<<

Does intellectual ability change uniformly through- out adulthood or are different patterns of ability present over the lifespan? What accounts for indi- MIND vidual differences in age-related changes in cogni- tive ability, especially in late life? Can cognitive ALERT decline with increasing age be reversed by educa- tional intervention? In this monograph, Dr. K. Warner Schaie and Dr. Sherry L. Willis discuss their findings from the Seattle Longitudinal Study, which spans three generations and almost 50 years.

American Society on Aging 833 Market St., suite 511 San Francisco, CA 94105 NTELLECTUAL UNCTIONING IN DULTHOOD Phone: (415) 974-9600 I F A : Fax: (415) 974-0300 GROWTH, MAINTENANCE, DECLINE, AND MODIFIABILITY E-mail: [email protected] 2005 SPECIAL LECTURE BY K. WARNER SCHAIE AND SHERRY L. WILLIS

A JOINT PROGRAM OF THE AMERICAN SOCIETY ON AGING AND METLIFE FOUNDATION On the cover: Lucile Wiggins, the first centenarian in the Seattle Longitudinal Study, relaxes in the home she shared with her younger sister in Bellingham, Wash. Lucile, who was in excellent health until one week before she passed away of a stroke at age 102, participated in the Seattle Longitudinal Study for more than 40 years. Photo by Charles Fick.

© 2005 American Society on Aging TABLE OF CONTENTS

MindAlert Lecture: Intellectual Functioning in Adulthood: Growth, Maintenance, Decline, and Modifiability ...... 2

About the Authors ...... 15

2005 MindAlert Awards ...... 17

About the MindAlert Program ...... 19

Past MindAlert Special Lectures ...... 20

INTRODUCTION

ognitive fitness in late life is determined by a number of factors over the life course. By Cstudying thousands of people over long periods of time in the Seattle Longitudinal Study, husband-and-wife research team K. Warner Schaie and Sherry L. Willis shed light on the differences between elders who maintain their intellectual functioning into late life and those whose cognitive abilities decline.

In the lectures transcribed in this booklet, Dr. Schaie offers data on the developmental in- fluences throughout the lifespan that affect intellectual functioning; in addition, he dis- cusses how such data can be used for the early detection of the risk of late-life dementia. Dr. Willis outlines her research on how intellectual functioning in late life can be modified through cognitive training. She also explains why certain leisure activities contribute to better cognitive fitness in late life.

Dr. Schaie and Dr. Willis presented these lectures in March 2005 at the Joint Conference of the American Society on Aging and the National Council on the Aging as part of the Mind- Alert Program. Sponsored by the American Society on Aging and MetLife Foundation, Mind- Alert is dedicated to sharing the findings of the latest research on maintaining and enhanc- ing cognitive function in later life.

Also included in this booklet are profiles of the winners of the 2005 MindAlert Awards, which recognize programs that promote mental fitness in older adults, as well as an anno-

tated list of past MindAlert monographs. ¥

1 INTELLECTUAL FUNCTIONING IN ADULTHOOD: GROWTH, MAINTENANCE, DECLINE, AND MODIFIABILITY

DR. K. WARNER SCHAIE AND DR. SHERRY L. WILLIS

Dr. Schaie: I began my career wondering bility of modifying the course of intellec- about the fact that I could observe some tual functioning as we get older, through old people who were the very font of wis- cognitive training. dom and others who were pretty decrepit, uninteresting folks. Puzzling over these METHODOLOGICAL ISSUES very different life courses, I decided that the only way to try to understand the rea- The issues I want to deal with are the sons for those differences would be to fol- differences between normal and pathologi- low the same people over long periods of cal aging, the problem of cross-sectional time. And so I started out, as everybody data vs. longitudinal data, the issue of co- does, with a cross-sectional study for my hort and generational differences in level dissertation research—but I continued of cognitive abilities and the rate of following people in my study over time. change of those abilities, and population Some of my classmates, all of whom are vs. family studies. Typically, people have retired by now except for me, claim that I of aging as a downward spiral: That is, some people think of aging as los- ing a marble a day, or a neuron a day, caus- People who are too rigid don’t do very well in late ing inevitable decline. Some people would argue that our brains have so many con- life because old age requires a lot of flexibility. nections, so much extra capacity, that this model of aging can’t be accurate. People didn’t get it right in the first place and so lose a lot more brain tissue in accidents, I had to continue doing it over and over for example—much more than what is lost again. But in doing it over and over with aging—and they do just fine. So per- again—next year the study will be 50 haps much of the decline in cognitive abil- years old—I think we’ve learned quite a ity with aging may well be due to disuse. few things, some of which Dr. Willis and I want to share with you. But there’s also another issue, and that’s obsolescence. As it turns out, successive I want to talk about a number of issues generations have functioned at higher and that need to be understood to see how in- higher cognitive levels, and though you tellectual functioning changes over the may not decline at all from the level you life course: methodological issues; the had as a young adult, you may no longer be growth, maintenance, and decline of cog- competitive with the younger genera- nitive functioning, as well as developmen- tion—that’s obsolescence. You can also tal influences throughout the lifespan think of aging as development, because that affect it; and how longitudinal data adults are agents of their own develop- of the kind that Dr. Willis and I have col- ment—they can adopt healthy or un- lected can be used to contribute to the healthy lifestyles; they can engage in stim- early detection of risk of late-life demen- ulating activities or be couch potatoes. tia. Then, Dr. Willis is going to spend quite And then there is the issue of successful vs. a bit of time telling you about the possi- unsuccessful aging. Some people gain wis- ¥

