Conditionals: a Theory of Meaning, Pragmatics, and Inference

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Conditionals: a Theory of Meaning, Pragmatics, and Inference Psychological Review Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2002, Vol. 109, No. 4, 646–678 0033-295X/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0033-295X.109.4.646 Conditionals: A Theory of Meaning, Pragmatics, and Inference P. N. Johnson-Laird Ruth M. J. Byrne Princeton University University of Dublin The authors outline a theory of conditionals of the form If A then C and If A then possibly C. The 2 sorts of conditional have separate core meanings that refer to sets of possibilities. Knowledge, pragmatics, and semantics can modulate these meanings. Modulation can add information about temporal and other relations between antecedent and consequent. It can also prevent the construction of possibilities to yield 10 distinct sets of possibilities to which conditionals can refer. The mental representation of a conditional normally makes explicit only the possibilities in which its antecedent is true, yielding other possibilities implicitly. Reasoners tend to focus on the explicit possibilities. The theory predicts the major phenomena of understanding and reasoning with conditionals. You reason about conditional relations because much of your If the TCE in the wells had been drawn from out of the river, knowledge is conditional. If you get caught speeding, then you pay then there’d be TCE in the riverbed. a fine. If you have an operation, then you need time to recuperate. But there isn’t any TCE in the riverbed. If you have money in the bank, then you can cash a check. Conditional reasoning is a central part of thinking, yet people do Schlictmann then argued that these premises were consistent with not always reason correctly. The lawyer Jan Schlictmann in a the proposition that no contamination came from the river. What celebrated trial (see Harr, 1995, pp. 361–362) elicited the follow- he overlooked is that the conclusion is not merely consistent with ing information from an expert witness about the source of a the premises but follows necessarily from them. Psychologists chemical pollutant trichloroethylene (TCE): disagree about the cause of such oversights and about conditional reasoning in general (e.g., Braine & O’Brien, 1998; Cheng & Holyoak, 1985; Johnson-Laird, 1986; Rips, 1983). Philosophers, P. N. Johnson-Laird, Department of Psychology, Princeton University; logicians, and linguists also disagree about the meaning of condi- Ruth M. J. Byrne, Department of Psychology, University of Dublin, tionals (Adams, 1975; Lewis, 1973; Stalnaker, 1968). No consen- Dublin, Ireland. sus exists about if. This research was made possible in part by a grant to P. N. Johnson- Our concern is everyday conditionals, including all sentences of Laird from the National Science Foundation (Grant BCS 0076287) to study the form If A then C or CifA,where A and C are declarative strategies in reasoning. We thank Jon Baron and Ray Nickerson for their clauses. Naive individuals can grasp the meaning of such asser- helpful criticisms of an earlier version of this article. We thank Yingrui tions, and they can use it to reason. The term naive refers here Yang for providing technical help and advice, Patricia Barres for carrying merely to people who have not studied logic in any depth. In the out Experiment 2, Rachel McCloy for carrying out a study of modus 1970s, psychologists assumed that such individuals reason using tollens, Dan Zook for writing a preliminary version of the program for pragmatic modulation, and David Over for engaging in many discussions formal rules of inference like those of a logical calculus. The of philosophical theories of conditionals. We are also grateful to many challenge was to pin down the particular rules (e.g., Braine, 1978; people: Peter Wason, now in retirement, for pioneering the psychological Johnson-Laird, 1975; Osherson, 1974–1976). Such theorizing ne- study of conditionals; Jonathan Evans and David Over for a critique of our glected a discovery made by Wason (1966). Intelligent adults in account, which they presented at the Workshop on Mental Models in his selection task regularly committed a logical error. He laid out Brussels, March 2001, and also for many discussions of conditionals; Luca four cards in front of them: Bonatti, the late Martin Braine, Keith Holyoak, Howard Margolis, David O’Brien, and Lance Rips for their vigorous criticisms of the model theory; AB23 Monica Bucciarelli, Orlando Espino, Vittorio Girotto, Eugenia Goldvarg, Paolo Legrenzi, Maria Sonino Legrenzi, Juan Garcı´a Madruga, Henry They knew that each card had a letter on one side and a number on Markovits, Mary Newsome, Cristina Quelhas, Sergio Moreno Rios, Carlos the other side. He showed them a conditional: If a card has the Santamarı´a, Fabien Savary, Walter Schaeken, Walter