The Hippocampus and Long-Term Object Memory in the Rat

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Hippocampus and Long-Term Object Memory in the Rat The Journal of Neuroscience, April 15, 1996, 16(8):2780-2787 The Hippocampus and Long-Term Object Memory in the Rat Norbert Vnek and Lawrence A. Rothblat Department of Psychology, George Washington University, Washington, DC 20052 Animal models of amnesia have yielded many insights into the ration lesions of the dorsal hippocampus, rats with neocot-tical neural substrates of different types of memories. Some very control lesions, and normal controls were trained on three important aspects of memory, however, have been ignored in object discrimination problems and then retrained 3 weeks later research using experimental animals. For example, to examine to measure retention. All animals showed the same level of long-term memory investigators traditionally have relied on performance during the training (acquisition) phase of testing, measures of information acquisition, which stand in contrast but the performance of animals with dorsal hippocampal injury to the measures of retention commonly used in work with fell below that of controls during retraining (retention). Taken humans. We have recently developed a behavioral paradigm together, these and our earlier results suggest that the hip- that measures both the acquisition and long-term retention pocampus and anatomically related structures are particularly of object discriminations, and found a selective retention im- important for retaining visual discriminations over long delay pairment in rats with entorhinal-hippocampal disconnection intervals. These findings may clarify the role of the hippocam- (Vnek et al., 1995). pus in nonspatial memory. The present study was designed to determine whether direct damage to the hippocampus likewise would lead to a selective Key words: acquisition; retention; amnesia; nonspatial mem- deficit in the retention of visual discriminations. Rats with aspi- ory; entorhinal cortex; hippocampus The medial portion of the temporal lobe is important for memory time (i.e., 15-20 set) to study a set of pictures, H. M. can (for review, see Murray, 1992; Squire, 1992; Jarrard, 1993; Zola- perform at the level of controls on certain memory tests (e.g., Morgan and Squire, 1993; Mishkin and Murray, 1994). Evidence yes/no recognition judgments) after delay intervals of up to 1 d, of this comes from studies of human clinical populations with but his performance drops dramatically when longer intervals amnesia and known or suspected medial temporal pathology (1 week or more) are used (Huppert and Piercy, 1979; Freed (Scoville and Milner, 19.57; Zola-Morgan et al., 1986; Braak and and Corkin, 1988). These results underscore the importance of Braak, 1993). However, because of the rarity of circumscribed measuring extended delay intervals in behavioral paradigms lesions in human patients, animal models of amnesic disorders used with experimental animals. have been developed so that more precise functional neuroana- Although some animal models of amnesia have measured long- tomical studies are possible. term memory, the tasks used to do so typically depart from those Perhaps the most widely modeled amnesic patient is H. M., who administered to human clinical populations in some very impor- underwent bilateral medial temporal resection in 1953 and whose tant respects. In studies with humans, subjects are shown a list of consequent memory functions and dysfunctions have since been stimuli, a delay interval is imposed, and a test of retention (recall studied intensively (Scoville and Milner, 1957). H. M.‘s memory or recognition) follows. In contrast, with the exception of a few appears normal within the parameters of short-term memory, but early studies (Orbach et al., 1960; Correll and Scoville, 1965) is typically grossly impaired when he is called on to remember investigations of long-term memory in experimental animals have information in excess of those parameters (Wickelgren, 1968). rarely used behavioral paradigms that assess both the acquisition Impaired long-term memory in the face of normal short-term and retention of the same set of stimuli. For example, a measure memory is in fact a defining feature of medial temporal amnesia. of long-term nonspatial memory commonly used in animal models Thus, to examine medial temporal amnesia in both human patient of amnesia is the concurrent object discrimination task, in which populations and experimental animals, it is imperative to assess the dependent variable is the amount of training needed to reach long-term memory functions. criterion on several discrimination problems (Moss et al., 1981; Behavioral paradigms used to measure memory in animal Zola-Morgan and Squire, 1985). This task is to a large extent an models commonly use visual discriminations, usually objects, as index of information acquisition, and as such is more a measure of stimuli. The validity of these tasks can be assessed by compar- learning than memory. ing the performance of lesioned animals to that of human amnesics on