<<

1 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

Running Head: TEACHING SIGNED

Using the Line to Teach Signed Numbers

For Remedial Community College Mathematics

Alice Welt Cunningham, Ph.D.

Hostos Community College

500 Grand Concourse

Bronx, NY 10451

Telephone: 718/518-6629

Fax: 718/518-6706

Email: [email protected]

© Copyright 2009 Alice Welt Cunningham

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

2 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

3 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

Abstract

Using the Number Line to Teach Signed Numbers For Remedial Community College Mathematics

This article considers the use of the number line to teach signed numbers in remedial

mathematics classes designed to prepare community college students for the high stakes exit

examination necessary to graduation and college-level work. The strategy involves using one arrow

for each number in the relevant operation, thus representing each number’s

and orientation. Each drawing reflects either the augmentation or cancellation process necessary to

determining the of the result, thus showing the sense behind the rules and promoting long-term

retention rather than short-term rule memorization. Such teaching for sense-making fosters

confidence in math, critical thinking skills and general academic confidence, in turn promoting

increased student retention for this academically-vulnerable population.

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

4 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

Using the Number Line to Teach Signed Numbers For Remedial Community College Mathematics

Historically, the arithmetic of negative quantities developed slowly and late

(Mukhopadhyay, Swapna, & Schauble, 1990). “Negative numbers have intrigued and confused

some of the greatest mathematicians who have ever lived” (Davis & Maher, 1993, p. 60).

Mathematicians who found them troublesome include the French mathematician Rene Descartes,

who considered negative solutions of equations to be false, and the Swiss mathematician Leonhardt

Euler (Davis & Maher, 1993; Thomaidis & Tzanakis, 2007). Because of such numbers’ lack of

intuitive support, it took over a thousand years for their acceptance in the mathematical community

(Ball, 1993). This paper outlines a strategy, used by the author with success, for teaching signed

numbers in two different levels of remedial mathematics at a community college in a large East

Coast city. The goal of the strategy is to illustrate the sense behind the rules, thus promoting

understanding and long-term retention rather than short-term rule memorization, as well as

appreciation for, and confidence in, mathematics.

The present paper is an outgrowth of a pilot study performed with learning-disabled fifth-

graders by this author while in graduate school several years ago (Cunningham, 2004). Although

variations on the strategy presented here have been used by a few other authors (National Research

Council, 2001; Martin-Gay, 2007; Akst & Bragg, 2009, Cemen, 1993), the ideas herein presented Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

5 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

are both original to this author and, for reasons that will be explained, presented with what this

author believes to be greater clarity from the standpoint of student understanding.

The strategy in question utilizes a number-line approach for teaching the ,

, , and of signed numbers. The article first discusses the school

and its students. The article next summarizes the relevant literature. This discussion addresses four

issues: such students’ need for sense-making teaching strategies, particular difficulties inherent in

mastering operations with signed numbers, the use of diagrams to improve the comprehension of

mathematical topics, and the choice of the number line as a diagrammatic tool for teaching such

operations. The article then details, with accompanying diagrams, the proposed strategy for using

the number line to support the teaching of signed numbers. A discussion of student success ensues,

followed by a brief conclusion.

Nature of the Course and Its Students

The community college in question is a two-year public, open-admissions institution that is

part of a city-wide system receiving state and city funding. Of the current total enrollment of 5500

students, approximately 70% are female, 59% are Hispanic, and 30% are black. Close to 90% of

matriculating students enter needing at least one remedial course in reading, writing, or

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

6 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

mathematics. Graduates take an average of four years to complete their associate’s degree, with a

graduation rate of only 24% after six years. Percentage retention and graduation rates are

significantly lower for black and Hispanic students. Difficulty completing mathematics courses

constitutes a significant contributing factor to low graduation rates (Hostos Community College,

2008).

The two remedial mathematics courses at issue here are each semester-long courses (Basic

Math Skills and ) designed to prepare entering students to pass the system-wide

algebra mastery test necessary for graduation and college-level study. During the spring 2008

semester, of the 2600 registrants at this community college enrolled in mathematics courses, close

to 53% were enrolled in these two courses. Such students have had little success with mathematics

and have even less confidence in their ability to do it. At least in this author’s experience, they are

anxious to be given step-by-step algorithms into which to substitute numbers in order to pass the

test.

Signed numbers form a significant part of the curriculum of each of the two courses. The

Basic Math Skills course covers basic arithmetic concepts, including operations with whole

numbers, fractions and decimals, ratios and proportions, percents, operations with signed numbers

and scientific notation, the metric system, and an introduction to word problems and applications.

The progress is somewhat leisurely. The Elementary Algebra course covers operations with real

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

7 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

numbers, algebraic expressions, solving linear equations in one variable (including applications and

word problems), exponential expressions, operations with and factoring polynomials, operations

with rational expressions, solving quadratic equations (again including word problems), roots and

radicals, graphing linear equations and finding the equations of lines, and solving systems of linear

equations in two variables. Concepts are covered at a gallop. Because of the importance of the exit

test for which the Elementary Algebra course is designed, the curriculum tends to emphasize rules

and algorithms necessary for success on the test. The reasoning underlying the rules can get lost in

the rush to cover the syllabus.

College Level Remedial Mathematics And the Need to Teach for Understanding

Recent research on mathematical cognition indicates that students who struggle with

mathematics view it as an obstacle course designed to present meaningless tasks, and their teachers

as out to “trick” them (Ginsburg, 1997, p. 25). Although motivation is thought to be the key factor

in the success or failure of education, by the time many students enter high school, disengagement

from course work and serious study is common (Ramaley & Zia, 2005). The consequences of such

disengagement are all the more serious for people from disadvantaged backgrounds, who often do

not get a second chance at education (Ramaley & Zia, 2005).

