Evolution of Female Choice Under Intralocus Sexual Conflict and Rstb.Royalsocietypublishing.Org Genotype-By-Environment Interactions

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Evolution of Female Choice Under Intralocus Sexual Conflict and Rstb.Royalsocietypublishing.Org Genotype-By-Environment Interactions Downloaded from http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/ on August 27, 2018 Evolution of female choice under intralocus sexual conflict and rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org genotype-by-environment interactions Xiang-Yi Li1 and Luke Holman2 1Department of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, University of Zurich, Winterthurerstrasse 190, Research 8057 Zurich, Switzerland 2School of BioSciences, University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria 3010, Australia Cite this article: Li X-Y, Holman L. 2018 Evolution of female choice under intralocus X-YL, 0000-0001-8662-0865 sexual conflict and genotype-by-environment In many species, females are hypothesized to obtain ‘good genes’ for their interactions. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 373: offspring by mating with males in good condition. However, female pre- 20170425. ferences might deplete genetic variance and make choice redundant. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0425 Additionally, high-condition males sometimes produce low-fitness offspring, for example because of environmental turnover and gene-by-environment interactions (GEIs) for fitness, or because fit males carry sexually antagonistic Accepted: 24 July 2018 alleles causing them to produce unfit daughters. Here, we extend previous theory by investigating the evolution of female mate choice in a spatially One contribution of 14 to a theme issue explicit evolutionary simulation implementing both GEIs and intralocus ‘Linking local adaptation with the evolution of sexual conflict (IASC), under sex-specific hard or soft selection. We show sex differences’. that IASC can weaken female preferences for high-condition males or even cause a preference for males in low condition, depending on the relative benefits of producing well-adapted sons versus daughters, which in turn Subject Areas: depends on the relative hardness of selection on males and females. We evolution, theoretical biology, ecology, discuss the relevance of our results to conservation genetics and empirical computational biology evolutionary biology. This article is part of the theme issue ‘Linking local adaptation with the evolution of sex differences’. Keywords: environmental stochasticity, GEI and GxE, lek paradox, local adaptation, mate preference, sexual antagonism 1. Introduction Selection resulting from mate choice preferences can profoundly affect evolution Author for correspondence: and demography, and can favour striking adaptations such as song, dance and Xiang-Yi Li elaborated coloration [1]. One hypothesis is that mate choice preferences evolved e-mail: [email protected] to allow the choosy sex (hereafter ‘females’ for brevity) to select mates carrying ‘good genes’ (using associated phenotypic cues, such as condition-dependent sexual signals [2]), such that alleles encoding mate choice will be found in high-fitness females more often than alleles encoding random mating [3]. A perennial problem for good genes models is that mate preferences tend to deplete genetic variance, making choice redundant and selecting against choosi- ness whenever it has a cost (the ‘lek paradox’, e.g. [4]). Another problem is that the notion of good genes is subjective. Because gene-by-environment interactions (GEIs) are ubiquitous [5], many alleles only confer high fitness in certain environ- ments. Choosy females may, therefore, obtain alleles that were advantageous in the male’s environment, but which are disadvantageous in the environment experienced by their offspring, due to spatial and/or temporal environmental turnover. Similarly, many alleles confer high fitness when expressed in the ‘environment’ of a male body, but low fitness in a female body, or vice versa [6,7]. Such intralocus sexual conflict (IASC) means that females selecting high- condition males may, therefore, produce fit sons but unfit daughters, reducing or reversing the net fitness of mate choice [8]. Electronic supplementary material is available Alternatively, IASC might sometimes drive the evolution of female choice. online at https://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9. Seger & Trivers [9] showed that females can evolve to prefer males carrying figshare.c.4183508. female-beneficial/male-detrimental alleles, due to either a build-up of linkage & 2018 The Author(s) Published by the Royal Society. All rights reserved. Downloaded from http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/ on August 27, 2018 disequilibrium between female preference and sexual antagon- time series), where (i) E values are static and homogeneous 2 istic loci, or partial population-mixing by migration between across habitat patches, (ii) E values are static but heterogeneous rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org demes. Albert & Otto [10] studied the effects of IASC on across habitats, (iii) E values are heterogeneous and fluctuate female-choice evolution under different sex-determination sys- mildly across patches or (iv) E values are