analogous memory tests. When given extended We have recently developed a behavioral paradigm that mea- sures both the acquisition and long-term retention of visual dis- crimination problems in rats (Vnek et al., 1995). On this task, rats Received Oct. 26, 1995; revised Jan. 4, 1996; accepted Jan. 8, 1996. This work was supported by Alzheimer’s Association Grant PRG-94-172. with entorhinal- hippocampal disconnection from angular bundle Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Lawrence A. Rothblat, Department transections show normal acquisition but impaired retention over of Psychology, George Washington University, Washington, DC 20052. a 2 week interval. The present study was conducted to determine Dr. Vnek’s present address: Section of Neurobiology, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT 06510. whether direct damage to the hippocampus would likewise lead to Copyright 0 1996 Society for Neuroscience 0270-6474/96/162780-08$05.00/O a selective retention deficit. Vnek and Rothblat l Hippocampus and Long-Term Memory J. Neurosci., April 15, 1996, 76(8):2780-2787 2781 MATERIALS AND METHODS positioned 13 cm behind the entrance ridge. Around the front (facing the Subjects. The subjects were 18 male Wistar rats (Charles River, ridge and runway) of the food well, five small holes were drilled into the Wilmington, MA). All animals were -120 d old at the start of the insert, each of which contained one 45 mg food pellet (Noyes, Lancaster, experiment and weighed between 400 and 500 gm. Animals were food- NH). These pellets were used to mask the smell of the food pellets used deprived to 85% of normal body weight during behavioral testing. to reward animals during object discrimination testing so that olfactory Dorsal hippocampal lesions were chosen instead of removing the entire cues could not be used to learn the discriminations. Between trials, hippocampus because recent studies have shown that the size of dorsal animals were restrained from traversing the runway with a removable 40 hippocampal lesions in rats is directly related to spatial memory impair- X 20 cm free standing door, which was placed 25 cm from the beginning ments whereas the size of ventral hippocampal lesions is not (Moser et of the runway. al., 1993, 1995). Based on the assumption that the relative importance of Procedure. On the object discrimination task, rats were trained on three the dorsal hippocampus can be extended to nonspatial memory, the successive object discrimination problems and then later retrained on lesions of the present study were intended to involve only this division of these discriminations to measure retention. The protocol for behavioral the hippocampus. Dorsal hippocampal lesions also can be achieved testing was identical to that of Vnek et al. (1995) except that a 3 week without damaging rhinal (i.e., entorhinal and perirhinal) cortex, which delay interval was used in the present study rather than the 2 week principally surrounds the ventral hippocampus in the rat. Avoiding dam- interval of our earlier study. For a given problem, each object from the age to this area is critical because lesions limited to rhinal cortex have test pair was placed on the food well of one of the goal arms of been found to lead to nonspatial memory impairments in both monkeys the Y-maze, although food pellets were placed only in the well that (Gaffan and Murray, 1992; Meunier et al., 1993) and rats (Otto and held the predetermined correct object. At this time, the animal was Eichenbaum, 1992; Bunsey and Eichenbaum, 1993; Mumby and Pine& placed at the beginning of the runway and was prevented from traversing 1994). the runway by the restraining door. To start a trial, the door was removed Surgery. Nine rats received bilateral aspiration lesions of the dorsal and the animal was allowed to navigate the runway and enter a goal arm hippocampus, 5 rats served as unoperated controls, and 4 rats received of his choosing. The rat was then allowed to displace the object covering lesions controlling for the cortical damage included in hippocampal the food well of the goal arm he chose. A choice, whether correct or lesions. incorrect, was dctcrmincd to have been made when the animal’s front During surgery, animals were anesthetized with a mixture of ketamine paws crossed the ridge demarcating the entrance of one of the goal arms. (90 mg/kg) and xylazine (15 mg/kg), administered by intraperitoneal The placement of the stimuli (both correct and incorrect objects) on the injection. Surgery utilized a stereotaxic device, with the rat’s head in a arms of the maze (i.e., the left/right arm assignment) was randomized to level position for all surgical procedures, and was visually guided using a preclude use of spatial cues in learning the problems. After a choice was surgical microscope. made, the animal was returned to the beginning of the runway, and the Under anesthesia, animals were placed in the stereotaxic device, an restraining door was replaced until the start of the next trial. The incision was made in the skin overlaying the dorsal surface of the skull, intertrial interval was -10 sec.