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

8 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

Such concerns are particularly important for students of the “net generation” (e.g., Oblinger

& Oblinger, 2005), now entering community colleges. Today, only about 25% of students enrolled

in post-secondary education are traditional students--those who enter college immediately after high

school, who attend full-time, who work, if at all, only part-time, and who are financially dependent

on their parents (Ramaley & Zia, 2005). By contrast, non-traditional students may enter as adults,

attend part-time, and work full-time while enrolled. Such students are more likely to begin their

secondary education in a community college such as this one, where the yield of successful

bachelor’s graduates is low compared to students who begin their post-secondary education at a

four-year institution (Ramaley & Zia, 2005).

Modern scholars of mathematics pedagogy view the development of mathematical

proficiency as “an exploratory, dynamic, evolving discipline rather than as a rigid, absolute, closed

body of laws to be memorized” (Schoenfeld, 1992, p. 335). Conceptual understanding is considered

to be as important as procedural fluency and strategic competence (National Mathematics Advisory

Panel, 2008; National Research Council, 2001). Rather than constituting a mere by-product of

substantive competence, conceptual understanding is thought to propitiate such competence (e.g.,

National Mathematics Advisory Panel, 2008; National Research Council, 2001; Robinson,

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

9 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

Menchetti, & Torgesen, 2002). Whereas relational understanding (knowing what to do and why) is

more adaptable to new tasks and easier to remember, instrumental understanding (knowing the rules

without reasons) is often fragile and error prone (e.g., Young-Loveridge, 2005; Tall, 2008).

Procedures need to be translated into an overall process that can be manipulated mentally in a

flexible way (Tall, 2008). While memorizing algorithmic procedures may have a short-term

advantage for passing an imminent test, understanding the process is necessary to the long-term

development of sophisticated mathematical thinking (Tall, 2008). Moreover, understanding the

reasoning behind the rules permits reconstruction of such rules in the case of confusion or a

memory lapse (Weber, 2002). Thus, teaching mathematics for sense-making should propitiate not

only students’ generic academic development but success on their crucial exit exam.

The goal of good teaching, then, is not the memorization of rules or algorithms, but

mathematical “sense-making” (Schoenfeld, 1992, pp. 335, 339, 340, 344). In the words of two

National Science Foundation scholars, “[i]t is important for students to see not only what they need

to know, but also why it is important” (Ramaley & Zia, 2005, p. 8.6). Again, “[l]earner-

constructed, sense-making experiences consistently are found to be the key to improved learning”

(Ramaley & Zia, 2005, p. 8.6). The true meaning of the ability to learn is not just to memorize the

rules of a particular task, but to be able to discern what the rules should be, and to make sense from

that input (Ramaley & Zia, 2005; Young-Loveridge, 2005). Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

10 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

By contrast, current-day teaching of remedial mathematics at the community college level

often is far removed from sense-making. Signed numbers are a case in point. To quote a recent

textbook expressly designed for teaching remedial mathematics at the college level, “[t]o add two

signed numbers, [i]f they have the same sign, add the absolute values and keep the sign. If they

have different signs, subtract the smaller absolute value from the larger and take the sign of the

number with the larger absolute value” Akst & Bragg, 2009, p. 347). For subtraction, the student is

directed to “[c]hange the operation of subtraction to addition, and change the number being

subtracted to its opposite,” then to “[f]ollow the rule for adding signed numbers” (Akst & Bragg,

2009, p. 355). Yet another recent textbook, designed for the same purpose, is to similar effect

(Martin-Gay, 2007). The students with the most fragile skill sets receive the least teaching for

understanding. Yet, as detailed above, it is precisely those students who most need teaching for

sense-making in order to learn.

Negative Numbers

Published material on the teaching and learning of signed numbers is scarce (Hativa &

Cohen, 1995). Such numbers have two components: a magnitude and an orientation (National

Research Council, 2001; Ball, 1993; see Bruno & Martinon, 1999). Negative numbers can be

viewed as representing both (a) an amount that is the opposite of something (for instance, -5 can Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

11 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

represent a $5 debt which is the opposite of a $5 credit balance), and (b) a location relative to zero

(for instance, -5 can represent a position that is five units away from zero, Ball, 1993). The

magnitude component leads to the concept of absolute value (Ball, 1993), meaning the distance

between the number and zero (e.g., Martin-Gay, 2007). Thus, comparing signed numbers becomes

complicated. In the words of one scholar, “[t]here is a sense in which -5 is more than -1 and equal

to 5, even though, conventionally, the ‘right’ answer is that -5 is less than both -1 and 5” (Ball,

1993, p. 379), because -5 is further along the negative side of the number line than either 0 or 5.

Simultaneously understanding that -5 is, in one sense more than -1, and, in another sense less than -

1, is central to understanding negative numbers (Ball, 1993). This tension between the magnitude of

negative numbers and their order on the number line underlies both the historical difficulties in

understanding negative numbers and many current students’ confusion over such numbers’ relative

size (Thomaidis & Tzanakis, 1970).

