Cheaters Must Prosper: Reconciling Theoretical and Empirical Perspectives on Cheating in Mutualism

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Cheaters Must Prosper: Reconciling Theoretical and Empirical Perspectives on Cheating in Mutualism Ecology Letters, (2015) 18: 1270–1284 doi: 10.1111/ele.12507 REVIEW AND SYNTHESIS Cheaters must prosper: reconciling theoretical and empirical perspectives on cheating in mutualism Abstract Emily I. Jones,1,2,3† Cheating is a focal concept in the study of mutualism, with the majority of researchers considering Michelle E. Afkhami,4 Erol Akßcay,5 cheating to be both prevalent and highly damaging. However, current definitions of cheating do Judith L. Bronstein,6 Redouan not reliably capture the evolutionary threat that has been a central motivation for the study of Bshary,7 Megan E. Frederickson,4 cheating. We describe the development of the cheating concept and distill a relative-fitness-based Katy D. Heath,8 Jason D. definition of cheating that encapsulates the evolutionary threat posed by cheating, i.e. that chea- Hoeksema,9 Joshua H. Ness,10 ters will spread and erode the benefits of mutualism. We then describe experiments required to 11 conclude that cheating is occurring and to quantify fitness conflict more generally. Next, we dis- M. Sabrina Pankey, Stephanie S. ‡ cuss how our definition and methods can generate comparability and integration of theory and Porter,12 Joel L. Sachs,12 Klara experiments, which are currently divided by their respective prioritisations of fitness consequences Scharnagl13 and Maren L. and traits. To evaluate the current empirical evidence for cheating, we review the literature on sev- Friesen13*,† eral of the best-studied mutualisms. We find that although there are numerous observations of low-quality partners, there is currently very little support from fitness data that any of these meet our criteria to be considered cheaters. Finally, we highlight future directions for research on con- flict in mutualisms, including novel research avenues opened by a relative-fitness-based definition of cheating. Keywords Ant–plant, cleaner fish–client, cooperation, fig–fig wasp, fitness conflict, legume–rhizobia, nectar larceny, partner quality, plant–mycorrhizae, yucca–yucca moth. Ecology Letters (2015) 18: 1270–1284 (Ghoul et al. 2014; Box 1, Fig. 1). There has been disagreement INTRODUCTION over what needs to be measured in order to determine whether Mutualisms are defined by the reciprocal net benefit that cheating is happening, as well as which partner is the focus of heterospecific partners receive through the exchange of such measurements. The literature also contains varying defini- resources and services. Although the outcome of mutualism is tions regarding how distinct cheaters are from other partners mutually beneficial, the underlying actions are self-interested – and whether phylogenetic restrictions should be imposed on individuals are expected to maximise their own net benefits who can be considered a cheater (Table 1). These differing defi- without regard to the consequences for their partners. Thus, nitions have led to inconsistency over how cheating is identified unless the interests of partners are perfectly aligned, there and modelled, hampering our ability to determine how common should be an incentive to ‘cheat’ (Trivers 1971; Soberon & cheating is and to predict how mutualisms will evolve. Martinez del Rio 1985). Adoption of a fitness-based definition of cheating has great However, while there is widespread agreement that cheating promise to promote comparability across studies and systems. is a fundamental concept in the study of mutualism, there has Yet, the definition that has been proposed – that cheating been very little consensus on the definition of cheating itself increases individual fitness and reduces partner fitness (Ghoul 1Department of BioSciences, Rice University, Houston, TX 77005, USA 8Department of Biology, University of Illinois, Urbana, IL 61801, USA 2Wissenschaftskolleg zu Berlin, Institute for Advanced Study, 14193 Berlin, 9Department of Biology, University of Mississippi, University, MS 38677, USA Germany 10Department of Biology, Skidmore College, Saratoga Springs, NY 12866, USA 3Department of Entomology, Washington State University, Pullman, WA 11Department of Molecular, Cell and Biomedical Sciences, University of New 99164, USA Hampshire, Durham, NH 08624, USA 4Department of Ecology & Evolutionary Biology, University of Toronto, 12Department of Biology, University