<<

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

ScienceDirect

Insects as models for studying the evolution of

animal

Meagan Simons and Elizabeth Tibbetts

Research on the evolution of cognition has long centered on finding stronger support for ecological selection pressures

vertebrates. Current research indicates that both complex [4,5], and other studies finding stronger support for social

social behavior and influence the evolution of selection pressures [6]. In recent years, there has been a

vertebrate cognition. provide a powerful and growing appreciation that social and ecological factors often



underappreciated model system for research on cognitive work together to influence the evolution of cognition [7 ].

evolution because they are a large group with multiple Multiple selective forces, and non-selective constraints



evolutionary transitions to complex social behavior as well as interact to shape cognitive evolution [6,7 ].

extensive ecological variation. Here, we integrate current

research on cognitive evolution in vertebrates and insects. We

In this review, we will integrate research on vertebrate

specifically highlight recent advances in vertebrate research

and cognition and highlight opportunities for

that are applicable to insects. We focus on two key topics:

future research. Cognition is the ability to acquire, pro-

1) The challenges of quantifying cognition 2) What factors

cess, store, and act on information [8]. Vertebrate studies

contribute to the evolution of cognition? Applying methods like

often use ‘intelligence’ interchangeably with ‘cognition’

comparative analysis and behavioral cognition measurement to

[9]. We use both terms in this review because using

insects are likely to provide key insight into the evolution of

similar terminology facilitates research integration across

animal minds.

vertebrates and insects. As described in more detail in the

section ‘Challenges of Quantifying Cognition’, quantify-

Address

ing and comparing cognition across taxa are challenging

University of Michigan, 1105 N. University Ave., Ann Arbor, MI 48104,

because it is difficult to fully capture cognitive variation

United States

with a single variable [10]. Nevertheless, comparative

Corresponding author: Tibbetts, Elizabeth ([email protected]) analyses provide a powerful method for studying cogni-

tive evolution [11]. There is much potential for future

work combining neuroanatomical and behavioral metrics

Current Opinion in Insect Science 2019, 34:117–122

to assess cognition in diverse insects.

This review comes from a themed issue on Social insects

Edited by Patrick Abbot and Sarah Kocher

Thus far, the vast majority of theoretical and empirical

work on cognitive evolution has focused on vertebrates.

As a result, insect and other invertebrate researchers will

benefit by using the conceptual framework developed by

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cois.2019.05.009 vertebrate researchers to understand variation in insect

2214-5745/ã 2019 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. cognition. In addition, the broader field of cognitive

evolution will benefit from incorporating insect research

because insects provide an important comparative per-

spective. Insects and vertebrates have independently

evolved complex behaviors such as cooperation, cen-

tral-place foraging, navigation, and complex communica-

Introduction tion with conspecifics [12,13]. Insects and vertebrate

There is enormous variation in cognitive complexity and also have distinct neural structures and diverged

size across species. Many explanations have been approximately 600 mya [14]. Identifying similarities and

proposed for the variation in cognition. The two best- differences between how insect and vertebrate cognition

known hypotheses are the ‘ecological intelligence’ and co-evolves with behavior will clarify how selection has

‘social intelligence’ hypotheses. The ‘ecological intelli- shaped cognition in diverse taxa, the generality of hypoth-

gence hypothesis’ proposes that the need to find and eses developed for vertebrates, as well as how cognitive

process food plays a key role in the evolution of enhanced evolution differs across distinct neural structures.

cognition [1,2]. The ‘social intelligence hypothesis’ pro- Although there has been some notable research on the

poses that large brains and enhanced cognition are favored evolution of and neuroanatomy in insects

in species that live in complex societies because individuals [15,16], we still know relatively little about the evolution

with superior cognitive capacity are better able to track of and many key hypotheses have not

many social relationships and respond appropriately [3]. been tested in insects or other invertebrates. Here, we

Both hypotheses have received support, with some studies will review existing research on cognitive evolution in

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Insect Science 2019, 34:117–122

118 Social insects

insects and describe opportunities for future research. We behavior relationships are broadly applicable across taxo-

will focus on two key aspects of cognitive evolution: nomic groups or whether there are multiple routes to

1) Challenges of quantifying cognition 2) What factors complex behavior, some of which require more informa-

contribute to the evolution of cognition? tion processing than others.