Schroyens, Alessan- letter A on one side then it has the number 2 on the other side. dra Tasso, and Valerie Thompson for their collaboration in studies of Their task was to select those cards that had to be turned over to conditional reasoning. We are also grateful to the following individuals for discover whether the conditional was true or false about the four many useful ideas: Bruno Bara, Victoria Bell, David Green, Uri Hasson, cards. Most people selected the A and 2 cards, or the A card alone. Mark Keane, Markus Knauff, Jim Kroger, Robert Mackiewicz, Hans- Georg Neth, Tom Ormerod, Rosemary Stevenson, Dan Sperber, Vladimir They failed to select the 3 card. However, if the 3 has an A on its Sloutsky, and Jean-Baptiste van der Henst. other side, the conditional is false. Indeed, nearly everyone judges Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to P. N. it to be false in this case. Individuals also generate this case when Johnson-Laird, Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Green they are asked to make a conditional false (Oaksford & Stenning, Hall, Princeton, New Jersey 08544. E-mail: [email protected] 1992), and they judge that the probability of this case is zero given 646 CONDITIONALS 647 the truth of the conditional (Johnson-Laird, Legrenzi, Girotto, model theory) run in parallel for many aspects of reasoning. Legrenzi, & Caverni, 1999). When Wason and Shapiro (1971) Insofar as both sorts of theory reflect logical principles, however, changed the assertion to a sensible everyday conditional, many they have potential limitations (cf. Sloman, 1996). And at the heart people made the correct selection. Theories based on formal rules of these limitations is the vexed question of conditionals. Are they have difficulty in explaining both the error with neutral condition- truth functional or not? The answer is not clear. Some theorists als and its correction with sensible content. have argued that conditionals have the same meaning as the Our theory is not formal. It assumes instead that reasoners use truth-functional connective of material implication (e.g., Grice, the meaning of premises, and general knowledge, to imagine the 1975; Wilson, 1975), which is shown in Table 2. Other theorists possibilities under consideration—that is, to construct mental mod- have rejected this analysis (e.g., Gazdar, 1979; O’Brien, 1999). els of them (see Johnson-Laird & Byrne, 1991; Polk & Newell, The profundity of the puzzle correlates with the length of its 1995). Mental models can be constructed from perception, imag- literature. One aim of the present article is to resolve this puzzle. ination, or the comprehension of discourse. They underlie visual Simple inferences from conditionals are of four main sorts: images, but they can also be abstract, representing situations that 1. Modus ponens: cannot be visualized. Each mental model represents a possibility. It is akin to a diagram in that its structure is analogous to the If there is a circle then there is a triangle. If A then C. structure of the situation that it represents, unlike, say, the structure There is a circle. A. of logical forms used in formal rule theories. І There is a triangle. І C. Some sentential connectives such as and and or have logical 2. Modus tollens: meanings that are easy to define. For example, consider the fol- lowing: If there is a circle then there is a triangle. If A then C. There is not a triangle. Not C. Either the printer is broken or else the cable is disconnected, І There is not a circle. І Not A. but not both. 3. Denial of the antecedent: The truth of an exclusive disjunction such as this one is a function of the truth of its constituent atomic propositions, that is, those If there is a circle then there is a triangle. If A then C. propositions that contain neither not nor connectives (the printer is There is not a circle. Not A. broken, the cable is disconnected). The exclusive disjunction is І There is not a triangle. І Not C. true in two cases: first, where it is true that the printer is broken but false that the cable is disconnected, and, second, where it is false 4. Affirmation of the consequent: that the printer is broken but true that the cable is disconnected. Logicians lay out these conditions in a truth table, as shown in If there is a circle then there is a triangle. If A then C. Table 1, in which we abbreviate the atomic propositions. Each row There is a triangle. C. І І in the truth table represents a different possibility. Thus, the first There is a circle. A. row represents the possibility in which both the printer is broken Modus ponens and modus tollens are valid for both conditionals and the cable is disconnected; in this possibility, the exclusive and biconditionals (If and only if there is a circle then there is a disjunction is false. The connective and can be defined in an triangle), whereas the other two inferences are valid only for analogous way.