of California, Riverside, CA 92521, USA Toronto, Ontario M5S 3G5, Canada 13Department of Plant Biology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI 5Department of Biology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104, 48824, USA USA *Correspondence: E-mail: [email protected] † 6Department of Ecology & Evolutionary Biology, University of Arizona, These authors contributed equally to this work. ‡ Tucson, AZ 85721, USA School of Biological Sciences, Washington State University, Vancouver WA 7Institute of Biology, University of Neuchatel,^ CH-2000 Neuchatel,^ Switzerland 98686 USA . © 2015 The Authors. Ecology Letters published by CNRS and John Wiley & Sons Ltd. This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited and is not used for commercial purposes. Review and Synthesis Cheating in mutualism 1271 Table 1 Variation in the concept of cheating. Major aspects of the defini- Box 1 Current opinions about cheating tion of cheating and the alternate perspectives that appear in the empirical and theoretical literatures on cheating in mutualism (for a review of alter- As studies of cheating in mutualism have accumulated, so native definitions see Ghoul et al. 2014, table A1). have definitions of cheating (Table 1, Fig. 2). To evaluate current perspectives in the community, we conducted an Aspect of the cheating definition Alternate perspectives anonymous survey of professional ecologists and evolution- What needs to be measured • Mutualist rewards ary biologists (Supporting Information S1). Of the respon- • Specific antagonistic dents who self-identified as working on mutualism, 30% behaviours also identified that they work on cheating. The average • Net fitness effects response to ‘How common is cheating in mutualisms?’ was Who needs to be measured • The putative cheater alone 2.476, with answers ranging from 1 to 4 on a scale of 1 • Multiple individuals for (nonexistent) to 5 (ubiquitous). The average response to comparison ‘How much of a threat does cheating pose for the persis- • Both the putative cheater tence of mutualisms?’ was 2.825, with answers ranging and its partner from 1 to 5 on a scale of 1 (no threat) to 5 (extreme • Multiple partner threat). Individuals rated the threat of cheating to be combinations higher than its prevalence (paired t-test, P = 0.0351). How different cheaters • Discretely different are from non-cheaters Respondents were asked to evaluate seven potential defini- • Quantitatively different tions of cheating from the literature on a scale of 1 (com- Phylogenetic restrictions • None pletely disagree) to 5 (completely agree). Of the potential • Only members of definitions, only ‘a cheater provides no reward’ was dis- (previously) mutualistic lineages can be considered agreed with on average. The potential definition that cheaters received the most agreement was ‘a cheater has a higher reward taken: reward given ratio than other prospective The perspectives that correspond with our definition (a cheater must have partners’. The level of agreement for each of the seven higher than average relative fitness and cause its partner to have lower than average relative fitness) are given in bold. potential definitions is shown in Fig. 1. BD F A BRIEF HISTORY OF THE CHEATING CONCEPT A C E G Individuals and species that are associated with mutualisms –0.5 –0.25 0 0.25 0.5 but do not confer benefits have been noted since biologists Disagree Neutral Agree first began studying mutualisms. For example, in 1878 Kerner wrote an entire book entitled ‘Flowers and their Unbidden Figure 1 Survey responses on the definition of cheating. Respondents were Guests’, a product of his years of studying pollination biology asked to evaluate seven potential definitions of cheating from the under the influence of Charles Darwin. Many early examples literature on a scale of 1 (completely disagree) to 5 (completely agree); we were summarised in ‘Parasitism and Symbiosis’, published by re-scaled these responses to calculate the overall extent of agreement ranging from À1to+1. Ordered from least to most agreement, the Caullery in 1952. Through at least the mid-1980s, the ecologi- definitions were (A) a cheater provides no reward, (B) a cheater takes cal literature used a wide diversity of terms to describe these more reward than other potential partners, (C) a cheater takes a benefit phenomena. For instance, ants that occupied acacias without but does not provide a reward, (D) a cheater takes more than its fair defending their hosts were termed ‘parasites’ (Janzen 1975), share, (E) a cheater provides less than its fair share, (F) a cheater the phenomenon in which orchids lured pollinators to non-re- provides less reward than other potential partners and (G) a cheater has a warding flowers was called ‘deception’ (Vogel 1978), and floral higher reward taken: reward given ratio than other prospective partners. visitors that collected nectar without pollinating were defined et al. 2014) – implies a comparison without specifying what as ‘robbers’ or ‘thieves’ depending on their mode of entry to needs to be compared. Here, we provide a brief history of the the flower (Inouye 1980). By the time the first
