Comparison of Roost Use by Three Species of Communal Roostmates

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Comparison of Roost Use by Three Species of Communal Roostmates The Condor 92~405-412 0 The CooperOrnithological Society 1990 COMPARISON OF ROOST USE BY THREE SPECIES OF COMMUNAL ROOSTMATES DOUGLAS W. MORRISON Department of BiologicalSciences, Rutgers University,Newark, NJ 07102 DONALD F. CACCAMISE Department of Entomology, Cook College,Rutgers University,New Brunswick,NJ 08903 Abstract. Our previous studies of communally roosting European Starlings (Sturnus vulgaris)revealed that each bird fed daily for months on its own “diurnal activity center” (DAC) and commuted to a variety of nearby and distant roosts. Since these observations contrast sharply with the predictions of most foragingexplanations for communal roosting, we wanted to determine if this DAC-centered roostingpattern occurredin other communally roosting species.In this study we used radiotelemetry to monitor feeding and roosting sites used by starlings,Common Grackles (Quiscuh quiscula),and American Robins (Turdus migratorious),three speciesthat share communal roosts in central New Jersey. Our goals were to determine (a) whether avian speciesthat roost together use the roosts in similar ways, and (b) when and why individuals change roosts. Foraging patterns were similar in all three species;individuals fed daily on their DACs for many weeks. Roosting patterns were similar for gracklesand starlings;individuals switchedamong nearby and distant roosts. In contrast, robins always roosted near their DACs, changingboth roosts and DACs at the end of the breeding season.Predation rates at roostswere extremely low and did not explain the use patterns of large and small roosts. We argue that (a) DACs and DAC-centered roostingare probably widespreadamong communally roosting species,and (b) DAC-based individuals select roosts primarily on the basis of their proximity to good sourcesof food. Key words: Communal roosting:foraging; diurnal activity center;predation; European Starling; Stumus vulgaris; Common Grackle; Quiscalusquiscula; American Robin; Turdus migratorious. INTRODUCTION individuals actually used a variety of roosts but Several investigators have hypothesized that returned to feed on their own “diurnal activity communal roosting by birds evolved to facilitate center” (DAC, an area of l-2 km?) day after day foraging in various ways. For example, roosting for months. Contrary to the assumptions of ear- in a central location can reduce average com- lier models, foraging and roosting were centered muting costs when food patches are short-lived around a stable feeding area (DAC) rather than (Horn 1968). Communal roostsmight also serve around a roost. as “information centers” at which individuals Foraging and roosting behaviors of starlings can learn about new or better food sourcesby change in midsummer. During most of the year, following successfulroostmates (Ward and Za- starlingsfeed exclusively on their DACs and join havi 1973). These and other “roost-centered” small (50-500 bird) communal roostsnearby (< 2 models (e.g., Weatherhead 1983) are based on km away). During late summer and fall, starlings the assumption that the roost is a relatively stable begin to commute to rooststhat are much larger base of operation from which individuals exploit (5,000-50,000 birds) and more distant (4-12 km). less stable food sources. These larger roosts do not form because of a A very different picture of communal roosting shortageof acceptable roosting sites (Lyon and emerged when roostmates were individually Caccamise 198 l), and they are not premigratory marked (Morrison and Caccamise 1985). Radio- aggregations,because they form months before tracking studies of communally roosting Euro- migration (Caccamise et al. 1983). pean Starlings (Sturnus vufguris) revealed that When using distant roosts, starlings continue to feed primarily on their DACs, but also stop briefly at food sourcesnear the distant roosts.By 1Received 26 July 1989. Final acceptance 7 Feb- roosting overnight near food patches far from ruary 1990. their DACs, starlingscan reducecommuting costs t4051 406 DOUGLAS W. MORRISON ANDDONALD F. CACCAMISE to supplemental food patchesby as much as 50% from cotton shoelace(Morrison and Caccamise (Caccamise and Morrison 1986). Recent obser- 1985). After an initial period of adjustment (6- vations of individual starlings en route to and 48 hr), the bird’s behavior was not noticeably from roosts support this “patch sitting” aspect affectedby the transmitter; the radio-taggedbirds of communal roosting (Caccamiseand Morrison flew and foragednormally for months. The range 1988). Is the DAC-centered roosting pattern of of the transmitters was greater than 1.0 km for starlings unique or do other communally roost- birds roosting in trees and less than 0.5 km for ing specieshave similar patterns? birds foraging on the ground. Transmitter life To investigate the relationship of communal averaged 90 (SD = 15) days. roosting to DACs, we undertook a comparative To determine whether the birds had DACs study of starlings, Common Grackles (Quisculus (i.e., spatially