David does not have amnesia, differences in desire could motivate him to assert (1) rather than (2), or vice versa. In normal circumstances, if Israel wants to tell someone his profession, he will use (1) and not (2). Suppose, for example, that David has made some interesting points following a paper by a guest lecturer at CSLI. After the session he chats with the speaker, who seems unsure that he can trust what David says about programming languages. It would be natural for David to say (1), and odd for him to say (2). He wants to reassure his interlocutor about the reliability of what he says about programming languages. (1) will do that; (2) might not. I’ll call this example Case 1. There are easily imaginable circumstances in which David’s desires would lead him to use (2) but not (1). Suppose a government bureaucrat has the job of identifying computer scientists, who will then be sent a letter offering a well-paid job building the Information Highway. The bureaucrat himself has a clear antipathy towards computer scientists. He reads David names from a list of people who work at SRI and asks if they were then or ever had been computer scientists. When he gets to “David Israel”, Israel might just say (2), figuring that the information that he was the person in question was simply irrelevant and would lead the bureaucrat to be rude to him. Here David would not want his interlocutor to know that he, the person being talked to, was a computer scientist but he would want him to know the individual who is designated by the token of “David Israel” on the list is a computer scientist. He thinks that he will pass on that information with (2). I’ll call this Case 2. Given that David knows that he is David Israel, his choice between (1) and (2) is based on the effect he wants his utterance to have on the beliefs of his interlocutor— what I am calling its “cognitive impact”. He thinks that if he utters (1), his interlocutor will realize that the person he is talking to is a computer scientist. If he utters (2), his interlocutor might not realize that, even if he believes what David says. In fact Israel is well-known as a computer scientist, but looks and acts more like a philosopher. A competent speaker who hears (1) may well be skeptical about what he hears, even though he readily assents to (2), or even asserts it himself, having read articles by Israel. How is this difference possible, if the propositions expressed by (1) and (2) are the same? 2 My thesis in this paper is that to understand the difference in cognitive significance between (1) and (2), we need to acknowledge several different kinds of content.The paper develops a line of argument put forward in the later papers of [Perry, 1993]. In those papers a distinction is made between reflexive and incremental content, and it is argued that the former is relevant to cognitive significance. This distinction is most intuitive in the case of indexicals. In this paper I show how the basic idea can be ex- tended to deal with proper names, without treating them as if they were indexicals. This requires examining the difference between reflexivity and indexicality, and explaining how the concept of reflexivity can be applied to expressions that are not indexicals. But I will start with indexicals, and then move to consider names. 2 Indexicals (A) captures most of what a competent English speaker knows about the word “I”: (A) A use u of “I” designates x iff x is the speaker of u. From this, we see that (1), will be true iff Q:Thereisanxsuch that i) x is the speaker of (1); ii) x is a computer scientist. Q is not the proposition expressed by (1); as we saw, that is P . The truth values of P and Q will agree in the actual world, but may disagree in other possible worlds. In the worlds in which Israel is a computer scientist, but does not utter (1), P is true and Q is false. This does not mean that Q is irrelevant to the cognitive significance of (1), however. Q gives us what a competent user of English who hears (1) will know, just on the basis of those facts, about its truth conditions. Someone who knows English and hears (1), but isn’t in a position to see who said it, would still realize that Q captured the conditions under which it was true. Such a person might express this knowledge by saying “This utterance is true, iff its speaker is a computer scientist.” The truth conditions he associates with (1) are reflexive in that Q is about the utterance (1) itself. Because he doesn’t know who the speaker is, he cannot “detach” the truth conditions of (1) from (1) itself. In contrast, someone who learns that Israel is the speaker of (1), can get at the truth conditions of (1) in a non-reflexive way: “This utterance is true iff Israel is a computer scientist.” I call this the incremental truth conditions of (1), given that Israel is the speaker. “Incremental” is used here in the sense of “additional”. Given that Israel is the speaker of u,whatmore hastobetrueforuto be true? Answer: Israel has to be a computer scientist. This is just the proposition P , the proposition expressed by both (1) and (2). The incremental truth conditions correspond to the ordinary philosophical concept of truth conditions; they are what is said by the speaker of the utterance. I’ll also call this “official content”. 3 The difference between Cases 1 and 2 above has to do with the mode of presenta- tion that Israel wanted his hearer to associate with the belief that he was a computer scientist. In Case 1, Israel wanted the hearer to recognize that the person spoken about was also the person speaking. The plan of understanding that he has in mind goes roughly like this: The hearer will perceive my utterance, and, as a competent speaker of En- glish will recognize that it is true iff the person who makes it is a computer scientist (Q). He will also see that I, the person in front of him and talking to him, is the speaker of the utterance.