Recommended publications
  • Retrograde Amnesia for Spatial Memory Induced by NMDA Receptor-Mediated Long-Term Potentiation
    The Journal of Neuroscience, January 1, 2001, 21(1):356–362 Retrograde Amnesia for Spatial Memory Induced by NMDA Receptor-Mediated Long-Term Potentiation Vegard Heimly Brun,1 Kristin Ytterbø,1 Richard G. M. Morris,2 May-Britt Moser,1 and Edvard I. Moser1 1Department of Psychology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, 7491 Trondheim, Norway, and 2Department of Neuroscience, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh EH8 9LE, Scotland, United Kingdom If information is stored as distributed patterns of synaptic acid (CPP) was administered before the high-frequency stimu- weights in the hippocampal formation, retention should be lation, water maze retention was unimpaired. CPP administra- vulnerable to electrically induced long-term potentiation (LTP) tion blocked the induction of LTP. Thus, high-frequency stimu- of hippocampal synapses after learning. This prediction was lation of hippocampal afferents disrupts memory retention only tested by training animals in a spatial water maze task and then when it induces a change in the spatial pattern of synaptic delivering bursts of high-frequency (HF) or control stimulation weights. The NMDA receptor dependency of this retrograde to the perforant path in the angular bundle. High-frequency amnesia is consistent with the synaptic plasticity and memory stimulation induced LTP in the dentate gyrus and probably also hypothesis. at other hippocampal termination sites. Retention in a later Key words: memory; spatial memory; synaptic plasticity; hip- probe test was disrupted. When the competitive NMDA recep- pocampus; dentate gyrus; LTP; NMDA receptor; CPP; water tor antagonist 3-(2-carboxypiperazin-4-yl)propyl-1-phosphonic maze; rat; saturation; retrograde amnesia Long-term potentiation (LTP) refers to the major cellular model this idea, McNaughton et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Deciphering Neural Codes of Memory During Sleep
    Deciphering Neural Codes of Memory during Sleep The MIT Faculty has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters. Citation Chen, Zhe and Matthew A. Wilson. "Deciphering Neural Codes of Memory during Sleep." Trends in Neurosciences 40, 5 (May 2017): 260-275 © 2017 Elsevier Ltd As Published http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2017.03.005 Publisher Elsevier BV Version Author's final manuscript Citable link https://hdl.handle.net/1721.1/122457 Terms of Use Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs License Detailed Terms http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ HHS Public Access Author manuscript Author ManuscriptAuthor Manuscript Author Trends Neurosci Manuscript Author . Author Manuscript Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 May 01. Published in final edited form as: Trends Neurosci. 2017 May ; 40(5): 260–275. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2017.03.005. Deciphering Neural Codes of Memory during Sleep Zhe Chen1,* and Matthew A. Wilson2,* 1Department of Psychiatry, Department of Neuroscience & Physiology, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY 10016, USA 2Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Picower Institute for Learning and Memory, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA Abstract Memories of experiences are stored in the cerebral cortex. Sleep is critical for consolidating hippocampal memory of wake experiences into the neocortex. Understanding representations of neural codes of hippocampal-neocortical networks during sleep would reveal important circuit mechanisms on memory consolidation, and provide novel insights into memory and dreams. Although sleep-associated ensemble spike activity has been investigated, identifying the content of memory in sleep remains challenging.