2 dom with and function quite dle-aged and young people in our study adequately until the very end of life. Others for whom we have data at the same ages just stick to the familiar and veer away for both a parent and a grandparent. from interesting things in life—they want We’ve also worked on the early detection to maintain routines and tend to have rigid of dementia. personalities. People who are too rigid don’t do very well in late life because old Our conceptual model argues that you age is a period of very rapid change that re- cannot understand intellectual functioning quires a lot of flexibility. per se unless you understand the various influences that affect it. These influences Two kinds of data have been used to include issues of heritability—in other characterize intellectual development in words, what genetic predispositions for terms of aging. One type of study looks at cognitive ability or decline are inherited— age differences, or what we might call inter-individual variations across groups— Six abilities represent most of what is generally that’s when we take people at one point in time and compare people of different described as human : ages. In gathering such cross-sectional data, we have no whatsoever whether verbal ability, spatial orientation, inductive a particular older person was ever at the level of the younger person we’re using for reasoning, numeric ability, perceptual speed, comparison, so it’s really in a way compar- ing apples and oranges: We don’t know and verbal . about people’s specific life histories. On the other hand, if we really want to under- the effect of personality styles, current stand how people change over their life family environment, and socioeconomic course, we must look at age changes— status; and the influence of chronic disease intra-individual changes over time that on cognitive ability as people age. We’ve occur within groups. Then there’s also the looked at cognitive ability and aging in issue of generational differences. In our terms of two different end stages, by study- research, we have observed important ing late-life cognitive functioning and by changes across generations, which can be looking at the neuropathology found in au- studied either in populations or in biolog- topsy—a good many of the older partici- ically related individuals—we’ve actually pants in our study have allowed us to study done both. their brains after their death.

THE SEATTLE LONGITUDINAL STUDY Six abilities represent most of what is generally described as : In the Seattle Longitudinal Study, verbal ability (vocabulary), spatial orien- which forms the basis of what I have tation (the ability to identify where you to tell you, we’ve looked at cognitive are in three dimensions), inductive rea- changes and cognitive differences with soning (the basis of problem-solving abil- age, we’ve examined the antecedents of ity), numeric ability, perceptual speed, individual differences in aging, we’ve tried and verbal memory (the ability to recall interventions to slow cognitive aging, what you have read). We have multiple we’ve studied generational differences, tests for all of these abilities; following and we’ve done studies within families. In are some examples of how we go about fact, we have covered three generations in measuring them. For verbal ability, we use

our familial studies: We have some mid- a simple vocabulary test—participants are ¥

3 given a word and asked to find the one there are significant differences in cogni- that’s most like it. For spatial ability, we tive ability: For example, the data show ask people to rotate in space some ab- that verbal ability remains at a fairly high stract or familiar object, such as the letter level while numeric ability suffers a very F. We studied and - substantial decline. The reason we didn’t ing by asking participants to find the prin- see this effect in the cross-sectional ciple in a series—for example, given a let- data—which showed no significant ter series, people must find the letter that change in numeric ability with age—is continues the series. And for numeric abil- that while most abilities have shifted ity, we asked people to check addition and across generations positively, numeric subtraction problems. ability has actually declined as people rely increasingly on hand calculators and the We have followed individuals in our like. In other words, the overall decline in study to get longitudinal data, and we’ve numeric ability in cohorts of younger gen- also brought in new samples of people erations made the numeric ability of older ages 25 to 81 at each point in our study generations look stronger in old age than for cross-sectional information. Cross-sec- it actually is. tional age differences that we observed at our last data point show very few age dif- DEVELOPMENTAL INFLUENCES ON ferences in verbal ability and numeric INTELLECTUAL FUNCTIONING ability in people of different ages, but in- tensive differences that are almost linear If you think that everybody changes in in nature for all of the other abilities. The the same way over time, the following is a cautionary tale. We’ve found that almost Some abilities decline with age, but no particular any pattern you might want is represented in the data, from virtual stability to very ability declines for every person. dramatic change—and we’ve found good for this. But, one has to be very problem with these cross-sectional data is careful in looking at the resulting averages that they do not take into account where as being really characteristic of any single people started from, whether they were individual. The other interesting question strong or weak in certain abilities to that people ask is, if one ability goes, begin with. As we gathered more and does everything go? Well, we have data on more data from people over time, we were the cumulative risk of decline for five dif- able to estimate the longitudinal changes ferent abilities for at least six points in that occurred within individuals, and time several years apart. (See Figure 1.) As there we found a very different story. The it turns out, chances are that by age 60 or longitudinal data suggest that there is ac- so everybody will decline on some ability. tually some gain in cognitive functioning But virtually nobody has declined on all from young adulthood through midlife, on five abilities even when they’re in their average, and that in fact anybody who 80s. So it’s not like everything goes as we shows decline prior to age 60 probably age, but some things go—and we think has some neuropathology. mostly those things go where you have not had any opportunity or good reason to We still find age differences in cognitive practice those particular skills. We can ability with the longitudinal data: After conclude, then, that patterns of aging are the 60s, decline is at first very minimal— very individual. Some abilities, on aver- but it increases steadily as people move age, decline with age, but no particular

¥ into their 80s. But even in the high 80s, ability declines for every person.

4 Here’s an interesting exercise for trying are a function of genetics: For example, if to figure out when you are going to expe- you do not have any cardiovascular or rience the first decline on a particular other chronic disease, you’re going to be ability. The time when people show a de- much at advantage in maintaining cogni- cline in a particular ability on average tive function. Also, if you live in favorable happens in the 60s—somewhat later on environmental circumstances—that is, verbal ability, somewhat earlier on spatial you have above-average education, occu- ability. But other variables also determine pational pursuits that involve high com- whether cognitive decline is going to hap- plexity, above-average income, and intact pen in the 60s for you as an individual. families—all of those things are plus For example, if you are a woman, you get points that are likely to help you maintain a credit of approximately five years, mean- intellectual function in late life. ing you are going to experience a decline five years later than the overall average for the total pop- ulation. If you are highly edu- cated, you get about a year’s credit for every year of education above the average education of the population.