heterogeneous and fluc- tems. They found that if both the IASC locus and the female tuate strongly across patches. In the last two types, E values are temporally and spatially autocorrelated such that nearby patches choice locus are autosomal, female choice cannot evolve. and time points tend to have similar E. Similarly, modelling work has shown that GEIs can both help and hinder the spread of alleles encoding female prefer- ence [11,12]. However, few if any previous models have (c) Condition, mate choice and reproduction simultaneously examined the effects of IASC and GEIs on We model discrete generations in which individuals are born, female-choice evolution. Since GEIs help maintain genetic disperse, reproduce and then die. We assume that an individ- variance, they help resolve the lek paradox by maintaining ual’s condition is determined by (i) the match between its Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B locally maladapted alleles. Conversely, GEIs lead to ‘mistakes’ genotype at the local adaptation locus and the environment(s) whereby females choose a male whose alleles are poorly it encounters and (ii) its genotype at the sexual conflict locus. j adapted to the environment that their offspring will experi- Specifically, the condition ( ) of an individual in the reproduc- tion phase is a weighted average of its conditions in the natal ence, reducing the benefits of being choosy. Interestingly, and breeding patches. In equation (2.1), the subscripts N and B these two effects do not cancel out: occasional mistakes actually represent the effects on condition of the environmental states in 373 promote the evolution of mate choice, as they help maintain a the natal and breeding patches, respectively, while C (concordant) pool of maladapted males for choosy females to avoid. and A (antagonistic) represent the effects on condition of the : 20170425 We hypothesize that when individual condition depends local adaptation and sexual conflict loci. The relative importance on both local adaptation and IASC, a female preference for of the natal versus breeding habitats in determining condition is high-condition males may yield locally adapted offspring, weighted by knatal, while kLA (local adaptation) controls the but runs the risk of producing unfit daughters. An additional relative contributions of local adaptation versus sexual conflict complication is that the proportion of genetic variance in to individual condition. The condition of an individual at the fitness that is sexually antagonistic is not fixed, but varies time of breeding is then dynamically in response to processes such as migration, j ¼ knatal(kLAjNÁC þ (1 À kLA)jNÁA) local adaptation, environmental changes and assortative þ (1 À knatal)(kLAjBÁC þ (1 À kLA)jBÁA): ð2:1Þ mating (e.g. empirical evidence [13–16] and theory [17,18]). Here, we use individual-based simulations to jointly For the local adaptation locus, condition is maximized when study the evolution of female mate preferences, local adap- the individual’s genotype (f) exactly matches the environmental tation under GEIs, IASC and sex-specific (potentially state E, and decreases exponentially as the phenotype deviates co-evolving) dispersal behaviour. Our principal aims are to from it, so that jiÁC ¼ Exp( 2 10jf 2 Ej), where i can take either test whether female preferences evolve more or less easily N (on natal patch) or B (on breeding patch). For the sexual in populations carrying sexually antagonistic alleles and to conflict locus, we consider two different causes of sexual antag- onistic selection. In the first scenario, sexual conflict arises from determine whether the conclusions of past work regarding IASC, and we assume that an individual with allelic value x at mate choice under GEIs are robust to the addition of co- the locus has the condition component jiÁA ¼ x if the individual evolving sexual conflict loci. We consider a range of is male, and jiÁA ¼ 1 2 x (i[fN, Bg) if the individual is female. assumptions about sex-specific dispersal, the scale of compe- In the second scenario, sexual conflict arises from different tition, and the relative importance of IASC and local environmental optima for males and females. In this case, we adaptation to individual condition. consider an independent environmental state E0 (details in electronic supplementary material, §A), and males have the 0 condition component jiÁA ¼ E while females have jiÁA ¼ 1 2 E0 (i[fN, Bg), so that male condition is better at higher E0 2. Material and methods values while female condition is better at lower E0 values. For males, condition affects fitness by influencing the prob- (a) The meta-population ability of mating, assuming that at least one of the females in We model a population of sexually reproducing, dioecious the patch is choosy. We model the relative preference of a haploids subdivided over K discrete habitat patches. The habitat female for a male of condition j using a beta distribution func- patches are arranged linearly in a ring, such that every patch has tion B(j; a, b) ¼ G(a)G(b)=G(a þ b), where j [(0, 1), and a and two neighbours and there are no ‘edge effects’. Each patch has an b are female preference factors that determine the shape of environmental state E, which can be thought of as an ecological the preference curve. Random mating occurs when a ¼ b ¼ 1. variable that affects fitness.