Recommended publications
  • Indexicality
    Workshop proposal for the 54th Annual Meeting of the Societas Linguistica Europaea (National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, 31 August – 3 September 2021) Indexicality Convenors: Peter Juul Nielsen (University of Southern Denmark) & María Sol Sansiñena (University of Leuven) Proposal The concept of indexicality – originating in the semiotics of C.S. Peirce and his triad symbol, icon and index – has been applied in the description of a broad range of linguistic phenomena, from the internal workings of phonology and morphology via relations within syntactic constructions and lexical and grammatical elements designed to hook on to features outside the clause, to the choice of linguistic variants in social interaction. In morphology, indexicality has been employed to describe the semiotic function of bound allomorphs (Anttila 1975, Andersen 2008: 29, see also Andersen 2010, Carstairs-McCarthy 2001, Enger 2019). Case in languages such as Modern German have been analysed as having indexical meaning, as in (1), where the nominative, accusative and dative case index the ditransitive predicate empfehlen ‘recommend’ that assigns argument status and semantic roles to the case-marked DPs (Heltoft 2019: 154-155). (1) der Rechtsanwalt hat dem Klient-en den Börsenmakler empfohlen the.NOM attorney has the.DAT client-OBL the.ACC stockbroker recommended ‘the attorney recommended the client the stockbroker’ Deictic elements, e.g. personal pronouns and deictic temporal adverbs such as now, are a classic example of linguistic indexicality (shifters, cf. Jakobson 1957) as the encoded meaning of the deictic element points beyond the internal structure of the utterance for interpretation. The term indexicality is also well established in sociolinguistics in analyses of how linguistic choices may index aspects of speaker or addressee identity, such as in-group identification, gender etc.
    [Show full text]
  • On the Logic of Two-Dimensional Semantics
    Matrices and Modalities: On the Logic of Two-Dimensional Semantics MSc Thesis (Afstudeerscriptie) written by Peter Fritz (born March 4, 1984 in Ludwigsburg, Germany) under the supervision of Dr Paul Dekker and Prof Dr Yde Venema, and submitted to the Board of Examiners in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MSc in Logic at the Universiteit van Amsterdam. Date of the public defense: Members of the Thesis Committee: June 29, 2011 Dr Paul Dekker Dr Emar Maier Dr Alessandra Palmigiano Prof Dr Frank Veltman Prof Dr Yde Venema Abstract Two-dimensional semantics is a theory in the philosophy of language that pro- vides an account of meaning which is sensitive to the distinction between ne- cessity and apriority. Usually, this theory is presented in an informal manner. In this thesis, I take first steps in formalizing it, and use the formalization to present some considerations in favor of two-dimensional semantics. To do so, I define a semantics for a propositional modal logic with operators for the modalities of necessity, actuality, and apriority that captures the relevant ideas of two-dimensional semantics. I use this to show that some criticisms of two- dimensional semantics that claim that the theory is incoherent are not justified. I also axiomatize the logic, and compare it to the most important proposals in the literature that define similar logics. To indicate that two-dimensional semantics is a plausible semantic theory, I give an argument that shows that all theorems of the logic can be philosophically justified independently of two-dimensional semantics. Acknowledgements I thank my supervisors Paul Dekker and Yde Venema for their help and encour- agement in preparing this thesis.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Identity, Indexicality, and the Appropriation of Slurs