On the other hand, if you have declined in flexibility—in other words, if you have become more rigid during the seven years prior to the assessment, you will get a negative credit because rigid people tend to show decline in cognitive abilities earlier than flexible people. If you have expe- rienced above-average success in your life, your individual decline is going to occur later. Many of Figure 1. Cumulative risk of decline of one or more of five mental abilities at ages 32 you may find that you are not to 88. Source: Schaie, K. W. (1989). “The hazards of cognitive aging.” Gerontologist, likely to experience a decline 29: 484–493. Reproduced by permission of the Gerontological Society of America. until very close to your death or maybe not even then, because you’re very likely to die before you are Furthermore, involvement in activities going to experience significant decline. characteristic of an intellectually stimulat- The educated portion of the population al- ing environment is a favorable factor: Peo- ways has this advantage. ple who have extensive reading habits, travel, and attend cultural and educational Why is it that people are so different in events, for example, are also at an advan- their patterns? One of the things we’ve tage. And as I mentioned earlier, having a learned from our longitudinal studies is flexible personality style at midlife has a that people have different , positive effect on maintaining cognitive and we have characterized some that are ability. Another interesting factor is being favorable for maintenance of intellectual married to an intelligent spouse. In our

functioning. Some of these experiences study, we followed a number of married ¥

5 couples over a long period of time. It turns they are in the same age range. When you out that married couples’ intellectual per- do that, you can plot the cumulative ad- formance converges over time: The spouse vantage—or disadvantage—that succes- with lower cognitive functioning early in sive generations have. Our data show the marriage moves in the direction of the some gain over a seven-year period—sub- higher-functioning spouse, cognitively stantial gain for some abilities—and loss speaking. So the bottom line is that you for others, notably for numeric ability and should marry someone who’s smarter than verbal ability. That is, right now one of you are. High levels of perceptual process- the reasons why old people look like they ing speed are also an advantage, and being have not declined on verbal ability may satisfied with one’s accomplishments is a very well have to do with the fact that plus point. So those are things that distin- they’re at an advantage at this age with guish people who function very well into respect to later-born cohorts, who may old age as opposed to those who decline. have started out with less verbal ability.

GENERATIONAL EFFECTS One of the things that over the last century has gone up virtually linearly is Now, what about the issue of trying to what we call inductive reasoning, or the deal with generational differences, of sep- basis for problem-solving skills, and we think that has to do with changes in educational prac- tice where discovery meth- ods rather than rote are emphasized, and also be- cause the accessibility of in- formation has grown dramat- ically. On the other hand, numeric ability is a function where rote learning is clearly essential, and giving up rote learning altogether is proba- bly a bad educational mis- take. For example, if you don’t have some basic nu- meric ability, then you’re in trouble—you won’t even be Figure 2. Cognitive change in biologically related individuals based on 14-year data. able to tell whether the an- Source: Data from the Seattle Longitudinal Study (Schaie, 2005). swer in your calculator really is probable or not. These so- cietal changes obviously are arating them from age differences? The very important and are linked to genera- only way that can be done is by studying tional effects on cognitive functioning. successive generations over the same age range, because then you’re holding age Some of the changes that have occurred constant, so you can tell whether people’s over generations are protective in a sense, differences stem from their generational but others cause more risk—like the level or not. To do so, you’ve got to have prevalence of obesity, which has increased samples from different generations of in- the risk of cardiovascular disease and dia-

¥ dividuals who are followed over time when betes, which have a negative effect on

6 cognitive ability. And so we have a trade- parents declined during that period, be- off. One of the things that we suspect is tween ages 60 and 67. For the younger very likely to occur is that individual vari- generation, except for numeric ability, the ability within the same generational co- abilities seem to be either stable or still hort is going to increase because of eco- going up at that age range. We’ve very re- nomics—the increasing gap between the cently finished compiling this data over a rich and the poor means the ability of people to have access to more protective factors for cognitive function, as well as to be better able to combat the risk factors for cognitive decline, will vary widely.

It’s important to understand that there are generational differences in cognitive level, but it’s also impor- tant to think about the possibility that perhaps it is reasonable for people to stay in the workforce a little longer. The basis for this argu- ment is that successive generations not only have become more able, but also the rate of cognitive de- Figure 3. Cognitive change over seven years by Apo-E allele type. Source: Data cline with age has slowed down. from the Seattle Longitudinal Study (Schaie, 2005). Looking at a group of individuals over the same age range—and I purposely chose people from ages 60 to 14-year period; it looks like for the next 74 because that’s the most probable range seven years, from ages 67 to 74, there’s in which one might argue for people stay- less of a decline for the younger genera- ing in the workforce longer—it turns out tion than was true for their parents at the there really hasn’t been all that much same age. (See Figure 2.) So from that slowing of cognitive ability. But these point of view, people could delay retire- data are from unrelated people, so this re- ment and stay in the workforce longer. sult is not as satisfactory as it could be. EARLY DETECTION OF DEMENTIA We can study generational effects even more precisely by acquiring data sets from In conclusion, I want to say just a little biologically related individuals so that we bit about early detection of dementia. can hold constant at least part of the ge- When I started this Seattle Longitudinal netic variables. And we’ve been able to do Study, nobody knew much about Alz- that—we now have something like 500 heimer’s disease, so I didn’t learn about pairs of related individuals on whom we Alzheimer’s disease when I was going to have data at relatively close ages. When school. But it turns out that the kind of we plot the same data for people who are data we have are very relevant to the early biologically related, we see a lot more evi- detection of risk for dementia because we dence for a slowing of the overall rate of can look back and see whether we could cognitive aging. The level of cognitive have identified people who now have be- ability for the children we studied is up come clinically diagnosable. We had neu-

above that of their parents, but also the ropsychologists screen our population— ¥

7 all community-dwelling elders ages 60 and when they were in their late 70s. One lady older—and we could find only about two- had what was described as one of those and-a-half percent who were probably im- super-clean brains at autopsy, with no paired and another four-and-a-half per- tangles or plaques; the other had all the cent who were borderline; there was a classical neuropathological symptoms of larger group, about 22 percent, that had Alzheimer’s disease. It appears that we something going on, but we couldn’t quite can go back to our data about 14 years put our finger on it—these older adults before people get clinically diagnosed probably should be monitored to keep with dementia and identify individuals track of their cognitive health. who might be at risk for the disease. We can do that from the kind of assessment We also were able to get information on that’s done in the normal community- Apo-E, the gene that is supposed to be as- dwelling population. I think that’s very sociated with Alzheimer’s disease. There important, because if we ever get to the are six possible Apo-E genotypes, with the point of having some prophylactic medica- presence of the E-4 allele being linked to tions for dementia, then we certainly want the disease in a dose-dependent manner. to know who is at risk so they can take We found that a fair portion of our sample the appropriate preventive measures.