    [Show full text]
  • Spatial Information Modulates the Neurocognitive Dynamics of Episodic Autobiographical Memory Retrieval
    Spatial Information Modulates the Neurocognitive Dynamics of Episodic Autobiographical Memory Retrieval by Melissa Hebscher A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Department of Psychology University of Toronto © Copyright by Melissa Hebscher, 2018 Spatial Information Modulates the Neurocognitive Dynamics of Episodic Autobiographical Memory Retrieval Melissa Hebscher Doctor of Philosophy Department of Psychology University of Toronto 2018 Abstract Episodic autobiographical memory (EAM) enables reliving past experiences, recalling the sensory information associated with that event. Spatial information is thought to play an early and important role in EAM, contributing to the dynamics of retrieval. Using a combination of behavioural, neuroimaging, and neurostimulation approaches, this dissertation aimed to examine the temporal dynamics of EAM and the role spatial information plays in its retrieval. Chapter 2 demonstrated a temporal precedence for spatial information at the behavioural level, but importantly found high individual variability in early recall of spatial information. Further, individual differences in spatial aspects of EAM were reflected in hippocampal and precuneus grey matter volumes. Chapter 3 examined the temporal dynamics of EAM at the neural level using magnetoencephalography (MEG). While cueing individuals with familiar locations altered the dynamics of retrieval, it did not confer an early neural advantage. Together with the findings from Chapter 2, these results indicate that early spatial information alters the dynamics of EAM retrieval, but does not play a ubiquitous or automatic role in EAM. This study also found that spatial perspective during EAM retrieval was associated with a well-established neural component of episodic memory recollection. Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) administered to the precuneus disrupted this association, demonstrating that this region is crucially involved in neural processing of spatial perspective during EAM.
    [Show full text]
  • Dissociating Visuo-Spatial and Verbal Working Memory: It's All in the Features
    Memory & Cognition https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-018-0882-9 Dissociating visuo-spatial and verbal working memory: It’sall in the features Marie Poirier1 & James M. Yearsley1 & Jean Saint-Aubin2 & Claudette Fortin3 & Geneviève Gallant2 & Dominic Guitard2 # The Author(s) 2018 Abstract Echoing many of the themes of the seminal work of Atkinson and Shiffrin (The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 2; 89–195, 1968), this paper uses the feature model (Nairne, Memory & Cognition, 16, 343–352, 1988;Nairne,Memory & Cognition, 18; 251– 269, 1990; Neath & Nairne, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2;429–441, 1995) to account for performance in working-memory tasks. The Brooks verbal and visuo-spatial matrix tasks were performed alone, with articulatory suppression, or with a spatial suppression task; the results produced the expected dissociation. We used approximate Bayesian computation techniques to fit the feature model to the data and showed that the similarity-based interference process implemented in the model accounted for the data patterns well. We then fit the model to data from Guérard and Tremblay (2008, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34,556–569); the latter study produced a double dissociation while calling upon more typical order reconstruction tasks. Again, the model performed well. The findings show that a double dissociation can be modelled without appealing to separate systems for verbal and visuo-spatial processing. The latter findings are significant as the feature model had not been used to model this type of dissociation before; importantly, this is also the first time the model is quantitatively fit to data. For the demonstration provided here, modularity was unnecessary if two assumptions were made: (1) the main difference between spatial and verbal working-memory tasks is the features that are encoded; (2) secondary tasks selectively interfere with primary tasks to the extent that both tasks involve similar features.