Recommended publications
  • The Effect of Purging on Sexually Selected Traits Through Antagonistic Pleiotropy with Survival Geir H
    The effect of purging on sexually selected traits through antagonistic pleiotropy with survival Geir H. Bolstad1, Christophe Pelabon´ 1, Line-K. Larsen2, Ian A. Fleming3, Aslaug˚ Viken2 & Gunilla Rosenqvist1 1Centre for Conservation Biology, Department of Biology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, 7491 Trondheim, Norway 2Norwegian Biodiversity Information Centre, Erling Skakkes gt. 47, 7491 Trondheim, Norway 3Ocean Sciences Centre, Memorial University of Newfoundland, St. John’s, Newfoundland, Canada A1C 5S7 Keywords Abstract Contextual analysis, genetic architecture, genetic correlation, Poecilia reticulata, Sexuallyselectedtraitsareexpectedtoevolvetoapointwheretheirpositiveeffect sexually selected traits. on reproductive success is counterbalanced by their negative effect on survival. At the genetic level, such a trade-off implies antagonistic pleiotropy between sur- Correspondence vival and the expression of sexually selected traits. Yet, the consequences of such Geir H. Bolstad, Centre for Conservation a genetic architecture have been largely overlooked in studies examining how in- Biology, Department of Biology, Norwegian breeding influences sexually selected traits. These studies have solely interpreted University of Science and Technology, 7491 Trondheim, Norway. Tel: +47 92 03 76 their results as an effect of increased homozygosity. An alternative, however, is 65; Fax: +47 735 96100; that purging of recessive alleles deleterious for survival when inbreeding increases E-mail: [email protected] can negatively affect the expression of sexually selected traits through antagonistic pleiotropy. We tested this hypothesis by analyzing the effects of inbreeding on sev- Received: 15 November 2011; Revised: 24 eral male ornaments and life-history traits across 20 captive populations of guppies February 2012; Accepted: 24 February 2012 (Poecilia reticulata) with varying levels of inbreeding. Only one ornament, orange area, decreased in its expression with an increasing level of inbreeding.
    [Show full text]
  • Towards a Resolution of the Lek Paradox
    letters to nature discussions regarding experimental design. We also thank G. Taylor from the Oregon State mating. The probability of mating was strongly affected by Climate Center for help with obtaining the climate data. J. A. Pounds, E. Post, M. Vronsky the male's courtship rate (logistic regression x2 79:76, n 232, and N. Chevoteravich provided helpful suggestions on this manuscript. Funding was 1 provided by the Declining Amphibian Population Task Force Seed Grant (to J.M.K.); P , 0.0001; effect size estimated as Pearson's correlation coef®cient NIH/NSF Ecology of Infectious Diseases grant (to J.M.K.); and the Department of Biology, r 0:6; Fig. 1). To ensure that this result was robust and not biased Pennsylvania State University. by measures derived from related males, we replicated the experi- Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to J.M.K. ment using males collected from a ®eld population: this replication 2 (e-mail: [email protected]). con®rmed the above result (logistic regression x1 43:30, n 80, P , 0.0001; effect size r 0:7). The preference function shown in Fig. 1 resulted in moderate directional selection for higher court- ship rate (intensity of directional selection (i) with 95% con®dence ................................................................. intervals (CI) and signi®cance test: i 0:331, CIlower 0:121, 22,23 CIupper 0:491; t400 3:20, P 0:0015; methods as described ). Towards a resolution We experimentally examined the condition-dependence of court- ship rate by manipulating the condition of males through their of the lek paradox nutritional state.