Challenges of quantifying cognition Cognitive test battery

Brain behavior relationships An alternative method for assessing intelligence is mea-

A major challenge associated with studying the evolution suring intelligence directly with a battery of cognitive

of is how to quantify cognition. Histori- tests rather than relying on proxies like relative brain size

 

cally, brain size was used as the key metric for measuring [9,18 ,45,46 ]. The major challenge associated with mea-

intelligence [37]. Whole brain size is relatively easy to suring cognition directly is that it is logistically difficult to

measure and there is some correlative evidence that design and implement appropriate cognitive tests that can

vertebrates with larger brain to body size ratios are be used across many taxonomic groups. As a result,

more intelligent than those with smaller brain to body cognitive test batteries have only been attempted in a



size ratios [38,39]. Although relative brain size studies small number of mammal [27] and bird [18 ] species. One

have been influential, relative brain size is an overbroad impressive example is Ashton et al. work on individual



and inaccurate measure of cognitive ability [40,41]. As a cognitive performance in wild Australian Magpies [18 ].

result, most current research uses more specific neuroan- Wild birds were tested on four different types of tasks:

atomical metrics as a proxy for intelligence [15]. Many inhibitory control, associative learning, reversal learning,

studies measure variation in the size of specific parts of and spatial memory. They found that performance on all

the brain to which a particular function can be ascribed four tests was correlated, suggesting that some birds have

[15,19]. For example, analysis of social intelligence focus higher general intelligence than other birds. Further,

on brain areas such as the isocortex, cerebellum, and the individuals from larger groups performed better than

forebrain (vertebrates) or and the cen- those from small groups, suggesting that living in large,

tral complex (insects) [19,42]. These metrics are more socially complex groups may promote general intelli-

informative than whole brain size as they incorporate gence. The results indicate that living in social groups

more specific analyses of selective pressure acting on may shape general cognitive development and evolution

neural function. However, it is still difficult to attribute within species. This work also highlights the value of field

neuroanatomical variation to specific behavioral traits experiments where multiple aspects of cognition are

because many areas of the brain regulate diverse measured.

behavior. For example, bird forebrain volume has been

correlated with many behaviors, including innovation Given the challenges of measuring cognition across

frequency, invasion success, social complexity, food multiple species, there have been few interspecific anal-

hoarding, and bower complexity [40]. It is not clear yses that directly measure vertebrate cognition with

how different behaviors interact to influence neural behavioral tests. In one notable study, MacLean et al.

investment specifically, or whether or not neural invest- assessed the cognitive performance of 36 mammal and

ment in specific brain regions correlates to general bird species with a problem-solving task that measured

intelligence. self-control [47]. They found that diet but not social

group size was a strong predictor of species differences in

In insects, both whole brain volume and specific neuro- self-control. This work provides intriguing support for

anatomical metrics have been used in comparative ecological rather than social factors influencing the evo-

analyses [15,19,20]. However, unlike vertebrates, neuro- lution of a cognitive skill. Subsequently, Benson-Amram

anatomical metrics linking functional differences in et al. measured problem solving in 39 mammalian carni-

cognition are still relatively understudied [43]. For exam- vore species, finding that problem solving was linked

ple, are relatively larger mushroom bodies linked with with relative brain size, but not socioecological variables

better problem solving, spatial learning, associative learn- like social complexity [38]. Although large-scale com-

ing, or a combination? A recent study by Li et al. found parative cognition research is methodologically challeng-

that microglomeruli density in the mushroom bodies was ing, these studies offer unique insight into the evolution

linked with an individual’s ability to learn and retain of cognitive skills.

information, suggesting that neuroanatomy may be asso-

ciated with functional differences in insect cognition [44]. Thus far, we lack comparative analyses that directly