We can look at data Given this overview of some of the things we’ve learned in our studies of nor- about 14 years before people get clinically mal aging, I now want to hand things over to my colleague, Dr. Willis, who is diagnosed with dementia and identify going to tell you about what we’ve done, particularly under her guidance, to try to individuals who might be at risk for the disease. see if we can slow cognitive aging in the normal population. (about 25%) have one E-4 allele, but very few people have two (less than 3%). The Dr. Willis: I’d like to talk to you about E-2/2 combination was omitted because our work on cognitive training, or staying of its low frequency of occurrence in our mentally active. Now, there are a lot of sample—people with this combination reasons for staying mentally fit in old age. tend to die early, so they don’t get One of the most important is we know that Alzheimer’s disease because they never mental ability is related to independent get to old age. When we look at the seven- functioning—the ability to read one’s year cognitive change by allele type, we medication labels, plan nutritious meals, find that it’s the people who have an E- drive—all of these are related to the cog- 2/4 or an E-4/4 allele combination who nitive abilities Dr. Schaie has told you are more likely to decline, particularly on about. Another interesting finding from memory. (See Figure 3.) These are people longitudinal research is that higher func- who are not impaired and have not been tioning on cognitive activities appears to diagnosed, but they may be on the way. be protective against cognitive impair- ment; that is, researchers have found that We’ve also been able to get some very more highly educated people have less interesting data from the people who let likelihood of Alzheimer’s disease and de- us do autopsies. We have cognitive data mentia, or experience it at a later age. for two women over a 35-year period for whom autopsies were conducted shortly We don’t know exactly why this effect

¥ after the last time we assessed them, occurs, but there are several hypotheses

8 about why cognitive activity or education new tasks. For all these reasons, staying may be protective against late-life demen- mentally fit is very important in old age. tia. One is that continuing to challenge oneself cognitively into old age may have INTERVENTION STUDIES OF a direct impact on brain development, MENTAL FITNESS contributing to dendritic growth and structural changes in the brain. Another A number of different approaches have possibility is that if one continues to been used in the study of staying mentally stimulate the brain and its mental abili- competent into old age. Observational ties, one reaches a higher level of cogni- studies, including longitudinal studies, are tive function—so one has further to de- the type that Dr. Schaie has talked to you cline before one does reach a critical about. What I’m going to be talking pri- threshold in Alzheimer’s disease or other dementias. We think we see Alzheimer’s Abilities that show relatively occurring later in people who have re- mained cognitively active because they early decline when people are in their 60s had a higher level of cognitive function- ing earlier in the lifespan. include reasoning, spatial orientation,

The other possibility to explain the pro- processing speed, and memory. tective nature of mental fitness against dementia is compensatory activities. We marily about are intervention studies, know that higher levels of education are which are experimental studies where you associated with higher levels of verbal take a group of elders and administer a ability, as well as higher levels of numeric baseline assessment, do an intervention ability—what we call crystallized abili- with them followed by a post-test, then ties—and these may compensate for some follow them for some period of time to see of the losses that one experiences in spa- if there’s maintenance in the training tial ability or even memory with age. We techniques. We’ve been lucky in the Seat- know memory is highly related to verbal tle study—in addition to this pre-test, ability because some of the strategies we treatment, post-test design, we have lon- use to remember are verbal in nature. gitudinal data on participants prior to the Again, people whose brains can compen- intervention, so we know whether they sate for losses in some abilities—fill in had declined or not prior to the interven- the gaps, so to speak—will have a higher tion we did. level of cognitive functioning overall, which seems to be protective. So main- I’m going to tell you briefly about some taining mental fitness in old age is a good of our cognitive training research, as well idea no matter what the specific mecha- as a newer approach to intervention called nism of protection against Alzheimer’s collaborative learning. Collaborative learn- turns out to be. ing involves pairs of individuals getting together and learning without a trainer— In addition, people who are more highly at the forefront of this method is the use educated, functioning at a higher level, of technology such as CD-ROMS. I’m also feel a greater sense of empowerment. going to discuss some research we’ve done They’re more willing to take on challenging using older couples. Finally, there’s the tasks; they persist at challenging tasks, whole issue of leisure activities: Are some and—because they have confidence in of the pastimes that people engage in for

themselves—they will more often take on fun related to cognitive functioning? ¥

9 The majority of work on cognitive train- to 1984, then did training with them in ing, both our own and the work of others, 1984 and tested them again. We classified has focused on abilities that show rela- people based on whether or not they had tively early decline when people are in declined in ability or had remained stable their 60s: reasoning, spatial orientation, during the 14 years prior to training. Some processing speed, and memory. There are people showed relatively little change huge individual differences, but these over the 14-year period, but we saw a pre- abilities are the ones that are more likely cipitous decline for others. One of our to show average decline at the earliest questions was, is training in a particular ability effective with people who have de- One very astute older woman said to my trainer, clined vs. people who have not declined?