    [Show full text]
  • View of Sexual Selection
    Evol Biol DOI 10.1007/s11692-015-9359-y ESSAY A Paradox of Genetic Variance in Epigamic Traits: Beyond ‘‘Good Genes’’ View of Sexual Selection 1 2,3 2 Jacek Radwan • Leif Engqvist • Klaus Reinhold Received: 29 June 2015 / Accepted: 4 November 2015 Ó The Author(s) 2015. This article is published with open access at Springerlink.com Abstract Maintenance of genetic variance in secondary Introduction sexual traits, including bizarre ornaments and elaborated courtship displays, is a central problem of sexual selection Darwin (1871) developed the theory of sexual selection to theory. Despite theoretical arguments predicting that strong explain the evolution of traits such as exaggerated antlers sexual selection leads to a depletion of additive genetic or bizarre plumage, which appeared inconsistent with the variance, traits associated with mating success show rela- idea of ‘‘survival of the fittest’’. Darwin argued that sur- tively high heritability. Here we argue that because of vival costs associated with the possession of such traits are trade-offs associated with the production of costly epi- compensated by increased success in reproductive com- gamic traits, sexual selection is likely to lead to an petition, either because these traits serve as weapons in increase, rather than a depletion, of genetic variance in intra-sexual competition, or because they make their those traits. Such trade-offs can also be expected to con- bearers more sexually attractive. tribute to the maintenance of genetic variation in ecologi- Maintenance of genetic variation in sexually selected cally relevant traits with important implications for traits has attracted much attention from evolutionary biol- evolutionary processes, e.g.
    [Show full text]
  • Consistent Positive Co-Variation Between Fluctuating Asymmetry and Sexual Trait Size: a Challenge to the Developmental Instability-Sexual Selection Hypothesis
    Symmetry 2015, 7, 976-993; doi:10.3390/sym7020976 OPEN ACCESS symmetry ISSN 2073-8994 www.mdpi.com/journal/symmetry Article Consistent Positive Co-Variation between Fluctuating Asymmetry and Sexual Trait Size: A Challenge to the Developmental Instability-Sexual Selection Hypothesis Michal Polak *, Kassie J. Hooker and Frances Tyler Department of Biological Sciences, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH 45221-0006, USA; E-Mails: [email protected] (K.J.H.); [email protected] (F.T.) * Author to whom correspondence should be addressed; E-Mail: [email protected]; Tel.: +1-513-556-9736. Academic Editor: John H. Graham Received: 9 March 2015 / Accepted: 1 June 2015 / Published: 3 June 2015 Abstract: The developmental instability (DI)-sexual selection hypothesis proposes that large size and symmetry in secondary sexual traits are favored by sexual selection because they reveal genetic quality. A critical prediction of this hypothesis is that there should exist negative correlations between trait fluctuating asymmetry (FA) and size of condition dependent sexual traits; condition dependent traits should reveal an organism’s overall health and vigor, and be influenced by a multitude of genetic loci. Here, we tested for the predicted negative FA-size correlations in the male sex comb of Drosophila bipectinata. Among field-caught males from five widely separated geographic localities, FA-size correlations were consistently positive, despite evidence that sex comb size is condition dependent. After controlling for trait size, FA was significantly negatively correlated with body size within several populations, indicating that developmental instability in the comb may reveal individual genetic quality. We suggest the possibility that condition dependent traits in some cases tap into independent units of the genome (a restricted set of genes), rather than signaling overall genetic properties of the organism.
    [Show full text]
  • Condition-Dependence, Genotype-By-Environment Interactions and the Lek Paradox
    Genetica (2008) 132:209–216 DOI 10.1007/s10709-007-9166-1 Condition-dependence, genotype-by-environment interactions and the lek paradox Hanna Kokko Æ Katja Heubel Received: 6 February 2007 / Accepted: 13 June 2007 / Published online: 7 July 2007 Ó Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2007 Abstract The lek paradox states that maintaining genetic fitness; the net balance of these two effects can be positive variation necessary for ‘indirect benefit’ models of female or negative, and often strongly context-dependent. choice is difficult, and two interrelated solutions have been proposed. ‘Genic capture’ assumes condition-dependence Keywords Lek paradox Á Genotype-by-environment of sexual traits, while genotype-by-environment interac- interaction Á Migration Á Condition-dependence Á Female tions (GEIs) offer an additional way to maintain diversity. choice However, condition-dependence, particularly with GEIs, implies that environmental variation can blur the relation- ship between male displays and offspring fitness. These Introduction issues have been treated separately in the past. Here we combine them in a population genetic model, and show that After decades of research, it is still debated to what extent predictions change not only in magnitude but also in females can gain indirect (i.e. genetic) benefits through direction when the timing of dispersal between environ- mate choice (e.g. Byers and Waits 2006; Kokko et al. 2006; ments relative to the life cycle is changed. GEIs can dra- Qvarnstro¨m et al. 2006). A central problem is that of the lek matically improve the evolution of costly female paradox: if female preferences consistently favour some preferences, but also hamper it if much dispersal occurs males over others, the favoured genotypes should spread between the life history stage where condition is deter- until genetic variation is eroded (Kirkpatrick and Ryan mined and mating.