An additional complicating factor is notational. The minus sign appended to a negative

number has three distinct meanings: (a) the operation of subtraction, (b) a negative magnitude, and

(c) inversion, or what Piaget would have called “’reversibility’” (Nunez, 1993, p. 64), meaning that

the prescribed operation must be reversed (Nunez, 1993, pp. 63-64). Specifically, the “! ” sign

means “the opposite of,” or “the additive inverse of” (e.g., Martin-Gay, 2007, p. 32), with the result

that, if a is a number, !(!a) = a. The impact on students of these multiple meanings for the minus Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

12 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

sign can be seen from one study showing that both young and adult learners who could offset

negative and positive numbers correctly in their heads became confused by the minus signs when

asked to solve the word problems in writing. Those students who were alerted in advance that the

minus sign was being used to denote the type of magnitude (negative) rather than an operation

(subtraction) performed as well on the written problems as on mental ones (Nunez, 1993).

Because of this confusion between negative magnitude and the operation of subtraction,

there have been a number of attempts to denote negative magnitude using a symbol other than the

traditional minus sign, such as an elevated minus sign (Nunez, 1993; Cemen, 1993), or a circumflex

(Ball, 1993). Calculators usually have two types of minus signs, a smaller, slightly raised symbol to

indicate the type of magnitude and a larger, traditional minus symbol to indicate the operation of

subtraction. However, the use of the same notation for the three different meanings (subtraction,

negative magnitude, and inversion) is neither arbitrary nor accidental, because those three situations

obey the same rule with respect to operations: cancellation (Nunez, 1993). Subtracting a number is

the same as adding its inverse (Davis & Maher, 1993; Nunez, 1993; Bruno & Martinon, 1999). For

example, a gain of negative 4 is equal to a loss of positive 4. Rather than negative and positive

numbers representing two separate attributes, one for gains and the other for losses, only one

attribute is involved, the idea of change. The sign of the change alone distinguishes a gain from a

loss. Cancellation becomes automatic, with the sign of the change determining whether the balance Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

13 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

is increasing or decreasing (Davis & Maher, 1993). In effect, the two operations, addition and

subtraction, have the same meaning, what some scholars have called “double language” (Bruno &

Martinon, 1999, p. 793). “I won 2” is equivalent to “I lost -2.” Using the operation of addition,

“John had 2 and he was given 3; now he has 2 + 3 = 5” is equivalent to saying that “John had 2 and

they took away -3; now he has 2 ! (-3) = 5” (Bruno & Martinon, 1999, p. 793). Analogously, using

the operation of subtraction, “John had 3 and they took [away] 2; now he has 3 ! 2 = 1” is

equivalent to “John had 3, and he was given -2; now he has 3 + (-2) = 1” (Bruno & Martinon, 1999,

p. 793).

In addition to the single attribute issue just discussed, operations with signed numbers

reflect one of the many situations in mathematics where previously learned knowledge causes

confusion that impedes learning. For example, previously learned knowledge suggests that the

operation of subtraction produces less, a result inconsistent with negative numbers, where

subtraction produces more (Tall, 2008). (For other number line studies voicing concern over the

impact of previous learning on students’ expansion of their mathematical understanding, see, e.g.,

Merenluto, 2003; Sirotik & Zazkis, 2007, both arguing that knowledge regarding the discrete nature

of the natural numbers impedes understanding the density of real numbers, which have no “next”

number).

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

14 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

Despite these difficulties, recent research has shown that even prior to instruction children

have a rudimentary concept of negative numbers (Hativah & Cohen, 1995; Mukhopadhyay,

Resnick, & Schauble, 1990; National Research Council, 2001). For example, children have

exposure to games where points are lost, resulting in scores below zero (National Research Council,

2001), while older but unschooled children have familiarity with buying and selling, owing and

paying (Mukhopdhyay, Resnick, & Schauble, 1990). Thus, it is thought that, building on such

informal knowledge, instruction in negative numbers can begin much earlier than is currently done

(Hativah & Cohen, 1995; National Research Council, 2001; National Mathematics Advisory Panel,

2008), a conclusion surely relevant to overcoming remedial students’ antipathy to such numbers.

The Role of Diagrams in Teaching for Understanding

The 2000 Principles and Standards of the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics cite

“representation” as one of the ten components of the connected body of mathematical knowledge

that students should acquire as they progress through school (NCTM, p. 29) and state that

representations such as diagrams, displays, and symbolic expressions should be treated as essential

elements in supporting students’ understanding of mathematical concepts. As early as 1945, George

Polya, in his book How to Solve it: A New Mathematical Method, named understanding the problem

as the first of the four important elements in his heuristic for problem-solving and suggested

drawing a figure as an important aspect of this understanding (Polya, 1988, pp. 4, 7). Other authors Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

15 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

speak of visual representations as important both in aiding reflection and for communicating

mathematical ideas (e.g., Singer, 2007; Elia, Gagatsis, & Demetriou, 2007; Diezmann & English,

2001). An increasing body of research supports the view that learning is affected positively by

presenting text and illustrations together (Mayer & Sims, 1994). For instance, one study showed a

better result when visual computer-animated and verbal explanations were presented to college

students simultaneously rather than separately, leading to better science problem-solving transfer

(Mayer & Sims, 1994). Another study showed that the use of diagrams in scientific texts improved

college students’ recall of the conceptual information being presented (Mayer & Gallini, 1990).

One scholar who used meta-analysis to integrate the reports of 487 studies to assess the effects of

diagrams on mathematics problem-solving concluded that, at the 99% confidence level, an

advantage was observed for using diagrams to represent problems (Hembree, 1992). According to

this report, pictures along with text provided the largest performance differences, with formats

using “minimum verbiage” tending toward better performance, especially with slower students

(Hembree, 1992, p. 261).