“Honey, let’s get this straight—is it your problem The good news is that both those who remained stable and those who had de- or my problem we’re gonna solve?” clined over the prior 14-year period showed significant improvement as a re- ages. The most common paradigm in the sult of the training—and what’s very in- past for doing cognitive training studies teresting is that the decline group got al- from a research perspective is studying in- most back up to where they were 14 years dividuals working with a trainer, either prior to training. So we have at least par- one-on-one or in small groups. tial remediation of decline in ability as a function of training. The other good In our own work on community-dwelling news—particularly important, perhaps, for older adults, we focused on two abilities keeping people in the workplace—is that that we had already examined in the Seat- old dogs can indeed learn new tricks. As a tle Longitudinal Study: inductive reasoning result of training, the elders in the stable and spatial orientation. Inductive reason- group are functioning at a higher level ing is being able to see a pattern in a se- than they were even 14 years previously. ries of numbers or letters or some other kind of information and the ability to use Dr. Schaie told you about cohort differ- that pattern to solve other problems. Spa- ences based on generation—people start tial orientation is the ability to orient one- at different levels of ability based on their self in space. This ability is very important birth cohort. The question is, do all of these for reading a road map, for example, or for cohorts experience cognitive training ef- looking at a diagram of a building and fig- fects? The data show that indeed they do, uring out how to get to a specific room. but what’s interesting is that the cohort ef- fects remain. Even though all of the partic- COGNITIVE TRAINING: ipants in our study experienced training METHODS AND DATA gain, the training—at least at the level we offered—was not effective in wiping out We conducted five one-hour, one-on- the cohort differences in ability level. one training sessions in each participant’s home; the training procedures used the Now, some caveats. The kind of training typical educational techniques of teaching that we did originally in the Seattle Longi- people strategies, then giving them prac- tudinal Study is very effective, because it tice on problems, offering feedback, and remediates training deficits, works in dif- modeling solutions. In one of our training ferent cohorts and is maintained years studies on inductive reasoning, we looked later. We followed these people over seven

¥ at people’s scores in this ability from 1970 years after training and still saw group dif-

10 ferences between people who had been ing material to use by themselves in their trained and those who had not been homes, without a trainer; this research trained on a specific ability. So there are a was done in collaboration with Dr. Jen- lot of positive things about this training. nifer Margrett, who was a postdoctoral However, people with serious cardiovascu- student in our lab. lar disease usually show fewer training ef- fects than those without, so the effective- COLLABORATIVE LEARNING ness of cognitive training has its limits. In addition, this type of training is very ex- We had a control group that got no pensive and very time consuming. We did training, we had couples where each one-on-one training in each individual’s spouse was assigned to work on the mate- home to get a maximum effect, but it rial alone, and we had a third group where would be prohibitive to administer that the couples worked together on the train- kind of training to a national sample or to ing material. Couples were randomly as- older adults in general. So the search now signed to the three conditions. In terms of is for other ways of providing training to individuals for cog- nitive stimulation. 5

One of the things we ask ourselves is, when we do the traditional kind of training 4 where we bring in a trainer for just five one-hour ses- sions, how can we provide ad- 3 ditional cognitive stimula- tion? Rather than this traditional, intensive one-shot method, what we need is 2 what I call the drip method. The drip method involves con- tinual stimulation throughout 1 the environment in the con- text in which people live, over longer periods of time. 0 How can we provide that? Pretest Im Post Del Post We could use technology to provide that kind of continual Figure 4. Collaborative learning in older couples. Source: Unpublished data from the cognitive stimulation, but an- Seattle Longitudinal Study. other way is to use people who are in the older adult’s environment. And one of the people who initial training, we found that both the in- is in that environment is the spouse, at dividuals who trained alone and the cou- least for a number of older people. Based ples who trained together significantly im- on this thinking, one of the first ways we proved compared with the control group, moved beyond traditional training was to but there was no real difference in training look at what we call collaborative learning effects between the couples trained indi-

in older couples. We gave them the train- vidually and the couples trained together. ¥

11 Now, this result is positive in the sense verbal ability, spatial orientation, inductive that these people are learning this mate- reasoning, numeric ability, perceptual rial without a trainer, through self-instruc- speed, verbal memory—relate to being tion. We followed up six months later, and able to do these tasks of daily living? Typi- found that the couples who trained to- cally, the way the instrumental activities of gether showed greater maintenance of the daily living are measured is just by asking strategies they learned than the people people whether they can use the phone or who trained individually. (See figure 4.) take their medicine independently, for ex- We take this outcome to mean that if you ample. But we wanted a performance- train people together—and it doesn’t have based measure. So for each of the domains to be spouses, it could be peers—people of activities—health and medications, fi- serve as reminders and reinforcers of the nances, household, telephone and commu- training effect for each other even after nications, transportation, shopping, food the formal training program is over. preparation—graduate students and I found materials that older adults use in When we started this research, we went their everyday lives and developed prob- to older people in central Pennsylvania lems related to these materials. where we live and we introduced the cog- nitive fitness training by telling them that For example, a very simple problem we were going to train them how to solve would be showing an older adult a medica- tion label and asking how many doses of It’s usually in the 70s or even early 80s before we medicine can you take in 24 hours, with the answer available right there in the instruc- see significant decline in community-dwelling tions. But we could also pose a more diffi- cult problem like, “Mr. Jones is a heavy elders on activities of daily living. smoker who has a terrible cough; what should he do?” To solve this problem, you problems. One very astute older woman have to read the warning on the medica- said to my trainer, “Honey, let’s get this tion and realize that Mr. Jones should prob- straight—is it your problem or my prob- ably see a doctor after a certain amount of lem we’re gonna solve?” That was some- time rather than continue to take this med- thing like 30 years ago, and we’re still try- ication. This more difficult kind of problem ing to answer that question. We know that requires putting different pieces of the in- basic problem-solving abilities, such as formation together to solve it, a process reasoning and spatial ability, are very im- called combinatorial reasoning. portant in maintaining independent func- tioning and efficacy in old age, but how So the question is, do these basic abili- do they relate to what people actually do ties that Dr. Schaie has been examining in in their everyday lives, like making meals, the Seattle Longitudinal Study underlie the taking medicine, driving, shopping at the ability to do these kinds of everyday tasks? supermarket or using a telephone? People And what do we know about age-related who provide services to elders talk about changes in these everyday tasks as related these activities of daily living and instru- to the basic abilities, and what’s the hope mental activities of daily living, which are of enhancing people’s ability to handle considered critical to being able to func- everyday cognitive tasks? Longitudinal tion independently in our society. data for reading short documents, such as a prescription drug label vs. lengthy docu- How do the basic abilities that we’ve ments, such as an income tax form, show a

¥ studied in the Seattle Longitudinal Study— slightly longer period of stability on these