    [Show full text]
  • The Landekirkpatrick Mechanism Is
    PERSPECTIVE doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.01054.x THE LANDE–KIRKPATRICK MECHANISM IS THE NULL MODEL OF EVOLUTION BY INTERSEXUAL SELECTION: IMPLICATIONS FOR MEANING, HONESTY, AND DESIGN IN INTERSEXUAL SIGNALS Richard O. Prum1,2 1Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, and Peabody Natural History Museum, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut 06520-8150 2E-mail: [email protected] Received December 31, 2009 Accepted May 26, 2010 The Fisher-inspired, arbitrary intersexual selection models of Lande (1981) and Kirkpatrick (1982), including both stable and unstable equilibrium conditions, provide the appropriate null model for the evolution of traits and preferences by intersexual selection. Like the Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium, the Lande–Kirkpatrick (LK) mechanism arises as an intrinsic consequence of genetic variation in trait and preference in the absence of other evolutionary forces. The LK mechanism is equivalent to other intersexual selection mechanisms in the absence of additional selection on preference and with additional trait-viability and preference-viability correlations equal to zero. The LK null model predicts the evolution of arbitrary display traits that are neither honest nor dishonest, indicate nothing other than mating availability, and lack any meaning or design other than their potential to correspond to mating preferences. The current standard for demonstrating an arbitrary trait is impossible to meet because it requires proof of the null hypothesis. The LK null model makes distinct predictions about the evolvability of traits and preferences. Examples of recent intersexual selection research document the confirmationist pitfalls of lacking a null model. Incorporation of the LK null into intersexual selection will contribute to serious examination of the extent to which natural selection on preferences shapes signals.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolutionary Optimum for Male Sexual Traits Characterized Using the Multivariate Robertson–Price Identity
    Evolutionary optimum for male sexual traits characterized using the multivariate Robertson–Price Identity Matthieu Delcourta, Mark W. Blowsb, J. David Aguirreb, and Howard D. Rundlea,1 aDepartment of Biology, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, ON, Canada K1N 6N5; and bSchool of Biological Sciences, University of Queensland, Brisbane QLD 4072, Australia Edited by Mark Kirkpatrick, University of Texas, Austin, TX, and accepted by the Editorial Board April 22, 2012 (received for review October 12, 2011) Phenotypes tend to remain relatively constant in natural popula- dependent male contact pheromone display consisting of a suite tions, suggesting a limit to trait evolution. Although stationary of cuticular hydrocarbons (CHCs) (25). CHCs are a direct target phenotypes suggest stabilizing selection, directional selection is of female mate preferences (SI Text), and artificial selection on more commonly reported. However, selection on phenotypes will them in the direction of these preferences conferred higher male have no evolutionary consequence if the traits do not genetically mating success over control populations. However, the response covary with fitness, a covariance known as the Robertson–Price to selection was stopped after seven generations by opposing Identity. The nature of this genetic covariance determines if phe- natural selection, despite a substantial increase in additive ge- notypes will evolve directionally or whether they reside at an netic variance as the new evolutionary limit was approached (14). evolutionary optimum. Here, we show how a set of traits can be This experiment showed that alleles conferring higher male shown to be under net stabilizing selection through an application mating success were segregating at low frequencies in the base of the multivariate Robertson–Price Identity.