Notably, some apparently complex insect behaviors may measure cognition in multiple insect species. We also

involve little information processing and minimal neural lack comparative work that explicitly examines verte-

investment suggesting there may be multiple evolution- brates and insects within the same framework. Insects

ary pathways to complex cognition [41]. Future work that have great potential for research as

directly links insect neuroanatomy with functional differ- they excel at a range of learning tasks. Honeybees are

ences in cognition will provide insight into whether brain/ capable of impressive spatial learning and navigation,

Current Opinion in Insect Science 2019, 34:117–122 www.sciencedirect.com

Insects and the evolution of animal cognition Simons and Tibbetts 119

bumblebees are able to complete multi-step learning long-term memory, inhibitory control, and motor control.

tasks for food rewards [48], and Polistes are capable If multiple aspects of cognition covary, it suggests that

of learning the individual faces of conspecifics and tran- insect cognition evolves in a domain-general manner. If

sitive inference [16,49,50]. However, insect researchers aspects of cognition are independent, it supports domain-

have yet to develop standardized cognitive tests that can specific cognitive evolution [52]. Of course, the hypothe-

be used across insect species to assess a variety of cogni- ses are not mutually exclusive. Domain-specific and

tive domains. In the future, it will be interesting to test domain general processes may interact to mediate cogni-

multiple insects on similar cognitive tasks to assess tive evolution. Understanding how both processes inter-

whether social and ecological factors are linked with act to influence cognition is key to predicting how socio-

variation in cognitive performance. For example, are ecological variables influence the evolution of animal

insects with central-place foraging better spatial learners cognition and whether this is consistent across taxa.

than species without central-place foraging? Are insects

that live in complex societies better at cognitively chal- In insects, we lack large-scale studies that compare per-

lenging tasks like problem-solving or reversal learning formance across many cognitive domains, though there is

than insects that do not live in complex societies? There is evidence for links across two cognitive domains [9].

great potential for future work testing whether social and Bombus that rapidly learn to associate a color with a reward

ecological selective pressures act in similar ways to shape are also quick to reverse this association, suggesting that

cognition across vertebrates and insects. associative learning and reversal learning are linked

[43,56]. There is also some evidence of domain-specific

Specialized versus generalized cognition cognitive differences between paper species. Polistes

Another major challenge associated with quantifying fuscatus wasps use face recognition to identify individual

intelligence is that intelligence is not one trait. It has conspecifics during social interactions and excel at learn-

multiple components that may vary independently or in ing unique wasp faces. A close relative, Polistes metricus,



concert [51 ]. One hypothesis is that selection acts does not recognize individual conspecifics and is unable

simultaneously on multiple aspects of cognition to pro- to learn unique wasp faces. Although the two species

duce domain-general differences in cognition (or general differ in their capacity for face learning, they do not differ

intelligence) [52]. The social and ecological intelligence in other types of visual learning [13]. Therefore, the social

hypotheses in Figure 1 posit that social or ecological context requiring individual face recognition influences

complexity produce domain-general differences in cog- cognition in a very specialized way rather than favoring



nition. Multiple studies in mammals [52] and birds [18 ] large-scale differences in visual discrimination learning.

support domain general cognitive evolution; some indi- This work provides promising evidence that standardized

viduals excel at multiple cognitive tasks, while other cognitive tests can be used to compare multiple insect

perform poorly on multiple tasks. For example, in wild species. Applying similar methods to additional taxa,

New Zealand robins, performance on six distinct cogni- contexts, and questions may provide insight into the

tive tasks were highly correlated, suggesting that a evolution of insect cognition.

general cognitive factor underpins cognitive perfor-

mance on multiple different types of tasks. [53]. The Factors associated with variation in cognition

alternative is that selection acts independently on the Over the past decades, the question of why some animals

specific cognitive modules involved in a particular task are more intelligent than others has received much atten-

to produce specialized differences in cognition. There is tion, with data supporting many different hypotheses.

some support for domain-specific cognitive differences Early work often focused on a single hypothesis in a small