12 everyday tasks than on basic abilities like of finances. When we look back at the ma- reasoning or spatial orientation. So the terial, much of the financial material is in good news is that it’s usually in the 70s or a graph, chart, or schedule format, and we even early 80s before we see significant think that spatial ability helps people read decline in community-dwelling elders on those graphs—going down the column activities of daily living. and across the row, for example. So we have some evidence that training on these But why would the capacity to do these basic abilities does transfer to everyday activities stay stable when testing specific problem-solving. Another challenge re- abilities shows decline? Because when you lated to this training would be setting pri- give people a test like we designed to un- orities on the abilities and activities of derstand medicine-bottle labels, you find most importance to each individual, and that these everyday tasks are cognitively training people accordingly. complex, meaning they involve multiple abilities. To read and understand a pre- LEISURE ACTIVITIES scription drug label, for example, you need verbal ability, mathematical ability, Now, what about leisure activities? Do memory, and speed of processing. We we know anything about people who work think that’s why we see longer stability crossword puzzles or jigsaw puzzles and with age for everyday problem-solving how such leisure activities relate to these than for the basic abilities, because multi- very basic abilities? Indeed, fluid ability— ple abilities are involved—and, as Dr. which involves reasoning, memory, and Schaie showed you, nobody declines on all six abilities at once. Better basic abilities are related to leisure

Therefore, some compensation may be activities like game-playing and going on—if you have a deficit in reason- ing ability, for example, you can stay sta- doing crossword puzzles. ble on everyday activities longer overall because your other basic abilities are still spatial orientation—and crystallized abil- strong. So that’s the interesting point. But ity, which involves verbal ability and nu- it makes doing cognitive training on meric ability, have relatively good correla- everyday tasks much more challenging, tions with these types of leisure activities. because it means not focusing on any one The relationship with abilities varies by ability but trying to enhance all of these type of leisure activity: Doing crossword abilities, which all seem to be related to puzzles is more related to verbal ability, performance of everyday tasks. doing jigsaw puzzles to spatial ability, and the frequency of game playing is associ- We have found some preliminary evi- ated with proficiency. What we see in peo- dence that training on basic abilities, ple who do jigsaw puzzles is that the spa- such as reasoning ability, is related to a tial component reflected in fluid ability is subset of everyday tasks. We’ve found that particularly strong. people who improved their reasoning abil- ity through training did better on our We’ve found that about 25 percent of tests of everyday problem-solving related the older adults in our samples regularly to meals, household, transportation, and do crossword puzzles, but 40 percent to 50 use of the phone. And, interestingly, we percent don’t ever do those kinds of found that people who got training with things. And we found that proficiency in

spatial ability did better on management basic abilities is fairly good both in those ¥

13 who do crossword puzzles and in those training gain on basic abilities is associ- who do jigsaw puzzles—though for jigsaw ated with enhanced functioning on every- puzzles, particularly for people ages 75- day tasks. Couples can improve basic abil- plus, the proficiency is somewhat lower. ity through self-directed instruction, and So, although we have not done research there does seem to be a connection be- using jigsaw puzzles or crossword puzzles tween the quality of marital relationships as a form of intervention, we think there and the efficacy of collaborative learning. is some solid evidence that certain leisure activities are related to cognitive func- And better basic abilities are related to tioning and could be used as a form of leisure activities like game-playing and cognitive stimulation. doing crossword puzzles—activities that have the advantage of being cognitively SUMMARY challenging while being enjoyable, so peo- ple are more likely to persist in doing The majority of community-dwelling, them. Keep in mind that chronic health nondemented older adults can, through conditions may affect elders’ ability to brief training, improve their cognitive keep mentally fit, and pathologies such as ability, remediate prior decline, and main- dementia impose serious limits on the use tain the training effects for several years. of these kinds of interventions. And even In everyday tasks that involve multiple healthy elders show individual differences abilities and are cognitively complex, age- in their response to training in basic cog- related declines occur somewhat later than nitive abilities. However, we believe that decline for basic abilities—possibly be- such cognitive training can have very pos- cause of the multiple abilities involved. itive effects on mental fitness and quality Also, there’s some evidence that reliable of life for most community-dwelling elders. ¥

14 ABOUT THE AUTHORS

Warner Schaie, Evan Pugh Professor of Human Development and at The K.Pennsylvania State University and director of the Penn State Gerontology Center, is regarded as one of the foremost scholars in the field of adult development and aging. Dr. Schaie holds a Ph.D. in psychology from the University of Washington, where he is an af- filiate professor of psychiatry and behavioral science. Since 1956, he has directed the Seat- tle Longitudinal Study, one of the most extensive psychological research studies of intel- lectual performance in older adults. Data from the several thousand people who have participated in the study show that most adults maintain their mental abilities well into their 60s—disproving scientists’ earlier belief that intelligence peaks in adolescence, then steadily declines.

Dr. Schaie has authored or edited 45 books and more than 250 scholarly publications on the psychology of aging, and has made substantial contributions to research methodology in the field of adult development and aging. His landmark paper, “A General Model for the Study of Developmental Problems,” originally published in 1965 in Psychological Bulletin, has been widely cited. In addition, he has served as editor of the Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences.

A fellow of the Gerontological Society of America and the American Psychological Asso- ciation, as well as past president and council representative of the APA Division of Adult Development and Aging, Dr. Schaie received the APA Distinguished Scientific Contributions Award in 1992. He also received the Kleemeier Award in 1987 from the Gerontological So- ciety of America for distinguished research contributions; the Method to Extend Research

in Time (MERIT) award from the National Institute on Aging in 1989; honorary doctorates ¥

15 in 1997 from Friedrich-Schiller University in Jena, Germany and in 2002 from West Vir- ginia University; and the 2001 Mensa Education and Research Foundation Lifetime Achievement Award.