clustereddiurnal sightings),we used quiscula),and American Robins (Turdus migra- an automobile with a rooftop antenna to locate torious),three speciesthat sharecommunal roosts each bird during the day, 6 days a week, at ran- in central New Jersey (Caccamise and Fischl domly predetermined times between 06:OO and 1985). This is the first study to monitor both 18:O0. Attempts to locate the birds were suc- roosting and foraging behavior of heterospecific cessfulover 85% of the time for all three species. roostmates. By radio-tagging individuals cap- This successrate was somewhat lower than in tured from the same feeding area, we controlled previous studies(Morrison and Caccamise 1985) for the effectsof habitat heterogeneity. We found becauseof a lack of experienced field assistants that DAC-based foraging behavior is not unique in 1987. to European Starlings and may be widespread For interspecific comparisons, spatial cluster- among communally roosting species.We argue ing was quantified as the mean distance from that DAC-based birds select among suitable each diurnal sighting to the use-weighted center roosting sitesprimarily on the basis of their prox- (i.e., mean x and y coordinates) of all diurnal imity to good feeding areas. sightingsmade on that bird. SeparateDACs were recognizedwhen all sightingsin one cluster were METHODS >2 km from all sightingsin the other cluster. In We radio-tagged nine European Starlings, nine practice, the few cases of multiple DACs in- Common Grackles, and nine American Robins, volved separations of several kilometers. all adults, in central New Jerseyduring the local We located the roost usedby each radio-tagged roosting season (June-November) of 1987. All bird six nights a week. Attempts to locate roost- starlingsand grackleswere postbreedingindivid- ing sites were successfulover 93% of the time. uals. Five of the robins were captured toward the The birds could almost always be found by cen- end of their breeding seasonand may have been susingafter dark the many traditionally usedroost reproductively active for the first 1O-20 days of sites in the study area. Distances from DACs to the 46-102 days (X = 77 days) that they were communal roosts were measured from the use- tracked (Bovitz 1990). weighted center of the bird’s DAC. All birds were captured during the day in the To describe long-term patterns of roost use, horticultural display garden of Rutgers Univer- we restrictedour analysisto the 17 birds for which sity, New Brunswick, New Jersey. Robins were we had greater than 40 days of observations: six mist-netted; starlings and grackleswere captured starlings (four males, two females), four grackles using decoy traps and baited walk-in traps. Birds (three males, one female), seven robins (four selectedfor radio-taggingappeared in goodhealth, males, three females). Radio contact with the with well-maintained plumage and body weights other 10 birds (X = 20 days) was much shorter. above the mean for that species and sex. We Four had transmitters with defective batteries; taggedand releasedall birds at the site of capture after only l-2 weeks in the field, their pulse rates 53 hr after they were trapped. The birds sub- increased almost two-fold, symptomatic of a sequentlyfrequented an urban-suburban area that voltage-drop in lithium batteries. The other six was primarily residential and commercial (8 1%) transmitters lost their antennas, reducing their but contained a few agricultural fields (4%) and effective range to < 100 m. woodlots (2%) (Fischl and Caccamise 1985). Estimates of predation rates were based on the The transmitter package (< 5 g) was attached fates of 63 adult starlings tracked during four to the back of the starling (7 l-85 g), grackle (95- summer roosting seasons(1983-l 986). Each bird 131 g) or robin (73-84 g) with a “vest” made was assignedto one of five categories:eaten, dead AVIAN COMMUNAL ROOSTING 401 but not eaten, battery expired, premature trans- munal roosts at a total of 15 different sites. The mitter failure, and unknown. The eaten category three specieswere frequently found roosting to- may have overestimated predation rate because gether. The six starlings(n = 487 nocturnal sight- it may have included birds eaten after dying of ings) and the four grackles (n = 279 nocturnal other causes.The battery expired category was sightings)joined roosts known to include both assignedonly when the signal was lost (a) after starlingsand gracklesover 97% of the time. Rob- the expiration date calculated on the basis of ins (n = 462 nocturnal sightings) roosted with battery capacity and transmitter current drain, starlings and grackles at least 57% of the time. and/or (b) after a significant (1.5 to 2.0-fold) Of the 15 roosts, 12 were minor roosts of increase in transmitter pulse rate, indicating a ~2,000 birds. One site (R-88) was occupied by voltage drop in lithium batteries. Premature an intermediate-sized flock that peaked at 3,000- transmitter failure was assigned only when the 5,000 in late August. Two sites-(R-42 and R-97) bird had been visually sightedwith a nonworking were occupiedby major roostingflocks of 1O,OOO- transmitter. The unknown categoryincludes un- 35,000 birds.