More recently, some neuropsychological and neuroimaging studies have suggested the

existence of a language-independent spatial representation of numbers in the human brain (e.g.,

Singer, 2007). This topological capacity is thought to substitute global perception for discrete

numerical perception, thus fostering mathematical understanding (Singer, 2007). Admittedly, some Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

16 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

recent studies claim that diagrams compete for limited working space in short-term memory, thus

interfering with problem-solving (Pike, 2003; Elia, Gagatsis, & Demetriou, 2007). However, net-

generation students, who have come of age using the graphical interfaces of modern technology,

prefer pictorially-grounded teaching methods (Oblinger & Oblinger, 2005).

The Effect of the Number Line

The identification of the system with the number line has been described as

“one of the main ideas in mathematics” (Bruno & Martinon, 1999, p. 791). In the words of a recent

report of the National Research Council (2001), the number line:

lets you interpret whole numbers, negative numbers, and fractions all as part of one overall

system. Furthermore, it provides a uniform way to extend the rational number system to

include numbers such as π and 2 that are not rational; it provides a link between arithmetic

and geometry; and it paves the way for analytic geometry, which connects algebra and

geometry (p. 87).

For example, representing fractions and their decimal equivalents as identical points on the line can

help to obviate the student misconception that fractions and decimals constitute two separate sets of

numbers (Pagni, 2004). The use of approximation permits the correct placement on the number line

of irrational numbers as well (National Research Council, 2001). The number line thus serves as a

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

17 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

dynamical mental structure rather than as a constraining step-by-step algorithm, stimulating the

ability to transfer from one context to another and shortening learning time (Singer, 2007).

Development of such understanding takes time. While the number systems N ! Z ! Q ! R

may be seen by mathematicians as successive number systems represented on the number line

which lies in the C, each such extension involves a sophisticated learning process for

a novice (Tall, 2008). Operating with whole numbers gives the sense that addition and

multiplication produce a bigger result, while subtraction produces a smaller one, a concept that

conflicts with the behavior of , where subtracting a gives more (Tall,

2008).

Moreover, the view of the utility of number lines for teaching mathematics is not universally

shared (Diezman & Lowrie, 2007). For example, one Cypriot study found that first and second-

graders had difficulty calculating addition and subtraction problems on a number line because of

confusion over mathematical information presented implicitly in a diagram (Shiakalli & Gagatsis,

2006), while a Dutch interview of several mathematically adept youngsters found use of the number

line to be constraining (van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, 2008). In particular, a nine-year old girl forced

to use a structured number line with intervals of one to calculate the difference between 59 and 17

found herself reverting to a counting strategy (van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, 2008). These interviews, Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

18 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

while anecdotal, support the views of at least one researcher, a great proponent of using computer

diagrams in teaching mathematics, who dismisses the number line as a “mismatch” in terms of

manipulatives because its use requires “counting the counts” (Clements & McMillen, 1996, pp.

208-209). In his view, when adding 5 and 4, the student must first locate 5 on the number line and

then count the 6 as 1, the 7 as 2, etc., to reach the result (Clements & McMillen, 1996; cited with

approval, Sarama, 2004).

Others criticize use of the number line as a diagrammatic tool because of confusion caused

by the line’s geometric interpretation of a number as both a point and a vector, as compared to the

line’s arithmetic interpretation as the distance between points (e.g., Elia, Gagatsis, & Demetriou,

2007). This criticism is particularly apposite in the context of research regarding children’s

understanding of measurement, where even older children respond to measurement with a nonzero

origin by reading off whatever number on the ruler corresponds to the end of the object being

measured (National Research Council, 2001). Other difficulties encountered by students include

counting the lines instead of the spaces (Mitchell & Horne, 2008) or failing to understand what part

of the number line constitutes a whole in calculating a fractional part (Hannulu, 2003).

Although at least one study has considered the difficulties that using the number line poses

to algebra students trying to understand quadratic inequalities (Thomaidis & Tzanakis, 2007), none Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

19 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

of the criticism just voiced involves teaching the arithmetic of negative numbers. By contrast, the

report of the National Research Council (2001) particularly recommends using the number line for

such teaching. For this purpose, the report depicts two different diagrammatic techniques. The first

is a two-arrow approach that uses zero as the starting point and separate directional arrows for the

numbers being added or subtracted. Variations of this technique can be found, for instance, in

Martin-Gay (2007, a remedial college textbook) and Cemen (1993, pre-college math). A second

approach uses a single arrow. The first of two numbers in an operation functions as the starting

point, or origin, of the calculation, and the second number is illustrated by an arrow representing

both that number’s magnitude and its orientation. This latter approach in effect involves a

geometrical interpretation of addition and subtraction as translations to the right or left on the

number line (National Research Council, 2001). This second approach (using the first number as

the starting point of the calculation) is used, for instance, by Akst & Bragg (2009), another remedial

college textbook. However, The National Research Council report (2001) views this approach as

“quite sophisticated” (p. 93). In view of students’ difficulties with negative numbers, and the

difficulties noted above that students experience measuring the length of an object when using a

starting point other than zero, the second approach strikes this author as unnecessarily complex.