Recommended publications
  • Gibbardian Collapse and Trivalent Conditionals
    Gibbardian Collapse and Trivalent Conditionals Paul Égré* Lorenzo Rossi† Jan Sprenger‡ Abstract This paper discusses the scope and significance of the so-called triviality result stated by Allan Gibbard for indicative conditionals, showing that if a conditional operator satisfies the Law of Import-Export, is supraclassical, and is stronger than the material conditional, then it must collapse to the material conditional. Gib- bard’s result is taken to pose a dilemma for a truth-functional account of indicative conditionals: give up Import-Export, or embrace the two-valued analysis. We show that this dilemma can be averted in trivalent logics of the conditional based on Reichenbach and de Finetti’s idea that a conditional with a false antecedent is undefined. Import-Export and truth-functionality hold without triviality in such logics. We unravel some implicit assumptions in Gibbard’s proof, and discuss a recent generalization of Gibbard’s result due to Branden Fitelson. Keywords: indicative conditional; material conditional; logics of conditionals; triva- lent logic; Gibbardian collapse; Import-Export 1 Introduction The Law of Import-Export denotes the principle that a right-nested conditional of the form A → (B → C) is logically equivalent to the simple conditional (A ∧ B) → C where both antecedentsare united by conjunction. The Law holds in classical logic for material implication, and if there is a logic for the indicative conditional of ordinary language, it appears Import-Export ought to be a part of it. For instance, to use an example from (Cooper 1968, 300), the sentences “If Smith attends and Jones attends then a quorum *Institut Jean-Nicod (CNRS/ENS/EHESS), Département de philosophie & Département d’études cog- arXiv:2006.08746v1 [math.LO] 15 Jun 2020 nitives, Ecole normale supérieure, PSL University, 29 rue d’Ulm, 75005, Paris, France.
    [Show full text]
  • The Presupposition of Subjunctive Conditionals
    The Presupposition of Subjunctive Conditionals Kai von Fintel M.I.T. 0. Introduction Why are some conditionals subjunctive? It is often assumed that at least one crucial difference is that subjunctive conditionals presuppose that their antecedent is false, that they are counterfactual (Lakoff 1970). The traditional theory has apparently been refuted. Perhaps the clearest counter-example is one given by Alan Anderson (1951: 37): If Jones had taken arsenic, he would have shown just exactly those symptoms which he does in fact show. A typical place to use such a subjunctive conditional would be in the course of an argument that tries to bolster the hypothesis that Jones did in fact take arsenic. But then it would of course be self-defeating to presuppose that the hypothesis is false. Thus, something else must be going on. 1. Preliminaries Let’s first assume the following “strict” semantics for “bare” conditionals, those in which the if-clause restricts an implicit quantifier over worlds: (1) Schematic Truth-Conditions for Bare Conditionals A bare conditional if p, q has the logical form " D (if p) (q). If defined, it is true in a world w iff all worlds w' in D(w) such that p(w') are such that q(w'): p Ç D(w) Í q. Here D is a function that assigns to any (evaluation) world w a set of accessible worlds. This semantics departs in some ways from the more standard Stalnaker-Lewis analysis and is justified in a recent paper of mine (von Fintel 1997). What it *Thanks to Paul Portner for very helpful comments on a previous draft.
    [Show full text]
  • Some Remarks on Indicative Conditionals
    Some Remarks on Indicative Conditionals Barbara Abbott Michigan State University 1. Introduction This paper concerns indicative conditionals, such as the English examples in (1): (1) a. If Lynn was at the meeting, she knows what's going on. b. If we leave now, we can make the 5:00 show. c. If it rains tomorrow, we won't have a picnic. We wi11look at several theories of indicative conditionals grouped into three categories: those that base its semantics on its logical counterpart (the material conditional); intensional analyses, which bring in alternative possible worlds; and a third subgroup which denies that indicative conditionals express propositions at all. We will also look at some problems for each kind of approach. 2. The Material Conditional Analysis There is a long tradition that associates natural language indicative conditionals with the material conditional of propositional logic. (Indeed, introductory logic texts typically assume this identification.) The semantics of the material conditional is given entirely by its truthtable, shown in (2). (2) p q p� q T T T T F F F T T F F T On this account natural language indicative conditionals express a truth function of their component sentences. 2.1. Problems fo r the Material Conditional Approach There are several problems for this approach. One stressed by Edgington (1995, 2003) is the following: assigning indicative conditionals the material conditional truth function means failing to distinguish between unequally probable conditionals with equally improbable antecedents, like those in (3) (from Jackson 197911991, 115): © 2004 by Barbara Abbott R. Young (ed), SA LT XIV 1-19, Ithaca, NY: Cornell University.