    Croatian Journal of Philosophy Vol. XVII, No. 50, 2017 Social Identity, Indexicality, and the Appropriation of Slurs KATHERINE RITCHIE City College of New York, CUNY, New York, USA Slurs are expressions that can be used to demean and dehumanize tar- gets based on their membership in racial, ethnic, religious, gender, or sexual orientation groups. Almost all treatments of slurs posit that they have derogatory content of some sort. Such views—which I call content- based—must explain why in cases of appropriation slurs fail to express their standard derogatory contents. A popular strategy is to take ap- propriated slurs to be ambiguous; they have both a derogatory content and a positive appropriated content. However, if appropriated slurs are ambiguous, why can only members in the target group use them to ex- press a non-offensive/positive meaning? Here, I develop and motivate an answer that could be adopted by any content-based theorist. I argue that appropriated contents of slurs include a plural fi rst-person pronoun. I show how the semantics of pronouns like ‘we’ can be put to use to explain why only some can use a slur to express its appropriated content. More- over, I argue that the picture I develop is motivated by the process of appropriation and helps to explain how it achieves its aims of promoting group solidarity and positive group identity. Keywords: Slurs, appropriation, reclamation, indexicals, social groups. Slurs are expressions that can be used to demean and dehumanize tar- gets based on their membership in social groups based on, e.g., race, * I thank audiences at the Dartmouth Mind and Language Workshop and the Dubrovnik Inter-University Center Philosophy of Language and Linguistics Workshop where I presented earlier versions of this paper for their helpful feedback.
    [Show full text]
  • The Limits of Meaning: Social Indexicality, Variation, and the Cline of Interiority
    The limits of meaning: Social indexicality, variation, and the cline of interiority Penelope Eckert Language, Volume 95, Number 4, December 2019, pp. 751-776 (Article) Published by Linguistic Society of America For additional information about this article https://muse.jhu.edu/article/743105 Access provided at 17 Dec 2019 23:13 GMT from Linguistic Society of America THE LIMITS OF MEANING: SOCIAL INDEXICALITY, VARIATION, AND THE CLINE OF INTERIORITY Penelope Eckert Stanford University The structural focus of linguistics has led to a static and modular treatment of meaning. View - ing language as practice allows us to transcend the boundaries of subdisciplines that deal with meaning and to integrate the social indexicality of variation into this larger system. This article presents the expression of social meaning as a continuum of decreasing reference and increasing performativity, with sociolinguistic variation at the performative extreme. The meaning potential of sociolinguistic variables in turn is based in their form and their social source, constituting a cline of ‘interiority’ from variables that index public social facts about the speaker to more inter - nal, personal affective states.* Keywords : variation, social meaning, semantics, pragmatics, iconicity, indexicality, semiotics ‘I have resisted the term sociolinguistics for many years, since it implies that there can be a successful linguistic theory or practice which is not social.’ (Labov 1972:13) 1. Introduction . Language is a social practice, a dialectic between structure and agency: structure constrains action, and action in turn reproduces structure. As Giddens (1984:2) puts it, ‘In and through their activities agents reproduce the conditions that make these activities possible’.