[45]. For example, birds that store and retrieve seeds taxonomic group. However, recent vertebrate studies are

have specialized cognitive differences [54]; they excel at using more sophisticated phylogenetic analyses, large

spatial learning, but are no better at other tasks than non- sample sizes, and testing how multiple variables interact

caching species. Overall, there is evidence that selection to influence cognition. Most of this work quantifies intel-

acts on vertebrates to produce both domain-specific and ligence as relative brain size. For example, DeCasien et al.

domain-general cognitive differences, though we still analyzed over 140 species of primates, testing whether

have much to learn about the relative importance of multiple measures of sociality and ecology explain varia-

these processes. tion in brain size [4]. They found that brain size is

predicted by diet rather than sociality, suggesting that

Little is known about whether insect cognition evolves in ecology may play a key role in cognitive evolution. This

domain-general or domain-specific manner. However study highlights the value of large-scale comparative

some recent work suggests behavioral flexibility in insects analyses that simultaneously test multiple hypotheses.

is linked to domain-general cognition [55]. Testing this

hypothesis requires assessing insect cognition across A relatively small number of comparative analyses have

multiple domains, for example, behavioral flexibility, tested the factors associated with insect neural invest-

associative learning, reversal learning, spatial memory, ment, though the results of these studies have been

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Insect Science 2019, 34:117–122

120 Social insects

Figure 1

Current Opinion in Insect Science

Table reviewing hypotheses for the evolution of cognition and support for the hypotheses in vertebrates and insects. Note that hypotheses may be



complementary rather than mutually exclusive. Hypotheses referenced include the Social Brain Hypothesis [17,18 ,19–22], Distributed Cognition

Hypothesis [23,24], Cooperative Breeding Hypothesis [25,26], Cognitive Buffer Hypothesis[27], Relationship Intelligence Hypothesis [28], Ecological



Intelligence Hypothesis [2,4,10,29,30], Predator–Prey Interactions [31 ,32–34], and Mate Choice [35,36].

promising. All three studies focused on mushroom bodies, development in the sweat , Megalopta genalis, with

an area of the insect brain associated with sensory from social groups having relatively larger mushroom

integration and learning. A 2010 study using sweat bees bodies than solitary bees [19]. The ‘distributed cognition

found intriguing support for the social brain hypothesis. hypothesis’ is the only hypothesis initially developed in

They found that sociality influenced mushroom body insects. It proposes that large-colony insects will have less

Current Opinion in Insect Science 2019, 34:117–122 www.sciencedirect.com

Insects and the evolution of animal cognition Simons and Tibbetts 121

temporal complexity of chimpanzee food: how cognitive

neural investment than smaller colony insects because

adaptations can counteract the ephemeral nature of ripe fruit.

large-colony insects have lower individual behavioral Am J Primatol 2016, 78:626-645.

flexibility [57]. Consistent with distributed cognition,

3. Dunbar RIM: The social brain hypothesis. Evol Anthropol Issues

solitary wasps had relatively larger mushroom bodies than News Rev 1998, 6:178-190.

social wasps. The distributed cognition hypothesis has

4. DeCasien AR, Williams SA, Higham JP: Primate brain size is

also been supported by vertebrate work in mole rats [24]. predicted by diet but not sociality. Nat Ecol Evol 2017, 1:0112.

Eusocial naked mole rats that live in large colonies have 5. Powell LE, Isler K, Barton RA: Re-evaluating the link between

brain size and behavioural ecology in primates. Proc R Soc B

lower neural investment than solitary relatives. Finally,

Biol Sci 2017, 284 pii: 20171765.

research by Farris supports the idea that large mushroom

6. Holekamp KE: Questioning the social intelligence hypothesis.

bodies arose in parasitoid wasps, approximately 90 million

Trends Cogn Sci 2007, 11:65-69.

years before the evolution of sociality [29]. These results

7. Gonza´ lez-Forero M, Gardner A: Inference of ecological and

suggest that ecology, especially cognitive demands of

 social drivers of human brain-size evolution. Nature 2018,

finding hosts, rather than sociality may have driven the 557:554-557.