Recommended publications
  • Dynamics of a Problematic Vulture Roost in Southwest Florida and Responses of Vultures to Roost-Dispersal Management Efforts
    DYNAMICS OF A PROBLEMATIC VULTURE ROOST IN SOUTHWEST FLORIDA AND RESPONSES OF VULTURES TO ROOST-DISPERSAL MANAGEMENT EFFORTS A thesis Presented to The Faculty of the College of Arts and Sciences Florida Gulf Coast University In Partial Fulfillment Of the Requirement for the Degree of Master of Science By Betsy A. Evans 2013 APPROVAL SHEET The thesis is submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science __________________________________ Betsy A. Evans Approved: December 2013 __________________________________ Jerome A. Jackson, Ph.D. Committee Chair / Advisor __________________________________ Edwin M. Everham III, Ph.D. Committee Member __________________________________ Charles W. Gunnels IV, Ph.D. Committee Member The final copy of this thesis has been examined by the signatories, and we find that both the content and the form meet acceptable presentation standards of scholarly work in the above mentioned discipline. iii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS First and foremost I thank my thesis advisor, Dr. Jerry Jackson, whose support, assistance, and interest throughout the whole process was vital in the preparation and completion of this project. I am grateful to my committee members, Dr. Billy Gunnels and Dr. Win Everham, who provided statistical guidance and vital assistance in the interpretation and editing of the project. I am appreciative to the Department of Marine and Ecological Sciences and the Department of Biological Sciences at Florida Gulf Coast University for their support and encouragement during the completion of my degree. Additionally, I thank the Guadalupe Center of Immokalee for allowing me to conduct research on their property and providing valuable information during the project. A special thank you to the Ding Darling Wildlife Society for their generosity in providing supplemental funding for the project.
    [Show full text]
  • Biological and Environmental Factors Related to Communal Roosting Behavior of Breeding Bank Swallow (Riparia Riparia)
    VOLUME 14, ISSUE 2, ARTICLE 21 Saldanha, S., P. D. Taylor, T. L. Imlay, and M. L. Leonard. 2019. Biological and environmental factors related to communal roosting behavior of breeding Bank Swallow (Riparia riparia). Avian Conservation and Ecology 14(2):21. https://doi.org/10.5751/ACE-01490-140221 Copyright © 2019 by the author(s). Published here under license by the Resilience Alliance. Research Paper Biological and environmental factors related to communal roosting behavior of breeding Bank Swallow (Riparia riparia) Sarah Saldanha 1, Philip D. Taylor 2, Tara L. Imlay 1 and Marty L. Leonard 1 1Biology Department, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada, 2Biology Department, Acadia University, Wolfville, Nova Scotia, Canada ABSTRACT. Although communal roosting during the wintering and migratory periods is well documented, few studies have recorded this behavior during the breeding season. We used automated radio telemetry to examine communal roosting behavior in breeding Bank Swallow (Riparia riparia) and its relationship with biological and environmental factors. Specifically, we used (generalized) linear mixed models to determine whether the probability of roosting communally and the timing of departure from and arrival at the colony (a measure of time away from the nest) was related to adult sex, nestling age, brood size, nest success, weather, light conditions, communal roosting location, and date. We found that Bank Swallow individuals roosted communally on 70 ± 25% of the nights, suggesting that this behavior is common. The rate of roosting communally was higher in males than in females with active nests, increased with older nestlings in active nests, and decreased more rapidly with nestling age in smaller broods.