    [Show full text]
  • A Changing Cinema: Re-Conceptualizing Indexicality and Digital Technologies
    A Changing Cinema: Re-conceptualizing Indexicality and Digital Technologies by Jennifer Elizabeth Dobbs, B.A. (Honours) A thesis submitted to the Faculty of Graduate Studies and Research in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts in Film Studies Carleton University Ottawa, Ontario ©2012 Jennifer Elizabeth Dobbs Library and Archives Bibliotheque et Canada Archives Canada Published Heritage Direction du 1+1 Branch Patrimoine de I'edition 395 Wellington Street 395, rue Wellington Ottawa ON K1A0N4 Ottawa ON K1A 0N4 Canada Canada Your file Votre reference ISBN: 978-0-494-93555-2 Our file Notre reference ISBN: 978-0-494-93555-2 NOTICE: AVIS: The author has granted a non­ L'auteur a accorde une licence non exclusive exclusive license allowing Library and permettant a la Bibliotheque et Archives Archives Canada to reproduce, Canada de reproduire, publier, archiver, publish, archive, preserve, conserve, sauvegarder, conserver, transmettre au public communicate to the public by par telecommunication ou par I'lnternet, preter, telecommunication or on the Internet, distribuer et vendre des theses partout dans le loan, distrbute and sell theses monde, a des fins commerciales ou autres, sur worldwide, for commercial or non­ support microforme, papier, electronique et/ou commercial purposes, in microform, autres formats. paper, electronic and/or any other formats. The author retains copyright L'auteur conserve la propriete du droit d'auteur ownership and moral rights in this et des droits moraux qui protege cette these. Ni thesis. Neither the thesis nor la these ni des extraits substantiels de celle-ci substantial extracts from it may be ne doivent etre imprimes ou autrement printed or otherwise reproduced reproduits sans son autorisation.
    [Show full text]
  • Frames and Simulated Documents: Indexicality in Documentary Videogames
    Frames and Simulated Documents: Indexicality in Documentary Videogames Cindy Poremba Concordia University [email protected] Abstract Traffic Games’ controversial 2003 documentary videogame JFK Reloaded—based on the assassination of JFK—has always fallen back in defense on its “documentary” status– specifically, that the core simulation at the heart of the gameplay, and supporting game elements such as vantage points, are based meticulously on real Warren Commission data. I will argue a strong indexical relation in JFKR lies between the game and the documents—not the historic act itself. As such, the game’s primary strength as a documentary work is in re-engaging the archive, rather than simulating history. Author Keywords Documentary videogames, indexicality, JFK Reloaded, simulation, docugames “I would love to see computer, faced with the problems of probabilities of the assassination taking place the way it did, with all these strange incidents which to place before and are continuing to take place after the assassination.” Penn Jones, Rush to Judgment (1967 documentary)1 DALLAS, TEXAS 12:30 pm, November 22, 1963. The Texas School Book Depository, sixth floor. The weather is fine. You have a rifle. JFK Reloaded (2003 Traffic Games) Often the most well known documentary videogames are the most controversial. JFK Reloaded—a game based on the assassination of American president John F. Kennedy—is one of the better known examples. When it rose to public attention in 2004, the game added fuel to a growing outcry over violence and inappropriate content in videogames. It was explicitly condemned by the Kennedy family—and even used to signify the moral vacuum of digital games in an episode of the television series Law and Order2.
    [Show full text]
  • The Syntactic Expression of Tense
    The Syntactic Expression of Tense Tim Stowell UCLA Tim Stowell UCLA Linguistics Department, 405 Hilgard Ave., Los Angeles, CA 90095-1543 USA [email protected] 1 Abstract In this article I defend the view that many central aspects of the semantics of tense are determined by independently-motivated principles of syntactic theory. I begin by decomposing tenses syntactically into a temporal ordering predicate (the true tense, on this approach) and two time-denoting arguments corresponding to covert a reference time (RT) argument and an eventuality time (ET) argument containing the verb phrase. Control theory accounts for the denotation of the RT argument, deriving the distinction between main clause and subordinate clause tenses. The theory of covert movement is used to account for the independent/indexical interpretation of relative clause tenses, and for the correlation between independent tense interpretation and a de re construal of the relative clause. A theory of “past polarity”, based on traditional negative polarity theory, accounts both for the simultaneous “sequence of tense” construal of past tenses in subordinate clauses embedded within past tense contexts, and for the obligatory indexical/independent interpretation of present tense in a relative clause embedded within a past tense context. Combined with the copy theory of movement, the polarity theory also provides an account of the semantics of double access sentences, treating them as involving a special kind of reconstruction effect. Key Words: Tense, Temporal control, Past polarity, De dicto/de re, Double Access 2 1. Tense and syntactic theory In this article I defend the view that many central aspects of the semantics of tense are determined by independently motivated principles of syntactic theory.