Dr. Schaie has been a visiting scientist at a number of universities and institutes, most re- cently in 2002 at the University of Geneva. His lifelong research interests focus on cognitive and personality development and modifiability from young adulthood to advanced old age, influences of health on behavior, and methodological issues in the developmental sciences.

herry L. Willis, who holds a Ph.D. from the University of Texas at Austin, is a professor Sof human development at The Pennsylvania State University. Funded by the National In- stitute on Aging for more than 25 years, Dr. Willis’ research focuses particularly on develop- ing and evaluating training programs to help older adults compensate for age-related de- clines. She also looks at practical intelligence—how people solve everyday problems, such as the instructions on prescription drug labels. Dr. Willis recently was funded for a five-year study of midlife predictors of cognitive impairment in old age, a project that received a MERIT award from the National Institutes of Health.

From 1993–94, Dr. Willis was president of the American Psychological Association’s Divi- sion of Adult Development and Aging, and she holds fellow status in two divisions of the American Psychological Association, as well as in the Gerontological Society of America. She was honored in 1992 by The Pennsylvania State University College of Health and Human De- velopment with the Pattishall Distinguished Research Award. In 1999, she was awarded Penn State’s Faculty Scholar Medal for Outstanding Achievement, and in 2001 she received the Pauline Schmitt Russell Distinguished Research Career Award from the College of Health and Human Development of The Pennsylvania State University.

Dr. Willis serves as a reviewer for , , and Aging, Psychology and Aging, Clinical Gerontologist, and Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. She is a coauthor, with K. Warner Schaie, of the textbook Adult Development and Aging, now in its fifth edition. She is also the author or coauthor of numerous journal articles and chapters related to cognitive training in elders. Dr. Willis’ most recent interest has focused on cogni- tive development in midlife. She has coedited two recent volumes on midlife: Middle Adult- hood: A Lifespan Perspective, with Dr. Mike Martin, and The Baby Boomers Grow Up: Contem- porary Perspectives on Midlife, with Dr. Susan Whitbourne. With Dr. Schaie, she has been closely involved in the Seattle Longitudinal Study, which has been following participants for almost 50 years. ¥

16 2005 MINDALERT AWARDS

he ASA-MetLife Foundation MindAlert learning and stimulating memory, speak- TAwards were established to recognize ing one’s mind, and thinking positively. innovations in mental fitness program- ming for older adults. Inspired by research This research provided the basis for de- showing that cognitive decline is not in- veloping and piloting the eight-week Men- evitable in aging, these awards recognize tal Fitness for Life program for adults ages programs, products or tools that promote 50 and older, and culminated in the book cognitive fitness in later life. The entries Mental Fitness for Life: Seven Steps to are judged for their innovation, their basis Healthy Aging (Bull Publishing, 2006). In in research, demonstration of their effec- keeping with the title, the final step for tiveness, their potential for replication program participants is to develop a per- and the extent to which they are accessi- sonal mental fitness program for life. ble to diverse populations of elders. The awards are given in three categories: Contact: Sandra Cusack, Simon Fraser University, 515 W. Hastings St., Vancouver, • Programs that enhance mental fitness British Columbia, Canada V6B-5K3; (604) for older adults in general 291-5177; [email protected]; Wendy Thomp- • Programs that enhance mental fitness son, 3790 Gerry St., Richmond, British Co- for elders with cognitive impairment lumbia, Canada V7E-2T5; (604) 275-0091. • Learning programs for older adults PROGRAM FOR ELDERS WITH The winners of this year’s awards were COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT recognized at the Joint Conference of the American Society on Aging and the Na- TimeSlips tional Council on the Aging held in Center on Age and Community Philadelphia March 10–13, 2005. For more University of Wisconsin, Milwaukee information on the MindAlert program, visit www.asaging.org/mindalert. The TimeSlips method is a storytelling technique that encourages people with PROGRAM FOR GENERAL MENTAL FITNESS dementia to develop and enhance their skills by creating stories Mental Fitness for Life: based on imagination rather than reminis- A 7 Step Guide to Healthy Aging cence. The National TimeSlips Project, Simon Fraser University headquartered at the Center on Age and Vancouver, British Columbia Community at the University of Wisconsin, Milwaukee, offers training in the method A team of older adults from Century to paid and unpaid facilitators in demen- House, a senior recreation center on the tia care settings. west coast of Canada, worked with a facil- itator and a researcher to develop the TimeSlips storytelling groups conducted Mental Fitness for Life program. The team in adult day programs showed positive came to define mental fitness as a condi- outcomes for participants, who were less tion of optimal functioning achieved agitated and communicated more on the through regular exercise and a healthy days of storytelling. With a grant from the lifestyle that includes setting personal Commonwealth Fund, TimeSlips has estab-

goals, engaging in creative thinking, lished six regional training bases across ¥

17 the United States and has embarked on a adults and youth in the political process. quantitative study of the method in 20 The program helps maintain the essential long-term care facilities. mental fitness of participating elders and encourages their continued involvement Contact: Anne Basting, Center on Age and in the civic life of their communities. Community, University of Wisconsin—Mil- Through a selection of topical workshops waukee, P.O. Box 413, Milwaukee, WI 53211; chosen with the input of participants, VOCAL (414) 229-2732; [email protected]. rouses interest in and gives information on current issues that concern older adults. LEARNING PROGRAMS FOR OLDER ADULTS Skills training sessions focus on key Understanding Age-Related Memory areas of speaking, writing, leadership, and Loss and Dementia coalition-building. Community partners S. Oregon Research and Extension Center from local organizations, clubs, businesses, Oregon State University, Central Point and agencies generate programs that ad- dress the interests and needs of diverse Learning through volunteer endeavors populations. can stimulate cognitive function for older adults in meaningful ways. Understanding Contact: Reva Greenberg, Westchester Age-Related Memory Loss and Dementia is Public-Private Partnership for Aging Ser- a two-hour workshop that provides instruc- vices, 9 S. First Ave., 10th Floor, Mount Ver- tion to Meals-on-Wheels volunteers, who non, NY 10583; (914) 472-4551; revag@ deliver food to homebound older adults. optonline.net.