    [Show full text]
  • Ecological Determinants of Pathogen Transmission in Communally Roosting Species
    Theoretical Ecology https://doi.org/10.1007/s12080-019-0423-6 ORIGINAL PAPER Ecological determinants of pathogen transmission in communally roosting species Andrew J. Laughlin1 & Richard J. Hall2,3,4 & Caz M. Taylor5 Received: 16 July 2018 /Accepted: 6 March 2019 # Springer Nature B.V. 2019 Abstract Many animals derive benefits from roosting communally but may also face increased risk of infectious disease transmission. In spite of recent high-profile disease outbreaks in roosting animals of conservation and public health concern, we currently lack general theory for how attributes of roosting animals and their pathogens influence pathogen spread among roosts and overall population impacts on roosting species. Here we develop a model to explore how roost size and host site fidelity influence the time for a pathogen to escape from its initial roost, overall infection prevalence, and host population size, for pathogens with density- or frequency-dependent transmission and varying virulence. We find that pathogens spread rapidly to all roosts when animals are distributed among a small number of large roosts, and that roost size more strongly influences spread rate for density- dependent than frequency-dependent transmitted pathogens. However, roosting animals that exhibit high site fidelity and dis- tribute among a large number of small roosts are buffered from population-level impacts of pathogens of both transmission modes. We discuss our results in the context of anthropogenic change that is altering aspects of roosting behavior relevant to emerging pathogen spread. Keywords Animal aggregations . Communal roost . Pathogen transmission . Roost size . Site fidelity Introduction Hamilton 1982; Thompson 1989), fish (Clough and Ladle, 1997), birds (Eiserer 1984; Beauchamp 1999), and inverte- Animal aggregations occur in a wide variety of taxa, at vary- brates (Mallet 1986;GretherandSwitzer2000).
    [Show full text]
  • Beyond the Information Centre Hypothesis: Communal Roosting for Information on Food, Predators, Travel Companions and Mates?
    Oikos 119: 277Á285, 2010 doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0706.2009.17892.x, # 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation # 2009 Oikos Subject Editor: Kenneth Schmidt. Accepted 1 September 2009 Beyond the information centre hypothesis: communal roosting for information on food, predators, travel companions and mates? Allert I. Bijleveld, Martijn Egas, Jan A. van Gils and Theunis Piersma A. I. Bijleveld ([email protected]), J. A. van Gils and T. Piersma, Dept of Marine Ecology, Royal Netherlands Inst. for Sea Research (NIOZ), PO Box 59, NLÁ1790 AB Den Burg, Texel, the Netherlands. TP also at: Centre for Ecological and Evolutionary Studies, Univ. of Groningen, PO Box 14, NLÁ9750 AA Haren, the Netherlands. Á M. Egas, Inst. for Biodiversity and Ecosystem Dynamics, Univ. of Amsterdam, PO Box 94248, NLÁ1090 GE Amsterdam, the Netherlands. Communal roosting Á the grouping of more than two individuals resting together Á is common among animals, notably birds. The main functions of this complicated social behaviour are thought to be reduced costs of predation and thermoregulation, and increased foraging efficiency. One specific hypothesis is the information centre hypothesis (ICH) which states that roosts act as information centres where individuals actively advertise and share foraging information such as the location of patchily distributed foods. Empirical studies in corvids have demonstrated behaviours consistent with the predictions of the ICH, but some of the assumptions in its original formulation have made its wide acceptance problematic. Here we propose to generalise the ICH in two ways: (1) dropping the assumption that information transfer must be active, and (2) adding the possibilities of information exchange on, for example, predation risk, travel companions and potential mates.