Recommended publications
  • Cleaning Symbiosis Among California Inshore Fishes
    CLEANING SYMBIOSIS AMONG CALIFORNIA INSHORE FISHES EDMUNDS. HOBSON' ABSTRACT Cleaning symbiosis among shore fishes was studied during 1968 and 1969 in southern California, with work centered at La Jolla. Three species are habitual cleaners: the seAoriF, Ozyjulis californica; the sharpnose seaperch, Phanerodon atripes; and the kelp perch, Brachyistius frenatus. Because of specific differences in habitat, there is little overlap in the cleaning areas of these three spe- cies. Except for juvenile sharpnose seaperch, cleaning is of secondary significance to these species, even though it may be of major significance to certain individuals. The tendency to clean varies between in- dividuals. Principal prey of most members of these species are free-living organisms picked from a substrate and from midwater-a mode of feeding that favors adaptations suited to cleaning. Because it is exceedingly abundant in a variety of habitats, the seiiorita is the predominant inshore cleaning fish in California. Certain aspects of its cleaning relate to the fact that only a few of the many seiioritas present at a given time will clean, and that this activity is not centered around well-defined cleaning stations, as has been reported for certain cleaning fishes elsewhere. Probably because cleaners are difficult to recognize among the many seiioritas that do not clean, other fishes.generally do not at- tempt to initiate-cleaning; rather, the activity is consistently initiated by the cleaner itself. An infest- ed fish approached by a cleaner generally drifts into an unusual attitude that advertises the temporary existence of the transient cleaning station to other fish in need of service, and these converge on the cleaner.
    [Show full text]
  • Major Evolutionary Transitions in Individuality COLLOQUIUM
    PAPER Major evolutionary transitions in individuality COLLOQUIUM Stuart A. Westa,b,1, Roberta M. Fishera, Andy Gardnerc, and E. Toby Kiersd aDepartment of Zoology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 3PS, United Kingdom; bMagdalen College, Oxford OX1 4AU, United Kingdom; cSchool of Biology, University of St. Andrews, Dyers Brae, St. Andrews KY16 9TH, United Kingdom; and dInstitute of Ecological Sciences, Faculty of Earth and Life Sciences, Vrije Universiteit, 1081 HV, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Edited by John P. McCutcheon, University of Montana, Missoula, MT, and accepted by the Editorial Board March 13, 2015 (received for review December 7, 2014) The evolution of life on earth has been driven by a small number broken down into six questions. We explore what is already known of major evolutionary transitions. These transitions have been about the factors facilitating transitions, examining the extent to characterized by individuals that could previously replicate inde- which we can generalize across the different transitions. Ultimately, pendently, cooperating to form a new, more complex life form. we are interested in the underlying evolutionary and ecological For example, archaea and eubacteria formed eukaryotic cells, and factors that drive major transitions. cells formed multicellular organisms. However, not all cooperative Defining Major Transitions groups are en route to major transitions. How can we explain why major evolutionary transitions have or haven’t taken place on dif- A major evolutionary transition has been most broadly defined as a change in the way that heritable information is stored and ferent branches of the tree of life? We break down major transi- transmitted (2). We focus on the major transitions that lead to a tions into two steps: the formation of a cooperative group and the new form of individual (Table 1), where the same problems arise, transformation of that group into an integrated entity.