Description of Strategy

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

20 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

For this reason, the strategy described here starts at zero and uses multiple arrows, one for

each number involved in the computation. In each case, the relevant number and sign is depicted

with its corresponding arrow; the result is demarcated by circling the consequent number. The

theory behind this approach is to reduce the students’ need to focus on both the concept of

magnitude and the concept of orientation at the same time. The drawings are meant to be depicted

with small numbers. That way, in case of a test-induced memory lapse, the relevant rules can be

reconstructed pictorially (see Weber, 2002) and then applied to the larger numbers involved in the

examination question. More importantly, this approach is intended to indicate, without resorting to

jargon-filled absolute value rules of the type quoted earlier in this article, the relevance of

magnitude, or absolute value, to the result. By using one arrow for each number in an operation,

the automatic augmentation or cancellation resulting from a visual comparison of each such

number’s magnitude and orientation immediately becomes apparent. Further to the right on the

number line means both ‘greater in absolute value’ and ‘greater in actual value.’ Further to the left

on the number line means both ‘greater in absolute value’ and ‘less in actual value.’ The pictorial

nature of the number line permits an intuitive understanding of this distinction (see Singer, 2007).

Opposites

The first concept to be covered is that of “opposites” or “additive inverses.” These are

numbers that have the same size or magnitude (i.e., absolute value) but that lie on opposite sides of Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

21 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

zero, so that they have different actual values. Because the numbers in each such pair are

equidistant from zero, when they are added together, they produce zero (the additive identity, that

number which, when added to any real number, gives the latter number, e.g., Keenan & Dressler,

1990. See Figures 1 and 2).

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

22 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

Addition

The result of the operation of addition depends on whether the two numbers being added

have the same or different signs.

Same Signs

Adding two numbers (“addends”) with the same sign, whether positive or negative,

increases the amount of the quantity being added and therefore produces a result (“sum”) with the Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

23 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

same sign. Adding two positive numbers must produce a positive result, because the amount of the

positive quantity is increased. Once another positive quantity is added to the initial positive

quantity, the result will be correspondingly further to the right, or positive side, of the number line.

For example, (+5) + (+3) = (+8). Similarly, adding two negative numbers with the same sign must

produce a negative result, because the amount of the negative quantity is being increased. For

example, (-5) + (-3) = (-8). Adding another negative quantity to the initial negative quantity results

in a negative number that is correspondingly further to the left, or negative side, of the number line

(see Figure 3).

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

24 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

Different Signs

In the case of numbers with different signs, the result of adding such numbers depends on

which number (quantity) has the larger size or magnitude. When the magnitude of the numbers is

compared pictorially (a process equivalent to comparing the two arithmetically by subtraction), the

sign of the larger number determines whether the result will lie on the positive or on the negative

side of the number line. If the positive number is larger, the result will lie on the positive side of the

line, while if the negative number is larger, the result will lie on the negative side of the number Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

25 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

line. For example, (+8) + (-5) = (+3). The addition of the negative number reduces the result to less

than the original number, but the result is still positive (to the right of zero on the number line),

because the negative quantity being added is less than the original positive number. By contrast, (-

8) + (+5) = (-3). The negative quantity being added is greater than the original positive number,

resulting in a number to the left of zero on the number line (see Figure 4). Comparing the quantities

visually obviates the need for memorizing rules regarding comparisons of the respective numbers’

absolute values.

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

26 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

The same results obtain when the order of the addends is reversed: (-5) + (+8) = (+3), and

(+5) + (-8) = (-3). Addition is commutative, and the same analysis applies regardless of the order in

which the two numbers are added. Pictorially comparing the magnitudes of the two addends results

in automatic cancellation, with the number having the larger magnitude determining whether the

result falls on the positive side of the number line (to the right of zero) or on the negative side of the

number line (to the left of zero).

Subtraction

As can be seen from Figures 5 and 6, subtracting a number is equivalent to the addition of

its “opposite,” because the operation of subtraction reflects taking away the quantity being

subtracted. Taking away a positive amount makes the original number smaller by reducing the net

positive quantity, thus moving the result further to the left on the number line and making the

operation equivalent to subtracting a positive number. Taking away a negative amount makes the

original number larger by reducing the net negative quantity, thus moving the result further to the

right on the number line and making the operation equivalent to adding a positive number. As in the

case of addition, the question is whether the effect of the operation is to increase the total positive

amount, making the result more positive (further to the right on the number line) or to increase the

total negative amount, making the result more negative (further to the left on the number line).

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

27 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

Different Signs

Because subtracting a number is equivalent to adding its “opposite,” subtracting numbers

with different signs is equivalent to adding numbers with the same sign. Once the number to be

subtracted is changed to its opposite, the sign of that number becomes the same as the sign of the

number from which it is being subtracted. For example, (+5) – (-3) is equivalent to (+5) + (+3), so

the result is (+8), as illustrated for addition in Figure 3 above. A negative quantity, (-3), is being

taken away, making the original positive quantity, (+5), larger: (+8), which is further to the right on

the number line. By the same analysis, (-5) – (+3) = (-5) + (-3) = (-8), because the original negative

quantity, (-5), is being increased by taking away a positive amount, (+3), making the original

negative quantity larger: (-8), which is further to the left on the number line. (For illustrations using

addition, see Figure 3; for correlative examples using subtraction, see Figure 5.)

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

28 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

Same Signs

By a similar analysis, subtracting a number from another number with the same sign is

equivalent to adding a number with the opposite sign. Once the sign of the number to be subtracted

is changed to its opposite, that number has a sign different from the number from which it is being

subtracted. Therefore, the same rules apply as for adding numbers with different signs.

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

29 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

Two positive numbers. In the case of subtraction with two positive numbers, whether the

result is positive or negative depends on whether the number being subtracted is smaller or larger

than the original number. For example, (+8) – (+5) = (+8) + (-5) = (+3). The original positive

quantity, (+8), is larger than the positive quantity being taken away, (+5), so the result, (+3), is

smaller than the original number, (+8), but is still positive (to the right of zero on the number line).