    [Show full text]
  • Redalyc.WHY ANIMALS COME TOGETHER, with the SPECIAL CASE of MIXED-SPECIES BIRD FLOCKS
    Revista EIA ISSN: 1794-1237 [email protected] Escuela de Ingeniería de Antioquia Colombia Colorado Zuluaga, Gabriel Jaime WHY ANIMALS COME TOGETHER, WITH THE SPECIAL CASE OF MIXED-SPECIES BIRD FLOCKS Revista EIA, vol. 10, núm. 19, enero-junio, 2013, pp. 49-66 Escuela de Ingeniería de Antioquia Envigado, Colombia Available in: http://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=149228694005 How to cite Complete issue Scientific Information System More information about this article Network of Scientific Journals from Latin America, the Caribbean, Spain and Portugal Journal's homepage in redalyc.org Non-profit academic project, developed under the open access initiative Revista EIA, ISSN 1794-1237 / Publicación semestral / Volumen 10 / Número 19 / Enero-Junio 2013 /pp. 49-66 Escuela de Ingeniería de Antioquia, Medellín (Colombia) WHY ANIMALS COME TOGETHER, WITH THE SPECIAL CASE OF MIXED-SPECIES BIRD FLOCKS Gabriel Jaime Colorado ZuluaGa* ABSTRACT Group living is a widespread, ubiquitous biological phenomenon in the animal kingdom that has attracted considerable attention in many different contexts. The availability of food and the presence of predators represent the two main factors believed to favor group life. In this review, major theories supporting grouping behavior in animals are explored, providing an explanation of animal grouping. This review is divided in two sections. First, major theories as well as potential mechanisms behind the benefit of grouping are described. Later, a special case on the widespread animal social system of mixed-species avian flocks is presented, exploring the available information in relation to the potential causes that bring birds together into this particular social aggregation.
    [Show full text]
  • Communal Roosting and Foraging Behavior of Staging Sandhill Cranes
    Wilson Bull., 106(l), 1994, pp. 62-71 COMMUNAL ROOSTING AND FORAGING BEHAVIOR OF STAGING SANDHILL CRANES DONALD W. SPARLINGJ’ AND GARY L. KRAPU ’ AssraAcr.-Each spring more than 300,000 Sandhill Cranes (GAS canadensis)roost communally at night in river channels in the Platte River Valley of Nebraska and disperse at dawn to forage in agricultural fields. Cranes with central roosts had activity ranges double the size of those with peripheral roosts; 42% of the birds changed activity ranges prior to the onset of migration. Minimum daily flight distance generally increased during the staging period. Cranes used native grassland and planted hayland more often than expected, relative to their percentage of occurrence, and fed longest there; cornfields were under-utilized. These differences probably reflect, in part, (1) limited distribution of grasslands and haylands resulting in a greater energy expenditure to acquire protein in the form of macroinvertebrates and (2) wider distribution of cornfields with adequate energy-rich foods but limited protein. Cranes probably forage more efficiently and conserve energy by following conspecifics from communal roosts to local feeding grounds, by settling in fields where foraging flocks are already present, and by establishing diurnal activity centers. Alert behavior varied with flock size but not as predicted from group size, presumably because predation of staging adult cranes is inconsequential. Received4 Jan. 1993, accepted15 June 1993. Sandhill Cranes of the midcontinent population acquire nutrient re- serves for migration and reproduction while on staging areas along the Platte and North Platte rivers in Nebraska (Krapu et al. 1985). Cranes roost communally at night in shallow waters of wide river channels or other wetlands and spend the days foraging in flocks on surrounding uplands (Krapu et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Passenger Pigeon of Living Passenger Pigeons in Penn- Ectopistes Migratorius Sylvania Were in 1906
    STATUS: Extinct; the last reports Passenger Pigeon of living passenger pigeons in Penn- Ectopistes migratorius sylvania were in 1906. The last known living passenger pigeon died at 1 p.m. September 1, 1914, in the Cincinnati Zoological Garden. The bird — named ―Martha‖ — was 29 years old. IDENTIFYING CHARACTERIS- TICS: A large dove; about 16 inches long, with a two-foot wing span; the head was slate blue with a black beak and red eyes; crimson feet; 12 tail feathers; gray to blue upper side; sides and back of neck, metallic golden violet; reddish- brown from throat to belly; white belly. Females were smaller than males. The birds weighed 9-12 ounces, two to three times the size of a mourning dove. BIOLOGY/NATURAL HISTORY: Passenger pigeons were once an abundant nester in the Common- wealth. Their North American popu- lation numbered as many as five billion birds when colonists arrived and comprised 25 to 40 percent of America’s total wild bird popula- tion. They captivated early residents with their spring and fall migrations. They ranged over eastern North America from Hudson’s Bay south to the Gulf of Mexico, and west into the Mississippi River valley in mixed hardwood forests. Hunters and market hunters alike pursued them with reckless abandon. The birds were trapped, shot, netted and clubbed; even the young, called "squabs," were knocked from nests with poles, or the trees holding their nests were cut down, and they, too, were collected for market. For years, the flocks seemingly withstood the losses. After 30 years of observation, John J.