    [Show full text]
  • Indicative Conditionals: Probabilities and Relevance
    Philos Stud https://doi.org/10.1007/s11098-021-01622-3 Indicative conditionals: probabilities and relevance 1,2 2 Francesco Berto • Aybu¨ke O¨ zgu¨n Accepted: 21 February 2021 Ó The Author(s) 2021 Abstract We propose a new account of indicative conditionals, giving acceptability and logical closure conditions for them. We start from Adams’ Thesis: the claim that the acceptability of a simple indicative equals the corresponding conditional probability. The Thesis is widely endorsed, but arguably false and refuted by empirical research. To fix it, we submit, we need a relevance constraint: we accept a simple conditional u ! w to the extent that (i) the conditional probability pðwjuÞ is high, provided that (ii) u is relevant for w. How (i) should work is well-understood. It is (ii) that holds the key to improve our understanding of conditionals. Our account has (i) a probabilistic component, using Popper functions; (ii) a relevance component, given via an algebraic structure of topics or subject matters. We present a probabilistic logic for simple indicatives, and argue that its (in)validities are both theoretically desirable and in line with empirical results on how people reason with conditionals. Keywords Conditionals Á Conditional probabilities Á Relevance Á Adams’ thesis Á Subject matter People do not consider the conditional probability to be the only basis for asserting a conditional, even if they judge the probability of the conditional to be the conditional probability. Evans & Over, If. & Francesco Berto [email protected] Aybu¨ke O¨ zgu¨n [email protected] 1 Arche´, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, Scotland 2 ILLC, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands 123 F.
    [Show full text]
  • Chasing Hook: Quantified Indicative Conditionals
    1 Chasing Hook: Quantified Indicative Conditionals Angelika Kratzer University of Massachusetts at Amherst May 2015 Abstract This chapter looks at indicative conditionals embedded under quantifiers, with a special emphasis on ‘one-case’ conditionals as in No query was answered if it came from a doubtful address. It agrees with earlier assessments that a complete conditional (with antecedent and consequent) is embedded under a quantifier in those constructions, but then proceeds to create a dilemma by showing that we can’t always find the right interpretation for that conditional. Contrary to earlier assessments, Stalnaker’s conditional won’t always do. The paper concludes that the embedded conditional in the sentence above is a material implication, but the if- clause also plays a pragmatic role in restricting the domain of the embedding quantifier. That an appeal to pragmatics should be necessary at all goes with Edgington’s verdict that “we do not have a satisfactory general account of sentences with conditional constituents.” 2 0. Introduction I should say this upfront. The Hook from Edgington’s Conditionals is a man with opinions. He thinks that if is a truth-functional connective and corresponds to material implication. My Hook is not a ‘he’ or a ‘she’, but an ‘it’. It is material implication itself. It is É. Hook is elusive. We know it has a connection with if, but we don’t quite know what the connection is. My project is to hunt Hook down in the back alleys of English. It’s not that I think Hook is that special. I am interested in Hook because it makes a good probe for exploring the properties of embedded conditionals.
    [Show full text]
  • A Note on Conditionals and Restrictors∗
    A note on conditionals and restrictors∗ Daniel Rothschild Introduction Within linguistic semantics, it is near orthodoxy that the function of the word `if' (in most cases) is to mark restrictions on quantification. Just as in the sentence `Every man smokes', the common noun `man' restricts the quantifier `every', in the sentence `Usually, if it's winter it's cold', `it's winter' acts as a restrictor on the situational quantifier `usually'. This view, originally due to Lewis (1975), has been greatly extended in work by Heim (1982) and, most notably, Kratzer (1981, 1986) into a rich theory of almost all uses of the word `if'. I call this the restrictor view of `if'. Despite its linguistic prominence, this view of the word `if' has played little role in the philosophical discussion of conditionals. Fairly recent philosophical surveys such as Bennett's (2003) book-length introduction or Edgington's (1995; 2008) review articles do not even mention the restrictor view. Stranger still, in his seminal work on conditionals and probability, Lewis (1976, 1986) does not mention the restrictor view which he pioneered, despite the intimate relation noted later by Kratzer (1986).1 This paper tries to fill in the gap left by these omissions.2 I make four main points. First, I argue that given the current state of things our best bet is to accept the `restrictor view' and to assume that `if' is not ambiguous, so ∗This paper was written for a conference in honor of Dorothy Edgington at the Institute of Philosophy, London in May 2011. I am grateful to Dilip Ninan, Scott Sturgeon, and Seth Yalcin for useful discussion of this topic, and particularly to Justin Khoo, whose recent draft (cited below) prompted me to think about these issues.