    [Show full text]
  • Three Challenges for Indexicalism LENNY CLAPP
    Three Challenges for Indexicalism LENNY CLAPP Abstract: Indexicalism is a strategy for defending truth-conditional semantics from under-determination arguments. According to indexicalism the class of indexical expres- sions includes not only the obvious indexicals, e.g. demonstratives and personal pronouns, but also unobvious indexical expressions, expressions which allegedly have been discovered to be indexicals. This paper argues that indexicalism faces significant obstacles that have yet to be overcome. The issue that divides indexicalism and truth-conditional pragmatics is first clarified. And then three general problems for indexicalism are presented, and some potential solutions that have been proposed in its defense are criticized. 1. Introduction The fundamental idea behind the family of views known as truth-conditional semantics is the following simple principle of truth-conditional compositionality: The truth-conditions expressed by an utterance of a declarative sentence S are determined by only two factors: (i) the logical form (LF) of S, and (ii) the semantic contents of the lexical items in S. Because of the influential work of Kaplan (1989) it is now a familiar point that in order to account for sentences that contain obvious indexicals—e.g. ‘I’, ‘now’, ‘she’, ‘that’ and perhaps some ‘relational terms’ such as ‘local’—a defender of truth-conditional semantics must reject this simple principle of truth-conditional compositionality in favor of a relativized principle: The truth-conditions expressed by an utterance of a declarative sentence Sina context c are determined by only two factors: (i) the logical form (LF) of S,and (ii) the semantic contents of the lexical items of Sinc.
    [Show full text]
  • Compositionality and Concepts in Linguistics and Psychology Language, Cognition, and Mind
    Language, Cognition, and Mind James A. Hampton Yoad Winter Editors Compositionality and Concepts in Linguistics and Psychology Language, Cognition, and Mind Volume 3 Series editor Chungmin Lee, Seoul National University, Seoul, South Korea Editorial board members Tecumseh Fitch, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria Peter Gaerdenfors, Lund University, Lund, Sweden Bart Geurts, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands Noah D. Goodman, Stanford University, Stanford, USA Robert Ladd, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK Dan Lassiter, Stanford University, Stanford, USA Edouard Machery, Pittsburgh University, Pittsburgh, USA This series takes the current thinking on topics in linguistics from the theoretical level to validation through empirical and experimental research. The volumes published offer insights on research that combines linguistic perspectives from recently emerging experimental semantics and pragmatics as well as experimental syntax, phonology, and cross-linguistic psycholinguistics with cognitive science perspectives on linguistics, psychology, philosophy, artificial intelligence and neuroscience, and research into the mind, using all the various technical and critical methods available. The series also publishes cross-linguistic, cross-cultural studies that focus on finding variations and universals with cognitive validity. The peer reviewed edited volumes and monographs in this series inform the reader of the advances made through empirical and experimental research in the language-related cognitive science disciplines.
    [Show full text]
  • A Two-Dimensional Logic for Two Paradoxes of Deontic Modality
    A Two-Dimensional Logic for Two Paradoxes of Deontic Modality Melissa Fusco and Arc Kocurek March 6, 2021 Abstract In this paper, we axiomatize the deontic logic in Fusco 2015, which uses a Stalnaker-inspired account of diagonal acceptance and a two-dimensional account of disjunction to treat Ross’s Paradox and the Puzzle of Free Choice Permission. On this account, disjunction-involving validities are a priori rather than necessary. We show how to axiomatize two-dimensional disjunc- tion so that the introduction/elimination rules for boolean disjunciton can be viewed as one-dimensional projections of more general two-dimensional rules. These completeness results help make explicit the restrictions Fusco’s account must place on free-choice inferences. They are also of independent interest, as they raise difficult questions about how to ‘lift’ a Kripke frame for a one-dimensional modal logic into two dimensions. 1 Introduction The validity of or -introduction seems to be a basic rule of natural language disjunction: from ), one may infer ) or #.1 Or-Intro. ) ( ¹) or #º Yet the rule apparently fails in the scope of deontic modals. For example, the nonentailment known as Ross’s Puzzle illustrates the failure of Or-Intro in the scope of ‘ought’ (henceforth $)(Ross, 1941): Ross’s Puzzle. $) * $¹) or #º As an illustration, observe that (1-a), famously, does not seem to entail (1-b): 1Throughout, we use ‘(’ for the intuitive entailment. Subscripted turnstiles will denote entail- ment relative to a specific semantics. 1 (1) a. You ought to post the letter. b. You ought to post the letter or burn it.