    [Show full text]
  • Communal Starling Roosts: Implications for Control
    University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Proceedings of the Fourteenth Vertebrate Pest Vertebrate Pest Conference Proceedings Conference 1990 collection March 1990 COMMUNAL STARLING ROOSTS: IMPLICATIONS FOR CONTROL Donald F. Caccamise Department of Entomology, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, NJ Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/vpc14 Part of the Environmental Health and Protection Commons Caccamise, Donald F., "COMMUNAL STARLING ROOSTS: IMPLICATIONS FOR CONTROL" (1990). Proceedings of the Fourteenth Vertebrate Pest Conference 1990. 12. https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/vpc14/12 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Vertebrate Pest Conference Proceedings collection at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in Proceedings of the Fourteenth Vertebrate Pest Conference 1990 by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. COMMUNAL STARLING ROOSTS: IMPLICATIONS FOR CONTROL DONALD F. CACCAMISE, Department of Entomology, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, NJ 08903. ABSTRACT: Roosting behavior is common to most avian pests of agriculture. Movements from highly aggregated distributions in roosts to highly dispersed distributions on foraging grounds determine pattern and severity of avian pest problems. This research seeks an understanding of how roosting behavior influences the dispersion of avian agricultural pests and the damage they cause. My focus is on why birds form communal roosts and how communal roosting influences the selection of foraging sites. I document patterns of roosting behavior in European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) through population level studies, followed by analysis of individual behavior using radio telemetry. Starlings maintain long-term fidelity (up to 130 days) to the same diurnal activity center (DAC), while using a variety of roosting sites at night.
    [Show full text]
  • Energetics of Communal Roosting in Chestnut-Crowned Babblers: Implications for Group Dynamics and Breeding Phenology Mark A
    © 2016. Published by The Company of Biologists Ltd | Journal of Experimental Biology (2016) 219, 3321-3328 doi:10.1242/jeb.144972 RESEARCH ARTICLE Energetics of communal roosting in chestnut-crowned babblers: implications for group dynamics and breeding phenology Mark A. Chappell1,*, William A. Buttemer2,3 and Andrew F. Russell4,5 ABSTRACT individual attributes (e.g. relatedness to other group members, sex, For many endotherms, communal roosting saves energy in cold age, condition; Russell and Hatchwell, 2001; Clutton-Brock et al., conditions, but how this might affect social dynamics or breeding 2002; Silk et al., 2014) and by a range of ecological factors phenology is not well understood. Using chestnut-crowned babblers including resource availability and competition (Koenig et al., (Pomatostomus ruficeps), we studied the effects of nest use and 1992; Russell, 2004), predation intensity (Elgar, 1989; Beauchamp, group size on roosting energy costs. These 50 g cooperatively 2008), disease risk (Nunn et al., 2015) and climate (Jetz and breeding passerine birds of outback Australia breed from late winter to Rubenstein, 2011; Cockburn and Russell, 2011). However, testing early summer and roost in huddles of up to 20 in single-chambered the effects of ecological factors on levels of sociality is often nests. We measured babbler metabolism at three ecologically challenging because many are both difficult to quantify and relevant temperatures: 5°C (similar to minimum nighttime common to the population under study. Studying social species temperatures during early breeding), 15°C (similar to nighttime living in temporally heterogeneous environments offers a way ‘ ’ temperatures during late breeding) and 28°C (thermal neutrality).