    [Show full text]
  • A Proximate Perspective on the Cooperative Behavior of a Lekking Passerine
    A Proximate Perspective on the Cooperative Behavior of a Lekking Passerine Ben Joseph Vernasco Dissertation submitted to the faculty of the Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy In Biological Sciences Ignacio T. Moore, Chair Thomas B. Ryder Kendra B. Sewall Joel W. McGlothlin Jeffrey R. Walters August 20th 2019 Blacksburg, Virginia Keywords: cooperation, testosterone, androgens, telomere, biological age, lek, manakin, cooperative display coalition © 2019 Ben Joseph Vernasco All Rights Reserved A Proximate Perspective on the Cooperative Behavior of a Lekking Passerine Ben Joseph Vernasco ABSTRACT Elucidating the mechanisms responsible for driving individual variation in behavior is a foundational question in organismal biology. Answering these types of questions is necessary for understanding how tradeoffs are mediated as well as potential constraints on evolutionary responses to selection. In Chapter I, I synthesize the evidence suggesting that testosterone plays a central role in driving individual variation in cooperative reproductive behaviors and mediating the tradeoff between cooperation and competition. The subsequent chapters of my dissertation then focus on understanding the mechanistic sources of individual variation in the cooperative courtship behaviors of male wire-tailed manakins (Pipra filicauda), a Neotropical lekking songbird. Wire-tailed manakins exhibit cooperative courtship display behaviors wherein both floater and territory-holding
    [Show full text]
  • Condition-Dependent Sexual Selection Can Accelerate Adaptation
    Evolutionary Ecology Research, 2003, 5: 867–881 Condition-dependent sexual selection can accelerate adaptation Patrick D. Lorch,* Stephen Proulx,‡ Locke Rowe and Troy Day§ Department of Zoology, University of Toronto, Ramsay Wright Labs, 25 Harbord Street, Toronto, Ontario, M5S 3G5, Canada ABSTRACT Sexual selection is responsible for much of the spectacular natural diversity of mating traits. It is unclear, however, how this powerful evolutionary force affects the evolution of traits unrelated to mating. Four recent theoretical studies have argued that sexual selection might increase the rate of adaptation, but each relies on the assumption of a substantial positive covariance between male condition (non-mating fitness) and display. Here we demonstrate, with an explicit genetic model, that sexual selection itself can easily lead to the evolution and maintenance of this covariance. This process occurs through the evolution of condition- dependent male display and the resultant transfer of genetic variance for condition into variance in male display. We also track the effect of the covariance between condition and display on the rate of adaptation. Our results demonstrate a powerful synergy between natural and sexual selection that can elevate population mean fitness. Moreover, this synergy can greatly accelerate the rate of adaptation, making the feedback between natural and sexual selection a particularly potent force in changing environments. This has important implications for several key evolutionary processes, including the evolution of sex, sexual conflict and speciation. Keywords: Fisher process, good genes, individual-based model, simulation. INTRODUCTION Sexual selection has generally been viewed as reducing population mean fitness (Haldane, 1932; Lande, 1980; Kirkpatrick, 1982; Grafen, 1990b; Kirkpatrick and Ryan, 1991; Price et al., 1993; Tanaka, 1996; McLain et al., 1999; Gavrilets et al., 2001; Houle and Kondrashov, 2001).
    [Show full text]
  • Genic Capture and Resolving the Lek Paradox
    Review TRENDS in Ecology and Evolution Vol.19 No.6 June 2004 Genic capture and resolving the lek paradox Joseph L. Tomkins1, Jacek Radwan2, Janne S. Kotiaho3 and Tom Tregenza4 1Environmental and Evolutionary Biology, Dyer’s Brae House, The Mitchell Building, University of St Andrews, Fife, UK, KY16 9TH 2Institute of Environmental Sciences, Jagiellonian University, ul. Ingardena 6, 30-060 Cracow, Poland 3Department of Biological and Environmental Science, University of Jyva¨ skyla¨ , PO Box 35, 40014 Jyva¨ skyla¨ , Finland 4Ecology and Evolution Group, School of Biology, University of Leeds, UK, LS2 9JT The genic capture hypothesis offers a resolution to the sexual display traits mean that mutations can indeed question of how genetic variation in male sexually maintain variation in the face of DIRECTIONAL SELECTION selected traits is maintained in the face of strong female created by female mate choice. preferences. The hypothesis is that male display traits Houle [14] argued that the relatively high genetic are costly to produce and hence depend upon overall variance of fitness-related life-history traits could be condition, which itself is dependent upon genes at explained by their dependence on many underlying many loci. Few attempts have been made to test the physiological and morphological traits, such that they assumptions and predictions of the genic capture sum genetic variation over many loci. This multitude of hypothesis rigorously and, in particular, little attention loci provides a large mutational target [15] that is resistant has been paid to determining the genetic basis of con- to erosion through directional selection. Similar to life- dition. Such tests are crucial to our understanding of history traits, sexually selected traits also have relatively the maintenance of genetic variation and in the evalu- high coefficients of additive genetic variation [4].