    [Show full text]
  • Contextual Two-Dimensionalism
    Contextual two-dimensionalism phil 93507 Jeff Speaks November 30, 2009 1 Two two-dimensionalist system of The Conscious Mind .............. 1 1.1 Primary and secondary intensions . 2 1.2 The necessary a posteriori, the contingent a priori, and indexicality . 2 1.2.1 Puzzles about the necessary a posteriori and contingent a priori . 3 1.2.2 The necessary a posteriori and conceivability arguments . 4 1.3 Explanatory ambitions of contextual two-dimensionalism . 5 1.4 The semantics of names and natural kind terms . 5 2 Objections to contextual two-dimensionalism . 7 2.1 Problems with the rigidification of names and kind terms . 7 2.1.1 Problems with ‘actually’-rigidified descriptions . 7 2.1.2 Problems with ‘dthat’-rigidified descriptions . 8 2.1.3 Problems finding descriptions to rigidify . 9 2.2 Problems with the semantics of attitude ascriptions . 9 2.2.1 Option 1: attitude ascriptions report relations to primary intensions 10 2.2.2 Option 2: attitude ascriptions report relations to secondary intensions 10 2.2.3 Option 3: hybrid views . 11 2.3 Misclassification of sentences as a priori . 11 There are two main varieties of two-dimensionalism, which (following Chalmers) I'll call contextual and epistemic two-dimensionalism. 1 Two two-dimensionalist system of The Conscious Mind A good representative of contextual two-dimensionalism is the theory presented by Chalmers in The Conscious Mind. On p. 56 of The Conscious Mind, Chalmers turns to consideration of a popular objection to conceivability arguments for dualism: the objection that the relationship between mental and physical properties might be necessary, though only discoverable a posteriori.
    [Show full text]
  • Epistemic Modality, Mind, and Mathematics
    Epistemic Modality, Mind, and Mathematics Hasen Khudairi June 20, 2017 c Hasen Khudairi 2017, 2021 All rights reserved. 1 Abstract This book concerns the foundations of epistemic modality. I examine the nature of epistemic modality, when the modal operator is interpreted as concerning both apriority and conceivability, as well as states of knowledge and belief. The book demonstrates how epistemic modality relates to the computational theory of mind; metaphysical modality; the types of math- ematical modality; to the epistemic status of undecidable propositions and abstraction principles in the philosophy of mathematics; to the modal pro- file of rational intuition; and to the types of intention, when the latter is interpreted as a modal mental state. Chapter 2 argues for a novel type of expressivism based on the duality between the categories of coalgebras and algebras, and argues that the duality permits of the reconciliation be- tween modal cognitivism and modal expressivism. Chapter 3 provides an abstraction principle for epistemic intensions. Chapter 4 advances a two- dimensional truthmaker semantics, and provides three novel interpretations of the framework along with the epistemic and metasemantic. Chapter 5 applies the modal µ-calculus to account for the iteration of epistemic states, by contrast to availing of modal axiom 4 (i.e. the KK principle). Chapter 6 advances a solution to the Julius Caesar problem based on Fine’s "cri- terial" identity conditions which incorporate conditions on essentiality and grounding. Chapter 7 provides a ground-theoretic regimentation of the pro- posals in the metaphysics of consciousness and examines its bearing on the two-dimensional conceivability argument against physicalism.
    [Show full text]