    [Show full text]
  • The Method of Loci in Virtual Reality: Explicit Binding of Objects to Spatial Contexts Enhances Subsequent Memory Recall
    Journal of Cognitive Enhancement https://doi.org/10.1007/s41465-019-00141-8 ORIGINAL RESEARCH The Method of Loci in Virtual Reality: Explicit Binding of Objects to Spatial Contexts Enhances Subsequent Memory Recall Nicco Reggente1 & Joey K. Y. Essoe1 & Hera Younji Baek1 & Jesse Rissman1,2 Received: 7 January 2019 /Accepted: 7 June 2019 # Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019 Abstract The method of loci (MoL) is a well-known mnemonic technique in which visuospatial spatial environments are used to scaffold the memorization of non-spatial information. We developed a novel virtual reality-based implementation of the MoL in which participants used three unique virtual environments to serve as their “memory palaces.” In each world, participants were presented with a sequence of 15 3D objects that appeared in front of their avatar for 20 s each. The experimental group (N = 30) was given the ability to click on each object to lock it in place, whereas the control group (N = 30) was not afforded this functionality. We found that despite matched engagement, exposure duration, and instructions emphasizing the efficacy of the mnemonic across groups, participants in the experimental group recalled 28% more objects. We also observed a strong relation- ship between spatial memory for objects and landmarks in the environment and verbal recall strength. These results provide evidence for spatially mediated processes underlying the effectiveness of the MoL and contribute to theoretical models of memory that emphasize spatial encoding as the primary currency of mnemonic function. Keywords Memory enhancement . Method of loci . Virtual reality Introduction the MoL is a complicated, time-consuming mnemonic to imple- ment, but in fact, it is rather straightforward.
    [Show full text]
  • The Three Amnesias
    The Three Amnesias Russell M. Bauer, Ph.D. Department of Clinical and Health Psychology College of Public Health and Health Professions Evelyn F. and William L. McKnight Brain Institute University of Florida PO Box 100165 HSC Gainesville, FL 32610-0165 USA Bauer, R.M. (in press). The Three Amnesias. In J. Morgan and J.E. Ricker (Eds.), Textbook of Clinical Neuropsychology. Philadelphia: Taylor & Francis/Psychology Press. The Three Amnesias - 2 During the past five decades, our understanding of memory and its disorders has increased dramatically. In 1950, very little was known about the localization of brain lesions causing amnesia. Despite a few clues in earlier literature, it came as a complete surprise in the early 1950’s that bilateral medial temporal resection caused amnesia. The importance of the thalamus in memory was hardly suspected until the 1970’s and the basal forebrain was an area virtually unknown to clinicians before the 1980’s. An animal model of the amnesic syndrome was not developed until the 1970’s. The famous case of Henry M. (H.M.), published by Scoville and Milner (1957), marked the beginning of what has been called the “golden age of memory”. Since that time, experimental analyses of amnesic patients, coupled with meticulous clinical description, pathological analysis, and, more recently, structural and functional imaging, has led to a clearer understanding of the nature and characteristics of the human amnesic syndrome. The amnesic syndrome does not affect all kinds of memory, and, conversely, memory disordered patients without full-blown amnesia (e.g., patients with frontal lesions) may have impairment in those cognitive processes that normally support remembering.
    [Show full text]
  • Converging Cognitive Enhancements
    Converging Cognitive Enhancements ANDERS SANDBERGa AND NICK BOSTROMb aOxford Uehiro Centre for Practical Ethics, Faculty of Philosophy, bOxford Future of Humanity Institute, Faculty of Philosophy & James Martin 21st Century School, Oxford University, Oxford, OX1 1PT, United Kingdom ABSTRACT: Cognitive enhancement, the amplification or extension of core capacities of the mind, has become a major topic in bioethics. But cognitive enhancement is a prime example of a converging technology where individual disciplines merge and issues transcend particular local discourses. This article reviews currently available methods of cognitive enhancement and their likely near-term prospects for convergence. KEYWORDS: cognitive enhancement; cognition; intelligence; biotechnol- ogy; collective enhancement; mental training; converging technologies CONVERGING COGNITIVE ENHANCEMENTS There are few resources more useful than cognitive ability. While other resources are necessary or desirable, cognition enables them to be used for achieving personal goals. While there is little evidence that high intelli- gence causes happiness there appears to be ample evidence that low intel- ligence increases the risk for accidents, negative life events, and low income (Gottfredson 1997, 2004) while higher intelligence promotes health (Whalley and Deary 2001) and wealth. We also need better cognition in order to bal- ance an increasingly complex society where information becomes more avail- able and our actions have more far-reaching consequences (Heylighen 2002a, 2002b). There may also be an intrinsic existential value in being able to per- ceive, understand, and interact well with the world. Cognitive enhancement may be defined as the amplification or extension of core capacities of the mind through improvement or augmentation of internal or external information processing systems.