    [Show full text]
  • Evolution of Animal Genitalia: Patterns of Phenotypic and Genotypic
    Genet. Res., Camb.(1998), 71, pp. 193–212. With 1 figure. Printed in the United Kingdom # 1998 Cambridge University Press 193 Evolution of animal genitalia: patterns of phenotypic and genotypic variation and condition dependence of genital and non-genital morphology in water strider (Heteroptera: Gerridae: Insecta) " # GO> RAN ARNQVIST * RANDY THORNHILL " Department of Animal Ecology, Uniersity of Umeac , S-901 87 Umeac , Sweden # Department of Biology, Uniersity of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87131-1091, USA (Receied 29 September 1997 and in reised form 27 January 1998) Summary Rapid and divergent evolution of male genitalia represents one of the most general evolutionary patterns in animals with internal fertilization, but the causes of this evolutionary trend are poorly understood. Several hypotheses have been proposed to account for genitalic evolution, most prominent of which are the lock-and-key, sexual selection and pleiotropy hypotheses. However, insights into the evolutionary mechanisms of genitalic evolution are hindered by a lack of relevant in-depth studies of genital morphology. We used a biparental progenies breeding design to study the effects of food stress during ontogeny on phenotypic expression of a suite of genital and non- genital morphological traits, both linear traits and multivariate shape indices, in a natural population of the water strider Gerris incognitus. In general, genitalic traits were as variable as non-genital traits, both phenotypically and genotypically. Average narrow-sense heritability of genital traits was 0±47 (SE ¯ 0±05). Further, while food stress during development had a large impact on adult morphology, and expression of genitalic traits exhibited significant levels of condition dependence, different genotypes did not significantly differ in their ability to cope with food stress.
    [Show full text]
  • Sexual Selection and the Evolution of Evolvability
    Heredity (2007) 98, 198–205 & 2007 Nature Publishing Group All rights reserved 0018-067X/07 $30.00 www.nature.com/hdy ORIGINAL ARTICLE Sexual selection and the evolution of evolvability M Petrie and G Roberts Evolution and Behaviour Research Group, School of Biology, University of Newcastle upon Tyne, Newcastle-upon-Tyne, UK Here we show that sexual selection can have an effect on the resulting decline in mean fitness. However, it also increases rate of mutation. We simulated the fate of a genetic modifier the probability of rare beneficial mutations arising, and of the mutation rate in a sexual population with and with- mating skew caused by female preferences for male subjects out sexual selection (modelled using a female choice carrying those beneficials with few deleterious mutations mechanism). Female choice for ‘good genes’ should reduce (‘good genes’) can lead to a mutation rate above that expected variability among male subjects, leaving insufficient differ- under natural selection. A choice of two male subjects was ences to maintain female preferences. However, female sufficient for there to be a twofold increase in the mutation rate choice can actually increase genetic variability by supporting as opposed to a decrease found under random mating. a higher mutation rate in sexually selected traits. Increasing Heredity (2007) 98, 198–205. doi:10.1038/sj.hdy.6800921; the mutation rate will be selected against because of the published online 22 November 2006 Keywords: mutation rate; sexual selection; mate choice; mutator genes; lek paradox; genetic variation Introduction maintained as a result of frequency-dependent or temporally and spatially varying selection (Charles- There is now evidence that female subjects can gain worth, 1987; Rice, 1988; Hartl and Clark, 1989; Kokko genetic quality benefits for their offspring when they and Lindstrom, 1996; Roff, 1998).
    [Show full text]