By contrast, (+5) – (+8) = (+5) + (-8) = (-3). The positive quantity being subtracted, (+8), is greater

than the original number, (+5), so the result is negative (to the left of zero on the number line. See

Figure 6).

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

30 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

Two negative numbers. In the case of subtraction with two negative numbers, the original

negative quantity is being reduced by the amount of the negative quantity being taken away, so the

result is larger than the original quantity (further to the right on the number line). However, whether

the result is positive or negative depends on whether the number being subtracted is smaller or

larger, in terms of magnitude, than the original number. For example,

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

31 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

(-8) – (-5) = (-8) + (+5) = (-3). The result, (-3), is larger than the original number, (-8) (i.e., further

to the right on the number line), but the result is still negative, because the amount of negative being

taken away, (-5), is less than the amount of negative in the original number, (-8). By contrast, (-5)

! (-8) = (-5) + (+8) = (+3). The result, (+3), is not only larger than the original number, (-5), but is

positive, because the amount of negative being taken away, (-8), is greater than the original number,

(-5). (See Figure 7.)

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

32 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

Multiplication

Just as adding or subtracting a number on the number line may be interpreted as a

translation of the line to the right or the left by the number of units being added or subtracted, so

multiplication and division on the number line may be regarded as transforming the line by

stretching or shrinking it (National Research Council, 2001). However, the strategy proposed here

requires a less subtle approach. Multiplication is repeated addition (e.g., National Research Council,

2001; Martin-Gay, 2007), and division is repeated subtraction. Thus, the number line strategy

detailed above for addition and subtraction (an individual arrow to represent both the magnitude

and the orientation of each component number) serves equally well for multiplication and division.

Same Signs

Multiplication of two numbers with the same sign, whether positive or negative, will always

produce a positive result. For example, (+3) x (+3) = (+9), because (+3) x (+3) = (+3) + (+3) + (+3).

Similarly, the multiplication of two negative numbers will always produce a positive result, because

the negative amount represented by one factor is being taken away the number of times represented

by the other factor. For example, (-3) x (-3) = (+9), because (-3) x (-3) = -(-3) – (-3) – (-3) = (+3) +

(+3) + (+3). Starting at zero on the number line, the quantity (-3) is being taken away 3 times.

Thus, the result must be positive (to the right of zero on the number line). Yet again, use of the

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

33 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

number line affords a readily understood representation of this often counterintuitive result (see

Figure 8).

Different Signs

By contrast, two factors with different signs will always produce a negative result. In this

case, multiplication is equivalent to adding the negative factor the number of times prescribed by

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

34 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

the positive factor, so that the negative quantity is increased, producing a negative result. For

example, (+3) x (-3) = (-9), because (+3) x (-3) is equivalent to (-3) + (-3) + (-3), which equals

(-9). Multiplying (-3) x (3) will again produce a negative result, again for the same reason.

Multiplying (-3) x (+3) is equivalent to -(+3) – (+3) – (+3) = (-3) + (-3) + (-3) = (-9). (See Figure

9.) Like addition, multiplication is commutative (e.g., Akst & Bragg, 2009).

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

35 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

Multiple Factors

The analysis just described extends to multiple factors with different signs.

An even number of the same sign. Multiplication by an even number of positive or negative

numbers will always produce a positive result. For the positive factors, the result is repeated

addition on the positive side of the number line. For the negative factors, the result is the repeated

taking away of a negative factor, again producing a positive result by eliminating the negative

quantity.

An odd number of the same sign. Multiplying an odd number of positive factors will again

produce a positive result, because the result is repeated addition on the positive side of the number

line. However, multiplication by an odd number of negative factors will produce a negative result.

Multiplying every factor but one (i.e., multiplication by an even number of negative factors) will

first produce a positive result for the reasons set forth above. For example, given factors with the

signs +, +, -, -, -, multiplication of the first two factors (+ x +) will produce a positive result.

Multiplication of this product by the third factor, (-), will produce a negative result, and

multiplication of that product by the fourth factor, (-), will produce a positive result. Multiplication

of this positive product by the sole remaining negative factor will produce a negative result, just as

in the case of the multiplication of two factors with different signs.

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

36 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

Division

Division is traditionally defined as multiplication by the reciprocal of the divisor.

a 1 Specifically, = a • (e.g., Keenan & Dressler, 1990, Martin-Gay, 2007). Therefore, the principles b b

set forth above regarding the multiplication of signed numbers are equally applicable to the

operation of division.

Gauging Success of the Number Line Strategy

According to one longitudinal study using the number line to teach arithmetical operations,

the “effectiveness [of this strategy] in school acts as the main argument for its sustainability in

practice” (Singer, 2007, p. 91). This study showed high test results (more than a 60% success rate at

solving open-ended questions as compared to a 20% success rate for similar questions on a national

assessment, Singer, 2007). Additionally, student use of teaching strategies on assessments

constitutes support for attributing improved student performance to the students’ instruction

(Weber, 2002).

By these standards, the number line strategy presented here has assisted the remedial

community college students who are the subject of this article in increasing their understanding of

signed numbers in a way that can be retained in the context of the high stakes exit examination

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

37 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

necessary to their graduation. Data detailing the school-wide results on the algebra exit test for the

most recent academic year (the author’s first year of college mathematics teaching) appear in

Tables I-III. Table I reflects school-wide results for both semesters (including the author’s results),

while Table II reflects the author’s results. Table III compares these respective sets of results, both

by semester and over the entire academic year.