    [Show full text]
  • What Is Mathematical Philosophy? Mathematical Philosophy Is the Study of Philosophical Problems with the Help of Mathematical Methods
    Mathematical is used in the sciences. Physicists have, of course, always used it. Meanwhile mathematical and statistical methods are also of tremendous use in many other sciences such as the biomedical and social sciences. Q: But why should one also use mathematics in philosophy? A: It works! Mathematical methods help us to solve problems that cannot be solved otherwise. Motivation What is Mathematical Philosophy? Mathematical Philosophy is the study of philosophical problems with the help of mathematical methods. Hannes Leitgeb and Stephan Hartmann Munich Center for Mathematical Philosophy (MCMP) Summer School on Mathematical Philosophy for Female Students Munich, 27 July 2014 H. Leitgeb and S. Hartmann (MCMP) What is Mathematical Philosophy? MCMP Summer School 1 / 45 H. Leitgeb and S. Hartmann (MCMP) What is Mathematical Philosophy? MCMP Summer School 2 / 45 Motivation Motivation Deeper reason: Mathematics is not actually about number-crunching! Modern mathematics is the study of formal structure by means of proof: Mathematical Philosophy is the study of philosophical problems with the whenever an object or a class of objects exhibits enough structure, help of mathematical methods. mathematics allows us to describe these objects in precise terms, to extract information from these descriptions, and to transform this Mathematical is used in the sciences. Physicists have, of course, always information into conclusions. used it. Meanwhile mathematical and statistical methods are also of tremendous use in many other sciences such as the biomedical and But structure can be found everywhere in philosophy: the concept of truth social sciences. has a formal structure, every system of rational beliefs must have a particular kind of structure, the classification of acts into morally right and Q: But why should one also use mathematics in philosophy? morally wrong ones has a certain structure, and so on.
    [Show full text]
  • Causal Discounting and Conditional Reasoning in Children
    1 2 Causal Discounting and 1. Introduction Conditional reasoning is widely viewed as central to human inference and a paradigm case of logical thought. Perhaps the most natural use of the indicative conditional is to Conditional Reasoning in Children express relationships between cause and effect, either in reasoning predictively from causes to effects (if I cut my finger, then I bleed); or reasoning diagnostically from effects to causes (if I bleed, then I cut my finger). To the extent that conditionals are used to describe cause-effect relationships, the study of cause and effect may help illuminate the Nilufa Ali function of conditionals. Cognitive, Perception and Brain Sciences Many studies of conditional reasoning have used experimental materials which University College London participants are likely to interpret in causal terms. Indeed, unless the domain of reasoning Anne Schlottmann is highly abstract (e.g., about numbers and letters printed on cards), it seems inevitable Developmental Science that people will recruit causal knowledge (about fingers, cuts, bleeding, etc) to solving University College London conditional reasoning problems. But in the majority of studies causal structure has been Abigail Shaw incidental, rather than the focus of inquiry (but see Griffiths & Tenenbaum, 2005; Developmental Science Sloman & Lagnado, 2005). In this chapter we suggest that the causal structure described University College London by the premises may be more important than its logical structure—the traditional focus of Nick Chater research in the psychology of conditional reasoning—in determining the inferences Cognitive, Perception and Brain Sciences people draw. University College London More specifically, we consider that conditional inference patterns should differ Mike Oaksford.