The authors demonstrate that social intelligence hypotheses do not fully

evolution of large, elaborate mushroom bodies in

account for the evolution of enhanced cognition, suggesting that ecolo-

Hymenoptera. gical factors may play a larger role. They conclude that the most likely

answer is a combination of both ecological and social factors.

An important goal of future work will be to test how 8. Shettleworth S: Cognition, Evolution, and Behavior. 198 Madison

Avenue, New York, New York, 10016: Oxford University Press;

multiple factors interact to influence insect brain size

2010.

(Figure 1). In addition, many hypotheses originally devel-

9. Shaw RC, Schmelz M: Cognitive test batteries in animal

oped for the evolution of vertebrate cognition could

cognition research: evaluating the past, present and future of

potentially apply to insects. For example, the predictions comparative psychometrics. Anim Cogn 2017, 20:1003-1018.

of the ‘Cooperative breeding hypothesis’ [25] are appli-

10. Rosati AG: Foraging cognition: reviving the ecological

cable to the social organization of many social insects. intelligence hypothesis. Trends Cogn Sci 2017, 21:691-702.

Other social insects are good models for testing the 11. Godfrey RK, Gronenberg W: Brain evolution in social insects:

advocating for the comparative approach. J Comp Physiol A

‘Relationship intelligence hypothesis’ [28]; the success

2019, 205:13-32.

of small-colony termites depends on the ability to coor-

12. Dyer FC: Spatial cognition: lessons from central-place

dinate and manage lifelong pair-bonds with mates.

foraging insects. Anim Cogn Nat 1998:119-154 http://dx.doi.org/

10.1016/B978-012077030-4/50057-X.

Overall, insect researchers will benefit by using

13. Sheehan MJ, Tibbetts EA: Specialized face learning is

the conceptual framework developed by vertebrate associated with individual recognition in paper wasps. Science

(80-) 2011, 334:1272-1275.

researchers to understand variation in insect cognition.

In addition, insects provide an important comparison to 14. Farris SM: Structural, functional and developmental

convergence of the insect mushroom bodies with higher brain

vertebrates because insects and vertebrates have inde-

centers of vertebrates. Brain Behav Evol 2008, 72:1-15.

pendently evolved many complex behaviors. Identifying

15. Gronenberg W, Ash LE, Tibbetts EA: Correlation between facial

similarities and differences between insect and verte-

pattern recognition and brain composition in paper wasps.

brate cognition may clarify how selection has shaped Brain Behav Evol 2008, 71:1-14.

cognition in diverse taxa as well as the generality of

16. Giurfa M: Cognition with few : higher-order learning in

hypotheses developed for vertebrates. Insects have insects. Trends Neurosci 2013, 36:285-294.

potential to be influential models for research on the 17. Dunbar RIM, Shultz S: Evolution in the social brain. Science (80-)

2007, 317:1344-1347.

evolution of animal cognition.

18. Ashton BJ, Ridley AR, Edwards EK, Thornton A: Cognitive

 performance is linked to group size and affects fitness in

Conflict of interest statement

Australian magpies. Nature 2018, 554:364-367.

Nothing declared. Using wild magpies, the authors demonstrate that birds in larger groups

have higher general intelligence than birds from smaller groups, using four

cognitive tests: inhibitory control, associative learning, reversal learning

Acknowledgement and spatial memory. This study effectively combines field techniques with

This material is based in part upon work supported by the National Science measuring cognition.

Foundation under grant number IOS-1557564.

19. Smith AR, Seid MA, Jime´ nez LC, Wcislo WT: Socially induced

brain development in a facultatively eusocial sweat bee

References and recommended reading Megalopta genalis (Halictidae). Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 2010,

Papers of particular interest, published within the period of review, 277:2157-2163.

have been highlighted as:

20. Ott SR, Rogers SM: Gregarious desert locusts have

substantially larger brains with altered proportions compared

 of special interest

with the solitarious phase. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 2010, 277:3087-

3096.