    [Show full text]
  • Plant-Pollinator Interactions in an Ecological and Evolutionary Context: the Promising
    Plant-Pollinator Interactions in an Ecological and Evolutionary Context: The Promising Role of 3D-Printing Technology and Mathematical Modeling Eric Octavio Campos A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy University of Washington 2017 Reading Committee: Thomas L. Daniel, Chair H.D. ‘Toby’ Bradshaw Janneke Hille Ris Lambers Program Authorized to Offer Degree: Biology ©Copyright 2017 Eric Octavio Campos University of Washington Abstract Plant-Pollinator Interactions in an Ecological and Evolutionary Context: The Promising Role of 3D-Printing Technology and Mathematical Modeling Eric Octavio Campos Co-Chairs of the Supervisory Committee: Professor H.D. ‘Toby’ Bradshaw Department of Biology Professor Thomas L. Daniel Department of Biology This dissertation concerns itself with the role of flower shape in affecting the foraging performance of pollinating animals. The pollinator used in this study is a model organism representing crepuscular hawkmoths in research involving the study of flight neuromuscular physiology and plant-pollinator interactions, Manduca sexta (hereafter Manduca). The broader goal of the work is to develop a new experimental framework for investigating the ecological and evolutionary consequences of plant-pollinator interactions. To that end, I have combined 3D-printing technology and mathematical modelling to construct artificial flowers, which can be manufactured with great precision and with objective, quantitatively describable shapes. First, I present a proof-of-concept study to demonstrate the feasibility of collecting foraging data from a real animal pollinator attempting to feed from 3D-printed artificial flowers. I show that Manduca’s foraging performance is extremely sensitive to variation in floral corolla curvature and nectary diameter.
    [Show full text]
  • Comparative Methods Offer Powerful Insights Into Social Evolution in Bees Sarah Kocher, Robert Paxton
    Comparative methods offer powerful insights into social evolution in bees Sarah Kocher, Robert Paxton To cite this version: Sarah Kocher, Robert Paxton. Comparative methods offer powerful insights into social evolution in bees. Apidologie, Springer Verlag, 2014, 45 (3), pp.289-305. 10.1007/s13592-014-0268-3. hal- 01234748 HAL Id: hal-01234748 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01234748 Submitted on 27 Nov 2015 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Apidologie (2014) 45:289–305 Review article * INRA, DIB and Springer-Verlag France, 2014 DOI: 10.1007/s13592-014-0268-3 Comparative methods offer powerful insights into social evolution in bees 1 2 Sarah D. KOCHER , Robert J. PAXTON 1Department of Organismic and Evolutionary Biology, Museum of Comparative Zoology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA 2Institute for Biology, Martin-Luther-University Halle-Wittenberg, Halle, Germany Received 9 September 2013 – Revised 8 December 2013 – Accepted 2 January 2014 Abstract – Bees are excellent models for studying the evolution of sociality. While most species are solitary, many form social groups. The most complex form of social behavior, eusociality, has arisen independently four times within the bees.
    [Show full text]
  • Diverse Nectar Robbers on Alpinia Roxburghii Sweet (Zingiberaceae)
    Journal of Asia-Pacific Biodiversity 8 (2015) 238e241 HOSTED BY Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Asia-Pacific Biodiversity journal homepage: http://www.elsevier.com/locate/japb Short communication Diverse nectar robbers on Alpinia roxburghii Sweet (Zingiberaceae) Xiaobao Deng a, Wen Deng b, Alice Catherine Hughes c, Dharmalingam Mohandass a,* a Key Laboratory of Tropical Forest Ecology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Menglun Town, Yunnan, PR China b Kunming Institute of Zoology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Jiaochang Donglu, Kunming, Yunnan, PR China c Centre for Integrative Conservation, Xishuangbanna Tropical Botanical Garden, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Menglun Town, Yunnan, PR China article info abstract Article history: This study records for the first time three mammal species as nectar robbers on the ginger Alpinia Received 29 April 2015 roxburghii Sweet. We examined the behavior of nectar robbers and compared with earlier studies on a Received in revised form single plant species. We recorded seven species of nectar robbers: three squirrels, one bird, and three 29 July 2015 bees. Timing of robbing nectars were similar; however, robbing behavior differed among robbers. In Accepted 30 July 2015 particular, squirrels damaged the flower parts while robbing the nectar. Available online 18 August 2015 Copyright Ó 2015, National Science Museum of Korea (NSMK) and Korea National Arboretum (KNA). Production and hosting by Elsevier. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http:// Keywords: animal behavior creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/). ginger plant mammal-nectar robbers tropical seasonal rainforest Introduction studied in detail. Therefore, nectar robbers on ginger species could be a relevant topic to understand ecological consequences.