The program provides practical, easy-to- AWARDS REVIEW COMMITTEE use information on healthy aging—an ef- fective tool for addressing the age-related The American Society on Aging ex- memory problems of many of the meal re- presses gratitude to the review committee cipients. Drawing from current research, for their work in assessing the MindAlert the information provided in the workshop award submissions: vividly demonstrates how various lifestyle choices—such as good nutrition—have James Birren, Center on Aging, Univer- the potential to positively or negatively sity of California, Los Angeles; Dean Ble- affect memory in elders. vins, Central Arkansas Veterans Health Care System, North Little Rock, Ark.; San- Contact: Sharon Johnson, Oregon State dra Cusack, Gerontology Research Centre, University, Southern Oregon Research and Simon Fraser University, Vancouver, Brit- Extension Center, 569 Hanley Rd., Central ish Columbia; Deanna Eversoll, University Point, OR 97502; (541) 776-7371, ext. of Nebraska, Lincoln; Barbara Ginsberg, 210; [email protected]. My Turn Program, Kingsborough Commu- nity College, Brooklyn, N.Y.; Rebecca VOCAL: Voices of Community Action Goodman, Osher Lifelong Learning Insti- and Leadership tute, University of Hawaii at Manoa, Hon- Westchester Public-Private Partnership olulu; Darby Morhardt, Cognitive Neurol- for Aging Services ogy and Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Mount Vernon, New York Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago; Kathy Porsella, A nonpartisan advocacy education pro- Evergreen Society, Johns Hopkins Univer-

¥ gram, VOCAL is designed to involve older sity, Columbia, Md.

18 ABOUT THE MINDALERT PROGRAM

he MindAlert Program seeks to dissemi- THE AMERICAN SOCIETY ON AGING Tnate research and innovative practices that address the steps that older adults can The American Society on take to maintain and enhance cognitive Aging is the largest associa- and mental functions in their later years. tion of professionals in the The program, established by the American field of aging in the United Society on Aging (ASA) in 2001 and sup- States. Founded in 1954, ASA ported with funding from MetLife Founda- seeks to promote the well- tion, has the following components: being of older adults and their families by enhancing the abilities and • An annual MindAlert lecture and commitment of those who work with them. monograph, which impart the latest re- To that end, ASA sponsors a wide variety of search findings on maintaining and en- conferences, networking opportunities, hancing cognitive function in late life. and Web-based training. The organization also publishes a quarterly journal, a • The ASA-MetLife Foundation Mind- bimonthly newspaper, seven quarterly Alert Awards, which honor innovative newsletters, and an e-mail newsletter for community-based programs that translate its members. To obtain more information research into practical activities that pro- on ASA or to join, call (800) 537-9728 or mote cognitive health in elders. visit www.asaging.org.

• A Web-based clearinghouse of re- MINDALERT PROGRAM SPONSOR: sources related to maintenance and en- hancement of cognitive and mental func- tioning in late life. MetLife Foundation, established in 1976 by • A curriculum on promoting cognitive the Metropolitan Life Insurance Company, health that aging-services and healthcare has been involved in a variety of aging-re- providers can implement in a wide variety lated initiatives. Since 1986, the founda- of settings with older adults. tion has supported research on Alzheimer’s disease through the MetLife Foundation • A trainers bureau and train-the-train- Awards for Medical Research and has con- ers program to facilitate the implementa- tributed more than $9.5 million to efforts tion of cognitive vitality programs in such to find a cure. In addition, the foundation settings as senior centers, adult learning has provided support for a traveling exhibit programs in community colleges, and park on memory; a public-education video for and recreation programs. use by caregivers and families of people with Alzheimer’s disease; and support for For more information about the Mind- healthy-aging projects addressing issues of Alert program, including its Web-based caregiving, intergenerational activities, clearinghouse of resources on mental fit- health and wellness programs, and volun- ness, visit www.asaging.org/mindalert, or teer opportunities. MetLife Foundation contact the American Society on Aging at supports health, education, civic, and cul- [email protected]. tural programs throughout the United States. For more information about the

foundation, visit www.metlife.org. ¥

19 PAST MINDALERT SPECIAL LECTURES

he annual MindAlert monographs from 2001 to the present are available on the American TSociety on Aging website at www.asaging.org/mindalert as free, easily downloadable PDF files. Past monographs:

GOOD NEWS ABOUT THE AGING BRAIN! Nationally known brain researchers Marian Diamond and Arnold Scheibel describe their groundbreaking studies of brain function and the optimistic implications for successful aging. (2001)

BRAIN HEALTH FROM 1 TO 100 Paul Nussbaum, a leading clinical neuropsychologist, describes what he refers to as a health promotion opportunity of unprecedented stature: the ability to foster our own brain well- ness for healthy, functional aging. (2002)

CENTENARIANS: LESSONS ON LIVING LONG AND LIVING WELL Head of the renowned New England Centenarian Study, Thomas Perls shares the findings of his research and talks about how we all can make our later years healthy, vital ones. (2003)

UNITING THE HEART AND MIND: HUMAN DEVELOPMENT IN THE SECOND HALF OF LIFE Gene Cohen, a pioneer in the field of creative aging, explains the implications of his four developmental stages of late life and how these phases represent a richly creative period, full of personal growth and societal engagement. (2004) ¥

20 Does intellectual ability change uniformly through- out adulthood or are different patterns of ability present over the lifespan? What accounts for indi- MIND vidual differences in age-related changes in cogni- tive ability, especially in late life? Can cognitive ALERT decline with increasing age be reversed by educa- tional intervention? In this monograph, Dr. K. Warner Schaie and Dr. Sherry L. Willis discuss their findings from the Seattle Longitudinal Study, which spans three generations and almost 50 years.

American Society on Aging 833 Market St., suite 511 San Francisco, CA 94105 NTELLECTUAL UNCTIONING IN DULTHOOD Phone: (415) 974-9600 I F A : Fax: (415) 974-0300 GROWTH, MAINTENANCE, DECLINE, AND MODIFIABILITY E-mail: [email protected] 2005 SPECIAL LECTURE BY K. WARNER SCHAIE AND SHERRY L. WILLIS

A JOINT PROGRAM OF THE AMERICAN SOCIETY ON AGING AND METLIFE FOUNDATION