    [Show full text]
  • Causation and Probability in Indicative and Counterfactual Conditionals
    Causation and probability in indicative and counterfactual conditionals Daniel Lassiter Stanford University Draft as of August 14, 2018. Comments, criticisms, suggestions, and questions very welcome: [email protected]. Abstract This paper provides and motivates a unified compositional semantics for indicative and counterfactual conditionals that have in their consequents probability operators: probable, likely, more likely than not, 50% chance and so on. The theory combines a Kratzerian syntax for conditionals with semantics based on causal Bayes nets, and develops an algorithm for interpreting probabilistic indicatives and counterfactuals on which the key difference between them involves the choice between two operations on causal models—conditioning and intervention. The interaction between probability operators and the two kinds of conditionals sheds new light on the vexed question of what real-world information is retained in counterfactual scenarios and what is discarded, and helps to explain why indicatives are not sensitive to details of causal structure while counterfactuals are. The second part of the paper develops a new empirical challenge for the revision procedure that emerges due to the interaction of counterfactuals and probability operators. The straightforward approach to revision developed in the first part is shown to be inadequate for certain scenarios in which real-world information that is discarded in the counterfactual scenario nevertheless informs the values of other variables. I describe two extensions of
    [Show full text]
  • Counterfactuals
    Counterfactuals Notes by R.J. Buehler Based on Counterfactuals by David Lewis May 6, 2014 ii Contents Preface v 1 An Analysis of Counterfactuals1 1.1 Conditionals..........................................1 1.1.1 Overview.......................................1 1.1.2 The Necessity of Being Vague............................3 1.2 Counterfactual Conditional Operators...........................4 1.2.1 The New Operators.................................4 1.2.2 Modal Logic.....................................4 1.3 Fixed Strict Conditionals..................................5 1.3.1 Strictness Orderings.................................5 1.3.2 Counterfactuals as Strict Conditionals.......................5 1.4 Variably Strict Conditionals.................................6 1.4.1 Truth Conditions...................................7 1.4.2 The Limit Assumption................................7 1.5 ‘Might’ Counterfactuals and Outer Modalities.......................8 1.5.1 Truth Conditions...................................8 1.5.2 Generating a Modal Logic..............................8 1.5.3 A New System....................................9 1.6 Impossible Antecedents................................... 10 1.7 True Antecedents and the Inner Modalities........................ 11 1.7.1 True Antecedents................................... 11 1.7.2 Weakly Centered Systems and the Inner Modalities................ 12 1.8 Counterfactual Fallacies................................... 13 1.8.1 Strengthening the Antecedent............................ 13 1.8.2 Transitivity.....................................
    [Show full text]
  • Indicative Versus Counterfactual Conditionals, Classical Versus Non
    (to appear in Philosophical Issues 16 (2006)) Indicative versus Subjunctive Conditionals, Congruential versus Non-Hyperintensional Contexts Timothy Williamson §0. A familiar if obscure idea: an indicative conditional presents its consequent as holding in the actual world on the supposition that its antecedent so holds, whereas a subjunctive conditional merely presents its consequent as holding in a world, typically counterfactual, in which its antecedent holds. Consider this pair: (1) If you had starved as a child, you would have been as tall now as you actually are. (2) If you starved as a child, you are as tall now as you actually are. I do not know how tall you are; I doubt that you starved as a child. My empirical evidence tells strongly against (1), by indicating that childhood malnutrition tends to reduce adult height. In contrast, (2) is logically trivial. How things actually are includes your actual height, whether or not it also includes your childhood starvation; how things would have been if you had starved as a child does not include your actual height. A 1 similar contrast arises between metaphysical and epistemic modalities. As a claim of metaphysical possibility, (3) is obviously true, while as a claim of epistemic possibility (4) is obviously false: (3) You could have been less tall now than you actually are. (4) You may be less tall now than you actually are. Such differences are no mere curiosities of natural language. They exemplify a very general difference between two kinds of sentential context. This paper expounds the general difference, and uses it to derive constraints on the semantics of indicative conditionals.
    [Show full text]
  • Indicative Conditionals, Strictly
    Indicative Conditionals, Strictly William B. Starr, Cornell University [email protected] | http://williamstarr.net February, 2014 ............................................................................................... A truth-functional analysis of conditionals is now widely held to be so untenable even Gricean heroics cannot save it. But a similar consensus about the prospects of a strict conditional analysis has recently broken into discord, led by the voices of Warmbrod¯ (1983), Veltman(1985, 1986) and Gillies(2004, 2009, 2010). While some merits of the resurgent strict analysis have been enumerated, no general comparison of it to other analyses (i.e. variably-strict and probabilistic ones) has been undertaken. Indeed, many central questions about the truth-conditions, logic and pragmatics of strict analyses remain unsettled and even unasked. This paper focuses on strict analy- ses of indicative conditionals and attempts to fill these gaps.1 A preferred articulation of the strict analysis emerges and its numerous advantages over variably-strict and probabilistic analyses are more thoroughly detailed. A new class of counterexamples to a suite of patterns typically validated by strict analyses is presented and diagnosed. They figure prominently in this comparison. 1 Introduction If Peirce is to be trusted, a strict analysis of conditionals was first developed by Philo the Logician, a member of the early Hellenistic Dialectical School.2 But Peirce himself seems to have been the first modern strict conditional theorist. ...[P]ossibility may be understood in many senses; but they may all be em- braced under the definition that that is possible which, in a certain state of information, is not known to be false. By varying the supposed state of information all the varieties of possibility are obtained.
    [Show full text]