    [Show full text]
  • The Psychology of Creativity
    History of Creativity Research 1 The Psychology of Creativity: A Historical Perspective Dean Keith Simonton, PhD Professor of Psychology University of California, Davis Davis, CA 95616-8686 USA Presented at the Green College Lecture Series on The Nature of Creativity: History Biology, and Socio-Cultural Dimensions, University of British Columbia, 2001. Originally planned to be a chapter in an edited volume by the same name, but those plans were usurped by the events following the 9/11 terrorist attack, which occurred the day immediately after. History of Creativity Research 2 The Psychology of Creativity: A Historical Perspective Psychologists usually define creativity as the capacity to produce ideas that are both original and adaptive. In other words, the ideas must be both new and workable or functional. Thus, creativity enables a person to adjust to novel circumstances and to solve problems that unexpectedly arise. Obviously, such a capacity is often very valuable in everyday life. Yet creativity can also result in major contributions to human civilization. Examples include Michelangelo’s Sistine Chapel, Beethoven’s Fifth Symphony, Tolstoy’s War and Peace, and Darwin’s Origin of Species. One might conclude from these observations that creativity has always been one of the central topics in the field. But that is not the case. Although psychology became a formal discipline in the last few decades of the 19th century, it took several generations before the creativity attracted the attention it deserves. This neglect was even indicated in the 1950 Presidential Address that J. P. Guilford delivered before the American Psychological Association. Nevertheless, in the following half century the field could claim two professional journals – the Journal of Creative Behavior and the Creativity Research Journal – several handbooks (e.g., Sternberg, 1999), and even a two-volume Handbook of Creativity (Runco & Pritzker, 1999).
    [Show full text]
  • A Virtual Reality Memory Palace Variant Aids Knowledge Retrieval from Scholarly Articles
    1 A Virtual Reality Memory Palace Variant Aids Knowledge Retrieval from Scholarly Articles Fumeng Yang, Jing Qian, Johannes Novotny, David Badre, Cullen D. Jackson, and David H. Laidlaw Abstract—We present exploratory research of virtual reality techniques and mnemonic devices to assist in retrieving knowledge from scholarly articles. We used abstracts of scientific publications to represent knowledge in scholarly articles; participants were asked to read, remember, and retrieve knowledge from a set of abstracts. We conducted an experiment to compare participants’ recall and recognition performance in three different conditions: a control condition without a pre-specified strategy to test baseline individual memory ability, a condition using an image-based variant of a mnemonic called a “memory palace,” and a condition using a virtual reality-based variant of a memory palace. Our analyses show that using a virtual reality-based memory palace variant greatly increased the amount of knowledge retrieved and retained over the baseline, and it shows a moderate improvement over the other image-based memory palace variant. Anecdotal feedback from participants suggested that personalizing a memory palace variant would be appreciated. Our results support the value of virtual reality for some high-level cognitive tasks and help improve future applications of virtual reality and visualization. Index Terms—Virtual Reality, Mnemonic Devices, Natural Language Documents, Human Memory, Spatialization, Spatial Memory F 1 INTRODUCTION UR memory is imperfect. We easily forget the names of does not provide good insights for knowledge workers, who O people we meet and the content of papers we read [1]. usually face much longer and more complicated documents.
    [Show full text]
  • Stories to Make Us Human: Twenty-First-Century Dystopian
    MELISSA CRISTINA SILVA DE SÁ Stories to Make Us Human: Twenty-First-Century Dystopian Novels by Women BELO HORIZONTE 2020 MELISSA CRISTINA SILVA DE SÁ Stories to Make Us Human: Twenty-First-Century Dystopian Novels by Women Tese de doutorado apresentada ao Programa de Pós-Graduação em Estudos Literários da Facul- dade de Letras da Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais, como requisito parcial para obtenção do título de Doutora em Letras: Estudos Literários. BELO HORIZONTE 2020 To the ones who dare to dream new worlds. Acknowledgements Research as extensive as the one required for a Ph.D. dissertation cannot be done without the support of many people and institutions. I want to express my gratitude to all the ones that were part of this process for their patience and unconditional dedication. Without any particular order, I recognize the importance of the following: Instituto Federal de Minas Gerais – IFMG – for the eighteen-month paid leave that allowed me to do my research. I also thank my fellow professors at the institution, namely Anderson de Souto and Thadyanara Martinelli, who spent their time talking to me about the crazy new worlds I studied between classes. Professor Julio Jeha, my advisor, who helped me to refine all my arguments and consider diverse viewpoints. I thank you for making me a better and more mature researcher. Also, the meetings at Intelligenza were quite memorable. Diego Malachias, my husband and colleague, whom I met at the beginning of this journey. What a story we have to tell! Thank you for being such a fantastic companion – both in life and academia.