    [Show full text]
  • Effect of Mutualistic and Antagonistic Bees on Floral Resources and Pollination of a Savanna Shrub
    Flora 232 (2017) 30–38 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Flora j ournal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/flora Effect of mutualistic and antagonistic bees on floral resources and ଝ pollination of a savanna shrub a a,b c c,∗ Marília Monteiro Quinalha , Anselmo Nogueira , Gisela Ferreira , Elza Guimarães a Graduation Program in Biological Sciences (Botany), Institute of Biosciences, UNESP – Univ Estadual Paulista, Botucatu, SP, Brazil b Centro de Ciências Naturais e Humanas, Universidade Federal do ABC, São Bernardo do Campo, SP, Brazil c Departament of Botany, Institute of Biosciences, UNESP – Univ Estadual Paulista, Botucatu, SP, Brazil a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t Article history: Since Darwin, cheaters have been described in plant-pollinator mutualisms. Bignoniaceae species have a Received 24 May 2016 wide interaction network with floral visitors, and most of those interactions are established with cheaters. Received in revised form 26 August 2016 Thus, our objective was to determine which role each floral visitor plays in a system composed by bees Accepted 30 August 2016 and a Bignoniaceae savanna species. So, here we described the bees’ behaviour and defined experi- Edited by S.D. Johnson mentally who are the mutualists and cheaters, we described the temporal sequence of interactions, Available online 6 September 2016 quantified pollen and nectar removal, and checked for the potential effect of robbery damages on pol- linator behaviour. Pollinators visited a small number of flowers, mainly in the early morning, while the Keywords: Bignoniaceae most frequent cheaters (robbers and thieves) visited the flowers throughout the day, increasing visi- Cheaters tation at midmorning, when pollinators had already visited the flowers.
    [Show full text]
  • Fish Cognition: a Primate's Eye View
    Anim Cogn (2002) 5:1–13 DOI 10.1007/s10071-001-0116-5 REVIEW Redouan Bshary · Wolfgang Wickler · Hans Fricke Fish cognition: a primate’s eye view Received: 8 February 2001 / Accepted after revision: 6 September 2001 / Published online: 20 November 2001 © Springer-Verlag 2001 Abstract We provide selected examples from the fish lit- erature of phenomena found in fish that are currently be- Introduction ing examined in discussions of cognitive abilities and evolution of neocortex size in primates. In the context of Recently increased interest in cognitive aspects of the be- social intelligence, we looked at living in individualised haviour of animals is connected with advances in the de- groups and corresponding social strategies, social learning velopment of both theoretical frameworks and methodol- and tradition, and co-operative hunting. Regarding envi- ogy. Cognitive psychologists have adopted a more evolu- ronmental intelligence, we searched for examples concern- tionary approach to cognitive research (see historical re- ing special foraging skills, tool use, cognitive maps, mem- view in Kamil 1998). The major contribution of primatol- ory, anti-predator behaviour, and the manipulation of the ogy to cognitive psychology was arguably the develop- environment. Most phenomena of interest for primatolo- ment of the social intelligence hypothesis (Humphrey 1976; gists are found in fish as well. We therefore conclude that Byrne and Whiten 1988; Dunbar 1992), which states that more detailed studies on decision rules and mechanisms the evolution of cognitive skills together with a large neo- are necessary to test for differences between the cognitive cortex in primates was caused by the social complexity abilities of primates and other taxa.