    [Show full text]
  • Day Roosts of Male Pallid Bats (Antrozous Pallidus) Along
    Day roosts of male pallid bats (Antrozous pallidus ) along Purgatoire River Valley, Las Animas County, Colorado Colorado Natural Heritage Program Warner College of Natural Resources Colorado State University Fort Collins, Colorado Colorado Natural Heritage Program May 2010 Warner College of Natural Resources Colorado State University Day roosts of male pallid bats (Antrozous pallidus ) Purgatoire River Valley, Las Animas County, Colorado Robert A. Schorr Zoologist Cover photographs (clockwise): 1. Male pallid bat 2. Jeremy Siemers (CNHP Zoologist) in cliff crack searching for a pallid bat 3. Rob Schorr overlooking Purgatoire River valley using telemetry equipment to find pallid bats 1 INTRODUCTION Bats are the second most speciose mammal order, and Colorado is home to 19 (Armstrong et al. 1994, Fitzgerald et al. 1994) of the 41 species found in North America (Wilson and Ruff 1999). Despite this diversity our knowledge of even simple natural history information, such as distribution and habitat use of bats, is constantly evolving. For example, the Allen’s big-eared bat ( Idionycteris phyllotis) was recently documented in Colorado. Likely this is not a recent expansion of range, but recognition of historic natural distribution (Hayes et al. 2009). Compared to other species of mammals our understanding of bat natural history is limited, usually restricted to those bat species that are urban-adapted or recognized vectors of human diseases (Neubaum et al. 2007, O’Shea et al. 2010). The paucity of biological information is a product of the ecology of and the difficulty of surveying for bats. Bats are the only volant mammals, able to fly up to 2500 m above ground (Peurach 2003).
    [Show full text]
  • Seasonal Patterns in Roosting Flocks of Starlings and Common Grackles
    Condor 85:474&31 G The Cooper Ornithological Society 1983 SEASONAL PATTERNS IN ROOSTING FLOCKS OF STARLINGS AND COMMON GRACKLES DONALD F. CACCAMISE LINDA A. LYON AND JOSEPH FISCHL ABSTRACT. -Communal roosting is often a regional phenomenon that involves wide-ranging and long-lasting relationships among associations.We examined roosting behavior on a scale sufficiently large to detect regional and seasonal patterns. For five roosting seasons(June-November), we studied the population dynamics of all roosting flocks of European Starlings(Sturnus vulgaris)and Com- mon Grackles (Quiscalus quiscula) located within a l,OOO-km2census area in central New Jersey. Roosts were active from 3-20 weeks and ranged in size from 2,000 to over 100,000 individuals. The total roostingpopulation (TRP) in “major” (>2,000 birds) flocks increased through early summer, generally achieving max- imum size in mid-August when the largest number of roosts was active. When TRP was largest, size of major roosts varied greatly (range 2,000-100,000 indi- viduals). Through late summer and early fall, size and number of major roosts and TRP declined. By late fall few major roosts were active, but those remaining were large (> 30,000). Movements of individual birds (radio-tagged) suggested that changesin size of TRP resulted largely from exchangeof the local population between small, “minor” roosts (largely undetected and not included in roost censuses)and major flocks. Current hypotheses concerning the functional basis of communal roosting do not adequately explain patterns of roosting behavior that we observed. - Communal roosting behavior occurs in birds Starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) and Common of diverse taxa and habitats.
    [Show full text]
  • Communal Roosting Sites Are Potential Ecological Traps: Experimental Evidence in a Neotropical Harvestman
    Communal roosting sites are potential ecological traps: experimental evidence in a Neotropical harvestman Gregory F. Grether, Abrahm Levi, Carmen Antaky & Debra M. Shier Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology ISSN 0340-5443 Volume 68 Number 10 Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2014) 68:1629-1638 DOI 10.1007/s00265-014-1771-2 1 23 Your article is protected by copyright and all rights are held exclusively by Springer- Verlag Berlin Heidelberg. This e-offprint is for personal use only and shall not be self- archived in electronic repositories. If you wish to self-archive your article, please use the accepted manuscript version for posting on your own website. You may further deposit the accepted manuscript version in any repository, provided it is only made publicly available 12 months after official publication or later and provided acknowledgement is given to the original source of publication and a link is inserted to the published article on Springer's website. The link must be accompanied by the following text: "The final publication is available at link.springer.com”. 1 23 Author's personal copy Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2014) 68:1629–1638 DOI 10.1007/s00265-014-1771-2 ORIGINAL PAPER Communal roosting sites are potential ecological traps: experimental evidence in a Neotropical harvestman Gregory F. Grether & Abrahm Levi & Carmen Antaky & Debra M. Shier Received: 30 December 2013 /Revised: 28 May 2014 /Accepted: 3 July 2014 /Published online: 18 July 2014 # Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2014 Abstract Situations in which animals preferentially settle in Keywords Conspecific attraction . Ecological trap . Habitat low-quality habitat are referred to as ecological traps, and selection .
    [Show full text]