Nesting Ecology of the Great Horned Owl Bubo Virginianus in Central Western Utah

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Nesting Ecology of the Great Horned Owl Bubo Virginianus in Central Western Utah Brigham Young University BYU ScholarsArchive Theses and Dissertations 1968-08-01 Nesting ecology of the great horned owl Bubo virginianus in central western Utah Dwight Glenn Smith Brigham Young University - Provo Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/etd BYU ScholarsArchive Citation Smith, Dwight Glenn, "Nesting ecology of the great horned owl Bubo virginianus in central western Utah" (1968). Theses and Dissertations. 7883. https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/etd/7883 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by BYU ScholarsArchive. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of BYU ScholarsArchive. For more information, please contact [email protected], [email protected]. NESTING ECOLOGYOF THE GREATHORNED OWL BUBOVIRGINIANUS IN CENTRALWESTERN UTAH L A Thesis Presented to the Department of Zoology and Entomology Brigham Young University In Partial Fulfi I lment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Science by Dwight G. Smith August 1968 This thesis by Dwight G. Smith is accepted in its present form by the Department of Zoology and Entomolo�y of Brigham Young University as satisfying the thesis require­ ment for the degree of Master of Science. Typed by Beth Anne Smith f i i ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Grateful acknowledgment is made for the valuable sug- gestions and help given by the chairman of my advisory com- mittee, Dr. Joseph R. Murphy, and other members of my com- mittee, Dr. C. Lynn Hayward and Dr. Joseph R. Murdock. Ap- preciation is extended to Dr. Herbert H. Frost for his editor- ial help in the preparation of the manuscript. Financial assistance was provided by a grant from the National Audubon Society. The Department of Zoology and Ento- mology, Brigham Young University, supplied laboratory space, equipment and supplies. Grateful recognition is extended to D. K. Rawlings for his help in the preparation of the thesis. Finally, to my wife, Beth Anne, to whom the thesis is dedi- cated, love and regard are expressed for the patience shown and encouragement given throughout the field work and writing of the manuscript. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS Page ACKNOWLEDGMENTS . • • . • • . i i i LIST OF TABLES . • • . V LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS . • . V INTRODUCTION . • 1 METHODSAND PROCEDURES• • • . • . • . 3 Study Area • . • . • • . • 3 Field Studies • • • . • • . • • • 5 RESULTS • . • . • . 7 Nesting population and Distribution • . • • 7 Territoriality • • . • . • • . • 16 Predation . • . • • • . 19 DISCUSSIONS AND CONCLUSIONS • • • • • • • . • 23 SUMMARY. • • • . • . • . 27 LITERATURECITED . • . • . • • . • 29 V LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1. Nesting population and density . • . 7 2. Site selection of the Great Horned Owl • • • • 9 3. Nest dimensions of the Great Horned Owl . • . 10 4. Nesting success of the Great Horned Owl • . • • 13 5. Food of Horned Owls in the study area . • . • . 21 LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS Figure Page 1. Study area location and vegetation. • 4 2. Nest locations of the Great Horned Owl • • • • 12 3. Territories of three pairs of Great Horned Owls •• 17 INTRODUCTION The Great Horned Owl, Bubo virginianus, is a common nocturnal raptor of North and South America, occurring in most habitat types. Although it exerts considerable predatory influence on the mammalian and avian populations of its locale, relatively little is known of its natural history and ecology due to its secretive habits and the inacessability of its nesting sites. Further interest is warranted in this raptor because of its decreasing numbers caused by a combination of factors, including habitat disruption due to encroaching civilization, road kil Is, and indiscriminate shooting by hunters. This study describes the nesting ecology of the Great Horned Owl in the Great Basin deserts of central western Utah. Major aspects include nesting population and distribution, territoriality, and predation. There are few comprehensive studies on the ecology of this species and there appear to be no previous detailed investigations of it in arid or semi-arid habitats. Included among the more notewo·rthy studies are Baumgartner's (1938) work on its distribution and territoriality in the New York forests, Errington's (1932) behavioral and predation studies in the cottonwoods of Wisconsin and the rnidwest, Orians and 2 Kuhlman•s (1956) population and distribution study, also in the Wisconsin area, and Fitch's (1940, 1947) population and predation studies in the California chaparral. Utah liter- ~ture on this species is limited, but includes notes on its nest site selection (Sugden, 1928), breeding records (Bee and Hutchings, 1942), some life history and ecology data (Bee, unpublished data), a trapping note reported by Stanford (1931), and numerous focal distributional information as exemplified by Hayward's (1967) Birds of the Upper Colorado River Basin. METHODSAND PROCEDURES Study Area The study locale of the first (1967) nesting season was the Thorpe Hil Is of central western Utah, an area of approximately twelve square miles of elevated topography located in Tooele and Utah counties (see Fig. 