    [Show full text]
  • Models of Working Memory
    MODELS OF WORKING MEMORY Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control Edited by AKIRA MIYAKE AND PRITI SHAH PUBLISHED BY THE PRESS SYNDICATE OF THE UNIVERSITY OF CAMBRIDGE The Pitt Building, Trumpington Street, Cambridge, United Kingdom CAMBRIDGE UNIVERSITY PRESS The Edinburgh Building, Cambridge CB2 2RU, UK http: //www.cup.cam.ac.uk 40 West 20th Street, New York, NY 10011-4211, USA http: //www.cup.org 10 Stamford Road, Oakleigh, Melbourne 3166, Australia © Cambridge University Press 1999 This book is in copyright. Subject to statutory exception and to the provisions of relevant collective licensing agreements, no reproduction of any part may take place without the written permission of Cambridge University Press. First published 1999 Printed in the United States of America Typeface Stone Serif 9/12 pt. System QuarkXpress™ [HT] A catalog record for this book is available from the British Library. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Models of working memory : mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control / edited by Akira Miyake, Priti Shah. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and indexes. ISBN 0-521-58325-X. – ISBN 0-521-58721-2 (pbk.) 1. Short-term memory. I. Miyake, Akira, 1966– . II. Shah, Priti, 1968– . BF378.S54M63 1999 153.1´3 – dc21 98–35134 CIP ISBN 0 521 58325 X hardback ISBN 0 521 58721 2 paperback 1 Models of Working Memory An Introduction PRITI SHAH AND AKIRA MIYAKE Working memory plays an essential role in complex cognition. Everyday cog- nitive tasks – such as reading a newspaper article, calculating the appropriate amount to tip in a restaurant, mentally rearranging furniture in one’s living room to create space for a new sofa, and comparing and contrasting various attributes of different apartments to decide which to rent – often involve mul- tiple steps with intermediate results that need to be kept in mind temporarily to accomplish the task at hand successfully.
    [Show full text]
  • Openness to Experience: a Four-Factor Model and Relations to Creative Achievement in the Arts and Sciences
    SCOTT BARRY KAUFMAN Opening up Openness to Experience: A Four-Factor Model and Relations to Creative Achievement in the Arts and Sciences ABSTRACT Openness to experience is the broadest personality domain of the Big Five, includ- ing a mix of traits relating to intellectual curiosity, intellectual interests, perceived intelligence, imagination, creativity, artistic and aesthetic interests, emotional and fan- tasy richness, and unconventionality. Likewise, creative achievement is a broad con- struct, comprising creativity across the arts and sciences. The aim of this study was to clarify the relationship between openness to experience and creative achievement. Toward this aim, I factor analyzed a battery of tests of cognitive ability, working mem- ory, Intellect, Openness, affect, and intuition among a sample of English Sixth Form students (N = 146). Four factors were revealed: explicit cognitive ability, intellectual engagement, affective engagement, and aesthetic engagement. In line with dual- process theory, each of these four factors showed differential relations with personality, impulsivity, and creative achievement. Affective engagement and aesthetic engagement were associated with creative achievement in the arts, whereas explicit cognitive ability and intellectual engagement were associated with creative achievement in the sciences. The results suggest that the Intellectual and Openness aspects of the broader openness to experience personality domain are related to different modes of information processing and predict different
    [Show full text]