1. Byrne RW: The technical intelligence hypothesis: an additional

evolutionary stimulus to intelligence? In Machiavellian 21. Riveros AJ, Seid MA, Wcislo WT: Evolution of brain size in class-

Intelligence II. Edited by Whiten A, Byrne RW. Cambridge based societies of fungus-growing ants (Attini). Anim Behav

University Press; 1997:289-311. 2012, 83:1043-1049.

2. Janmaat KRL, Boesch C, Byrne R, Chapman CA, Gone´ Bi ZB, 22. Molina Y, Harris RM, O’Donnell S: Brain organization mirrors

Head JS, Robbins MM, Wrangham RW, Polansky L: Spatio- caste differences, colony founding and nest architecture in

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Insect Science 2019, 34:117–122

122 Social insects

paper wasps (: Vespidae). Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 40. Healy SD, Rowe C: A critique of comparative studies of brain

2009, 276:3345-3351. size. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 2007, 274:453-464.

23. O’Donnell S, Bulova SJ, DeLeon S, Khodak P, Miller S, Sulger E: 41. Lihoreau M, Latty T, Chittka L: An exploration of the social brain

Distributed cognition and social brains: reductions in hypothesis in insects. Front Physiol 2012, 3.

mushroom body investment accompanied the origins of

42. Farris SM: Evolution of complex higher brain centers and

sociality in wasps (Hymenoptera: Vespidae). Proc R Soc B Biol

behaviors: behavioral correlates of mushroom body

Sci 2015, 282 pii: 20150791.

elaboration in insects. Brain Behav Evol 2013, 82:9-18.



24. Kverkova´ K, Belı´kova´ T, Olkowicz S, Pavelkova´ Z, O’Riain MJ,

ˇ  43. Chittka L, Niven J: Are bigger brains better? Curr Biol 2009, 19:

Sumbera R, Burda H, Bennett NC, Nemec P: Sociality does not

R995-R1008.

drive the evolution of large brains in eusocial African mole-

rats. Sci Rep 2018, 8:9203.

44. Li L, MaBouDi H, Egertova´ M, Elphick MR, Chittka L, Perry CJ: A

possible structural correlate of learning performance on a

25. Burkart JM, van Schaik CP: Revisiting the consequences of

colour discrimination task in the brain of the bumblebee. Proc

cooperative breeding. J Zool 2016, 299:77-83.

R Soc B Biol Sci 2017, 284 pii: 20171323.

26. Burkart JM, Hrdy SB, Van Schaik CP: Cooperative breeding and

45. Herrmann E, Call J, Herna` ndez-Lloreda MV, Hare B, Tomasello M:

human cognitive evolution. Evol Anthropol Issues News Rev

Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition:

2009, 18:175-186.

the cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science 2007,

27. Holekamp KE, Dantzer B, Stricker G, Shaw Yoshida KC, Benson- 317:1360-1366.

Amram S: Brains, brawn and sociality: a hyaena’s tale. Anim

46. Shaw RC: Testing cognition in the wild: factors affecting

Behav 2015, 103:237-248.

 performance and individual consistency in two measures of

28. Emery NJ, Seed AM, von Bayern AM, Clayton NS: Cognitive avian cognition. Behav Processes 2017, 134:31-36.

adaptations of social bonding in birds. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol The authors use wild robins to assess potential confounding factors in

Sci 2007, 362:489-505. tests of individual variation in cognition. The findings show that in two

cognitive tests, novel motor and detour reaching, results were impacted

29. Farris SM, Schulmeister S: Parasitoidism, not sociality, is

by previous experience and motivational state. This study highlights the

associated with the evolution of elaborate mushroom bodies

importance of addressing these factors in future field tests of cognitive

in the brains of hymenopteran insects. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci variation.

2011, 278:940-951.

47. MacLean EL, Hare B, Nunn CL, Addessi E, Amici F, Anderson RC,

30. Farris SM, Roberts NS: Coevolution of generalist feeding

Aureli F, Baker JM, Bania AE, Barnard AM et al.: The evolution of

and gyrencephalic mushroom bodies in insects.

self-control. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2014, 111:E2140-E2148.

Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2005, 102:17394-17399.

48. Loukola OJ, Perry CJ, Coscos L, Chittka L: Bumblebees show



31. Amodio P, Boeckle M, Schnell AK, Ostojic L, Fiorito G, Clayton NS:

cognitive flexibility by improving on an observed complex

 Grow smart and die young: why did cephalopods evolve

behavior. Science 2017, 355:833-836.

intelligence? Trends Ecol Evol 2018, 34:45-56.

The authors demonstrate that the impressive intelligence displayed by 49. Tibbetts EA: Visual signals of individual identity in the wasp

cephalopods evolved as a mechanism to avoid predation. This study Polistes fuscatus. Proc Biol Sci 2002, 269:1423-1428.

highlights the importance of invertebrates in better understanding the

50. Tibbetts EA, Agudelo J, Pandit S, Riojas J: Transitive inference in

evolution of enhanced cognition.

Polistes paper wasps. Biol Lett 2019, 15 pii: 20190015.

32. van der Bijl W, Kolm N: Why direct effects of predation

51. Ashton BJ, Thornton A, Ridley AR: An intraspecific appraisal of

complicate the social brain hypothesis: and how incorporation

 the social intelligence hypothesis. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci

of explicit proximate behavioral mechanisms might help.

2018, 373 20170288.

BioEssays 2016, 38:568-577.

The authors demonstrate the importance of intraspecific studies using

33. Craig CL: Limits to learning: effects of predator pattern and evidence from wild magpies and several other taxa. The study highlights

colour on and avoidance-learning by prey. Anim the fact that interspecific work often leads to conflicting results, and

Behav 1994, 47:1087-1099. stresses the importance of considering both genetic and developmental

factors.

34. Craig CL: Predator foraging behavior in response to perception

and learning by its prey: interactions between orb-spinning 52. Burkart JM, Schubiger MN, van Schaik CP: The evolution of

spiders and stingless bees. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 1994, 35:45-52. general intelligence. Behav Brain Sci 2017, 40:e195.

35. Chen J, Zou Y, Sun Y-H, ten Cate C: Problem-solving males 53. Shaw RC, Boogert NJ, Clayton NS, Burns KC: Wild

become more attractive to female budgerigars. Science (80-) psychometrics: evidence for ‘general’ cognitive performance

2019, 363:166-167. in wild New Zealand robins, Petroica longipes. Anim Behav

2015, 109:101-111.

36. Hollis B, Kawecki TJ: Male cognitive performance declines in

the absence of sexual selection. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 2014, 281 54. Krebs J, Sherry D, Healy S, Perry V, Vaccarino A, Anderson RC,

20132873. Aureli F, Baker JM, Bania AE, Barnard AM et al.: Hippocampal

specialization of food-storing birds. PNAS 2014, 86:1388-1392.

37. Reader SM, Laland KN: Social intelligence, innovation, and

enhanced brain size in primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2002, 55. Perry CJ, Chittka L: How foresight might support the behavioral

281(1781):20132873 http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.062041299. flexibility of arthropods. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2019, 54:171-177.

38. Benson-Amram S, Dantzer B, Stricker G, Swanson EM, 56. Raine NE, Chittka L: No trade-off between learning speed and

Holekamp KE: Brain size predicts problem-solving ability in associative flexibility in bumblebees: a reversal learning test

mammalian carnivores. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2016, with multiple colonies. PLoS One 2012, 7:e45096.

113:2532-2537.

57. O’Donnell S, Bulova SJ, DeLeon S, Khodak P, Miller S, Sulger E:

39. Deaner RO, Isler K, Burkart J, van Schaik C: Overall brain size, Distributed cognition and social brains: reductions in

and not , best predicts cognitive mushroom body investment accompanied the origins of

ability across non-human primates. Brain Behav Evol 2007, sociality in wasps (Hymenoptera: Vespidae). Proc R Soc B Biol

70:115-124. Sci 2015, 282.

Current Opinion in Insect Science 2019, 34:117–122 www.sciencedirect.com