Table I

School-Wide Algebra Exit-Test Pass Rates (including author’s scores)

Semester Number Tested Number Passing Number Failing Percent Passing Fall 2008 362 200 162 55.2% Spring 2009 312 177 135 56.7% Overall 674 377 297 55.9%

Table II

Author’s Algebra Exit-Test Pass Rates

Semester Number Tested Number Passing Number Failing Percent Passing Fall 2008 12 8 4 83.3% Spring 2009 24 14 10 58.3% Overall 36 24 12 66.6%

Table III

Comparison of School-Wide and Author Exit-Test Pass Rates Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

38 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

Semester School-Wide Author Only Fall 2008 55.2% 83.3% Spring 2009 56.7% 58.3% Overall 55.9% 66.6%

The data show the author’s percentage pass rate on the algebra exit test to be higher both

each semester and overall than the school-wide pass rate in each category. These results cannot be

considered to be significant, in that they reflect a test that covers a multitude of subjects. No

separate data yet exist for the author’s number line teaching strategy. However, in-class

assessments over the course of the year reflected student use of this teaching strategy. Moreover,

preliminary results on an anonymous student questionnaire reflect appreciation for the pictorial

comparison approach. Comments include “now I understand how to add a positive and a negative,”

and “the graph basically shows you how many spaces [are] in between [the starting point and the

ending point].” Another student stated about the number line approach:

It’s really helpful because it help[s] students that [are] having problems with

sign[ed] numbers, and even more to do operation[s] +, ! , ÷, * and word problems.

Student use of the strategy on assessments, coupled with the author’s first-year pass rates on

the algebra exit test, provide evidence in support of strategies designed to teach remedial

mathematics for long-term understanding rather than for short-term rule memorization. Specific

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

39 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

testing of the number line approach and other sense-making strategies used by the author awaits

implementation in coming semesters.

Conclusion

This paper sets forth a strategy for teaching arithmetical operations with signed numbers to

college students taking remedial mathematics classes. The strategy involves the use of a single

arrow for each number in the relevant arithmetic operation, so that both the magnitude and the

orientation of each number is represented by its own arrow, obviating the need for separate mental

juggling of these two defining characteristics of signed numbers. Each number line drawing

immediately reflects either the augmentation or the cancellation process necessary to determining

the sign of the result. Thus, in an area of mathematics that has plagued mathematicians for centuries

and that is rife with rules and jargon, this pictorial strategy shows the sense behind the rules.

To the extent that high pass rates on a compulsory exit examination, together with students’

use of the strategy on in-class assessments, evidence support for this approach, the strategy has

been successful. Enabling students in remedial courses to view the study of mathematics as sense-

making, rather than as yet another onerous remedial barrier to be hurdled, should facilitate not only

success in the course but generic academic confidence, thus fostering increased student retention for

this academically-fragile population. Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

40 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

Acknowledgements

The author wishes to thank Professor Alexander Vaninsky for his thorough and meticulous

comments on an earlier draft of this paper and Professor Rees Shad for his patience and artistry in

teaching an inept pupil to construct computer number line drawings.

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

41 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

References

Akst, G., & Bragg, S. (2009). Basic Mathematics Through Applications. Boston: Pearson Education.

Ball, D. (1993). With an eye on the mathematical horizon: Dilemmas of teaching elementary school mathematics. The Elementary School Journal, 93(4), 217-241.

Bruno, A. & Martinon, A. (1999). The teaching of numerical extensions: The case of negative numbers. International Journal of Mathematics, Education, Science & Technology, 30(6), 789-809.

Cemen, P.B. (1993). Teacher to teacher: Adding and subtracting integers on the number line. The Arithmetic Teacher, 40(7), 388-389.

Clements, D. H. & Mcmillen, S. (1996). Rethinking “concrete manipulatives.” Teaching Children Mathematics, 2, 270-279.

Cunningham, A. W. (2004, April). Using the number line to teach signed numbers to learning disabled children. Unpublished manuscript prepared for Professor Herbert P. Ginsburg, Teachers College, Columbia University, NY.

Davis, R. B., & Maher, C. A. (1993). The reality of negative numbers. In R. B. Davis & C. A. Maher (Eds.), Schools, mathematics, and the world of reality (pp. 51-60). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Diezmann, C.M. & English, L.D. (2001). Promoting the use of diagrams as tools for thinking. In A. A. Cuoco & F. R. Curcio (Eds.), The roles of representation in school mathematics (pp. 77- 89). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (2001 Yearbook).

Diezmann, C. & Lowrie, T. (2007). The development of primary students’ knowledge of the structured number line. In Woo, J. H., Lew, H. C., Park, K. S. & Seo, D. Y. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education: Vol. 2, Seoul, 201-208. Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

42 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

Elia, I, Gagatsis, A, & Demetriou, A. (2007). The effects of different modes of representation on the solution of one-step additive problems. Learning and Instruction, 17, 658-672.

Ginsburg, H.P. (1997). Mathematics learning disabilities: A view from developmental psychology. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 30, 20-33.

Hannulu, M. S. (2000). Locating fractions on a number line. Proceedings of the 27th International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education Conference (held jointly with the 25th PME-NA Conference): Vol. 3, Honolulu, HA, 17-24.

Hativa, N. & Cohen, D. (1995). Self learning of negative number concepts by lower division elementary students through solving computer-provided numerical problems. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 28, 401-431.

Hembree, R. (1992). Experiments and relational studies in problem solving: A meta-analysis. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 23(3), 242-273.

Hostos Community College (2008, February). Report for President’s Executive Retreat.