    [Show full text]
  • Geographic Variation in the Behaviour of the Cleaner Fish Labroides Dimidiatus (Labridae)
    Ethology 108, 353—366 (2002) Ó 2002 Blackwell Verlag, Berlin ISSN 0179–1613 Geographic Variation in the Behaviour of the Cleaner Fish Labroides dimidiatus (Labridae) Carley Bansemer*, Alexandra S. Grutter & Robert Poulinà *Queensland Parks and Wildlife Service, Moreton Bay Marine Park, Cleveland, Queensland, Australia; Department of Zoology and Entomology, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia; àDepartment of Zoology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand Abstract Geographical variation in the outcome ofinterspecific interactions has a range ofproximate ecological causes. For instance, cleaning interactions between coral reeffishes can result in benefits forboth the cleaner and its clients. However, because both parties can cheat and because the rewards of cheating may depend on the local abundance ofectoparasites on clients, the interaction might range from exploitative to mutualistic. In a comparative analysis ofbehavioural measures ofthe association between the cleaner fish Labroides dimidiatus and all its client species, we compared cleaning interactions between two sites on the Great Barrier Reefthat differ with respect to mean ectoparasite abundance. At Heron Island, where client fish consistently harbour fewer ectoparasites, client species that tended to pose for cleaners were more likely to receive feeding bites by cleaners than client species that did not pose for cleaners. This was not the case at Lizard Island, where ectoparasites are significantly more abundant. Client fish generally spent more time posing for cleaners at Lizard Island than their conspecifics at Heron Island. However, fish at Heron Island were inspected longer on average by cleaners than conspecifics at Lizard Island, and they incurred more bites and swipes at their sides per unit time from cleaners.
    [Show full text]
  • Nectar Robbers and Hummingbirds in a Highland Forest in Mexico
    Color profile: Generic CMYK printer profile Composite Default screen 997 Multiple ecological interactions: nectar robbers and hummingbirds in a highland forest in Mexico María del Coro Arizmendi Abstract: Nectar robbers are animals that extract the nectar produced by plants without effecting pollination. These animals can have negative, positive, or neutral effects on the interaction, depending on the direct and indirect effects of each participant in the system. The purpose of this work was to analyze the prevalence of nectar robbing and its tem- poral dynamics in a tropical highland forest in western Mexico. The system was studied in terms of the seasonal changes in (i) specific composition and abundance of hummingbird pollinators, their hosts, and the nectar robbers, (ii) use of resources by the nectarivorous birds and flowers, and (iii) prevalence of flower piercing by the cinnamon flowerpiercer, Diglossa baritula. The guild consisted of 17 species of hummingbirds, 2 species of nectar robbers, and 21 plant species that were visited by nectarivorous birds (robbers and pollinators). The relative abundance of birds and available flowers showed two peaks, one in winter and the other in the rainy season. Some plant species were used more than expected from their abundance. Overlap in the use of flowers among hummingbirds and also between hum- mingbirds and the nectar robbers was high. Nectar robbing was found to be widespread, occurring in almost all of the plants known to be hummingbird pollinated. The ratio of hummingbirds to nectar robbers was around 10 throughout the year. Résumé : Les voleurs de nectar sont des animaux qui extraient le nectar des plantes sans participer à leur pollinisation.
    [Show full text]
  • The False Cleanerfish Relies on Aggressive Mimicry to Bite Fish Fins
    www.nature.com/scientificreports OPEN The false cleanerfsh relies on aggressive mimicry to bite fsh fns when benthic foods are scarce in their local habitat Misaki Fujisawa1,2 ✉ , Yoichi Sakai1,3 & Tetsuo Kuwamura4,5 The false cleanerfsh, Aspidontus taeniatus (Blenniidae), is known for its morphological resemblance to the bluestreak cleaner wrasse Labroides dimidiatus (Labridae). It has been suggested that A. taeniatus, which acts as a mimic, can easily bite the fns of other fshes that are deceived into requesting cleaning from it or allowing it to approach them. In fact, A. taeniatus frequently utilises benthic food items, such as damselfsh eggs, the Christmas tree worm Spirobranchus giganteus, and the boring clam Tridacna crocea. Although geographical variation in the reliance on aggressive mimicry (fn biting) has been reported, the factors have not been determined. We hypothesised that one of the factors is the abundance of benthic food items. To examine our hypothesis, we compared the feeding behaviour of A. taeniatus at two locations showing contrasting abundances of benthic food items in Okinawa, southern Japan. The frequency of fn biting by the small A. taeniatus in Ishigaki Island, where S. giganteus and T. crocea were very rare, was signifcantly higher than that in Sesoko Island, where the two food items were abundant. We conclude that the importance of aggressive mimicry in A. taeniatus varies depending on local food conditions. Aggressive mimicry is a form of imitation in which a predator or parasite (mimic) closely copies another organ- ism (model) that is attractive or harmless to a third organism (dupe) to gain enhanced access to prey.
    [Show full text]
  • Guide to Theecological Systemsof Puerto Rico
    United States Department of Agriculture Guide to the Forest Service Ecological Systems International Institute of Tropical Forestry of Puerto Rico General Technical Report IITF-GTR-35 June 2009 Gary L. Miller and Ariel E. Lugo The Forest Service of the U.S. Department of Agriculture is dedicated to the principle of multiple use management of the Nation’s forest resources for sustained yields of wood, water, forage, wildlife, and recreation. Through forestry research, cooperation with the States and private forest owners, and management of the National Forests and national grasslands, it strives—as directed by Congress—to provide increasingly greater service to a growing Nation. The U.S. Department of Agriculture (USDA) prohibits discrimination in all its programs and activities on the basis of race, color, national origin, age, disability, and where applicable sex, marital status, familial status, parental status, religion, sexual orientation genetic information, political beliefs, reprisal, or because all or part of an individual’s income is derived from any public assistance program. (Not all prohibited bases apply to all programs.) Persons with disabilities who require alternative means for communication of program information (Braille, large print, audiotape, etc.) should contact USDA’s TARGET Center at (202) 720-2600 (voice and TDD).To file a complaint of discrimination, write USDA, Director, Office of Civil Rights, 1400 Independence Avenue, S.W. Washington, DC 20250-9410 or call (800) 795-3272 (voice) or (202) 720-6382 (TDD). USDA is an equal opportunity provider and employer. Authors Gary L. Miller is a professor, University of North Carolina, Environmental Studies, One University Heights, Asheville, NC 28804-3299.
    [Show full text]
  • The Personality Behind Cheating: Behavioural Types and the Feeding Ecology of Cleaner Fish Alexander D
    ethologyinternational journal of behavioural biology Ethology The Personality Behind Cheating: Behavioural Types and the Feeding Ecology of Cleaner Fish Alexander D. M. Wilson*†, Jens Krause†‡, James E. Herbert-Read§ & Ashley J. W. Ward¶ * Department of Biology, Carleton University, Ottawa, ON, Canada † Department of Biology and Ecology of Fishes, Leibniz-Institute of Freshwater Ecology and Inland Fisheries, Berlin, Germany ‡ Department for Crop and Animal Sciences, Humboldt University, Berlin, Germany § Mathematics Department, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden ¶ School of Biological Sciences, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia Correspondence Abstract Alexander D. M. Wilson, Department of Biology, Carleton University, Ottawa, ON The complex mutualistic relationship between the cleaner fish (Labroides K1S5B6, Canada. dimidiatus) and their ‘clients’ in many reef systems throughout the world E-mail: [email protected] has been the subject of debate and research interest for decades. Game- theory models have long struggled with explaining how the mixed strate- Received: February 8, 2014 gies of cheating and honesty might have evolved in such a system and Initial acceptance: April 6, 2014 while significant efforts have been made theoretically, demonstrating the Final acceptance: May 4, 2014 nature of this relationship empirically remains an important research (W. Koenig) challenge. Using the experimental framework of behavioural syndromes, we sought to quantitatively assess the relationship between personality doi: 10.1111/eth.12262 and the feeding ecology of cleaner fish to provide novel insights into the Keywords: mutualism, behavioural underlying mechanistic basis of cheating in cleaner-client interactions. syndromes, Labroides dimidiatus, First, we observed and filmed cleaner fish interactions with heterospecif- personality, game theory, boldness ics, movement patterns and general feeding ecology in the wild.
    [Show full text]