Kennan, E.P., & Dressler, I. (1990). Integrated Mathematics, Course II (2nd ed.). NY: Amsco School Publications.

Martin-Gay, E. (2007). Introductory Algebra (3rd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NY: Pearson Prentice Hall.

Mayer, R.E. & Gallini, J.K. (1990). When is an illustration worth ten thousand words? Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(4), 715-726.

Mayer, R. E., & Sims, V. K. (1994). For whom is a picture worth a thousand words? Extensions of a dual-coding theory of multimedia learning. Journal of Educational Psychology 86 (3), 389-401.

Merenluto, K. (2003). Abstracting the density of numbers on the number line—A quasi- experimental study. Proceedings of the 27th International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education Conference (held jointly with the 25th PME-NA Conference): Vol. 3, Honolulu, HA, 285-292. Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

43 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

Mitchell, A. & Horne, M. (2008). Fraction number line tasks and the additivity concept of length measurement. In M. Goos, R. Brown, & K. Makar (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Mathematics Education Research Group of Australia (pp. 353-360). MERGA.

Mukhopadhyay, S, Resnick, L., & Schauble, L. (1990, July). Social sense-making in mathematics: Children’s ideas of negative numbers. Paper presented at the Psychology of Mathematics Education Conference, Mexico City. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh (Learning Research and Development Center, 1-11).

National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (2000). Principles and standards for school mathematics. Reston, VA: Author.

National Mathematics Advisory Panel (2008). Foundations for success: The final report of the National Mathematics Advisory Panel. Washington, D. C.: U.S. Department of Education.

National Research Council (2001). Adding it up: Helping children learn mathematics. J. Kilpatrick, J. Swafford, & B. Findell (Eds.), Mathematics Learning Study Committee, Center for Education, Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

Nunes, T. (1993). Learning mathematics: Perspectives from everyday life. In R. B. Davis & C. A. Maher, (Eds.), Schools, mathematics, and the world of reality (pp. 61-78). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Oblinger, D. G. & Oblinger, J. L. (2005). Is it age or IT: First steps toward understanding the net generation. In D. G. Oblinger & J. L. Oblinger (Eds.), Educating the net generation (pp. 2.1- 2.20). Educause: www.educause.edu/educatingthenetgen/.

Pagni, D. (2004). Fractions and Decimals. Australian Mathematics Teacher, 60(4), 28-30.

Pyke, C. L. (2003). The use of symbols, words, and diagrams as indicators of mathematical cognition: A causal model. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 34(5), 406-432.

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

44 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

Polya, G. (1988). How to solve it: A new aspect of mathematical method (2nd ed). NJ: Princeton University Press (1st Princeton Science Library Printing).

Ramaley, J. A. & Zia, L. (2005). The real versus the possible: Closing the gaps in engagement and learning. In D. G. Oblinger & J. L. Oblinger (Eds.), Educating the net generation (pp. 8.1- 8.21). Educause: www.educause.edu/educatingthenetgen/.

Robinson, C.S., Menchetti, B.M., & Torgesen, J. K. (2002). Toward a two-factor theory of one type of mathematics disability. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 17(2), 81-89.

Sarama, J. (2004). Technology in early childhood mathematics: Building Blocks as an innovative technology-based curriculum. In D. H. Clements & J. Sarama (Eds.), Engaging young children in mathematics: Standards for early childhood mathematics education (pp. 361- 375). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Schoenfeld, A. H. (1992). Learning to think mathematically: Problem Solving, metacognition, and sense making in mathematics. In D. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 334-370). NY: Macmillan.

Shiakalli, M., & Gagatsis, A. (2006). Compartmentalization of representation tasks related to addition and subtraction using the number line. In Novotna, J., Moraova, H., Kratka, M., & Stehlikova, N. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education: Vol. 5, Prague, 105-112.

Singer, F. M. (2007). Beyond conceptual change: Using representations to integrate domain- specific structural models in learning mathematics. Mind, Brain, and Education, 1(2), 84-97.

Sirotic, N. & Zazkis, R. (2007). Irrational numbers: The gap between formal and intuitive knowledge. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 65, 49-76.

Tall, D. (2008). The transition to formal thinking in mathematics. Mathematics in Education Research Journal, 20(2), 5-24.

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj

45 MATHEMATICS TEACHING-RESEARCH JOURNAL ONLINE V 3, N 4

December 2009

Thomaidis, Y., & Tzanakis, C. (2007). The notion of historical “parallelism” revisited: Historical evolution and students’ conception of the order relation on the number line. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 66, 165-183.

Van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, M. (2008). Learning from “didactikids”: An impetus for revisiting the empty number line. Mathematics Education Research Journal 20(3), 6-31.

Weber, K. (2002). Developing students’ understanding of exponents and logarithms. In D.S. Mewborn, P. Sztajn, D. Y. White, H. G. Wiegel, R. L. Bryant, & K. Nooney (Eds.), Proceedings of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, North American Chapter: Vol. 2, Athens, GA, 1019-1027.

Young-Loveridge, J. (2005). Fostering multiplicative thinking using array-based materials. Australian Mathematics Teacher, 61(3), 34-40.

Readers are free to copy, display, and distribute this article, as long as the work is attributed to the author(s) and Mathematics Teaching-Research

Journal On-Line, it is distributed for non-commercial purposes only, and no alteration or transformation is made in the work. All other

uses must be approved by the author(s) or MT-RJoL. MT-RJoL is published jointly by the Bronx Colleges of the City University of New York.

www.hostos.cuny.edu/departments/math/mtrj