1). In the second study season (1968) the area was enlarged to include the adjacent Topliff Hi I Is, which increased the study area to some twenty-five square miles. These hil Is are low-lying northern extensions of the Tintic Range, ·with their highest elevations rising to some 1000 feet above the surrounding valley floors of Cedar and Rush Valleys. Geologically the hills consist of masses of sandstone, limestone, and ortho- quartzites occurring in alternating layers (Bui lock, 1951). Vegetation composition was determined by the quarter method (trees) and the plot method (shrubs). The vegetation is dominated by Juniperus osteosperma and Artemisia tridentata, both of which serve as nesting material and nest sites for predatory birds, including the Great Horned Owl. Pinus monophylla and Pseudotsuga taxifolia also occur as do the shrubs Chr~sothamnus nauseosus, Atriplex confertifolia, Eurotia lanata and Sarcobatus vermiculatus. Principal grasses include Oryzopsfs hymenoides, Hilaria jamesii and Agropyron spicatum. 4 VEGETATION 0 Des•t Shrub e Douglas Fir {j Plnyon.Jooiper 10/am (/JJPlnyon•.bliper IDD/auo \ Fig. 1: Study Area Location and Vegetation 5 These topographic and vegetational features provide numerous suitable habitats for raptors, and the study area supports a high winter and summer population of avian pred- ator species. Field Studies The field work.for this study was conducted for a per- iod of two nesting seasons, the spring. of 1967 and the spring of 1968. Field observations were aided by the use of 7 by 50 binoculars and a 20 power spotting scope. Nests on the study area were located by a systematic search of all potential sites, that is, alf cliff lines, rock outcroppings, abandoned quarries and wooded areas. Those nests found were then plotted on a master map to det~rmfne the re- lative nesting population and distribution of the owls. Gaps in the suspected distribution were then rechecked several times during the nesting season for signs of roosting birds or neBts previously missed. It was felt that al I nests active during a given season were discovered, and that an accurate picture of the nesting population and distribution of the Great Horned Owl was established during the two nesting sea- sons of the study. Territorial studies were conducted at three nests, each representative of a different site. These included a west facing quarry nest and two cliff nests, one of which was loc- ated in an east-opening canyon and the other situated on a plateau with a western exposure. Territoriality was determined by visual observations during the hours from sunset until dark 6 and was conducted from constructed blinds. Additional infor- mation on bird movements was gained from the plotting of sight- ing ·occurrences during the night. Owl movements were plotted, then territory size determined by planimeter measurements of the polygon formed by connection of points denoting the ex- treme distances from the nest site, fol lowing the method dis- cussed by Odum and Kuenzler (1955). Owl predation was determined by (I) weekly nest visi- tations to record prey types and frequency, and (2) pellet analysis. On discovery of a nest site egg productivity was checked and the nest placed under weekly observation to note adult habits and egg pipping time. After the young hatched the nest was inspected more frequently to ascertain food procure- ment items and adult territorial behavior. At nest abandon- ment with the development of flight by the young, nest mea- surements were taken and the remaining food items recorded and the pellets removed for analysis. Data taken included nest size and material composition, its height relative to cliff size and its elevation. Additional data gathered included information on inter- specific relations as determined by a search and location of active predatory bird nests in the vicinity of the owl nest sites. RESULTS Nesting population and distribution Fourteen nests were found on the study area during the two breeding seasons, including four during the 1967 season and ten the fol lowing year on the enlarged study area. Horned Owl population density on the study area averaged .38 nesting pairs per square mile during the two study years. During this period five additional nests were found outside the study area but in similar habitat type; unless otherwise noted, al I observations pertain to the study area nests. TABLE1 NESTINGPOPULATION AND DENSITY Year Number of Nests Nesting Density* 1967 4 .36 1968 10 .40 * pa1rs. per square m1. I e 8 Nests and nest site Nest site and nest dimension data are presented in Tables II and III. Al I four of the 1967 season nest sites were used the fol lowing year, thus ten different sites were located during the two year study. Birds on the study area utilized cliffs, abandoned quarries, caves and junipers for nest sites. Favored sites were large sandstone or limestone cliff faces or rock outcroppings. Three sites were deep In canyons, one on a plateau, and the remainder in the hi I Is and foothi I Is.
Recommended publications
  • Behavioral Profiles
    Terra Explorer Volume 1 The Terra Explorer series is copyrighted © 2009 by William James Davis. All rights reserved. Copyrights of individual stories in- cluded in the last section of the book, “Adventures in the field,” belong to their respective authors. (see following page for more details) William James Davis, Ph.D. Copyright © 2009 by Wm. James Davis ISBN 978-0-9822654-0-6 0-9822654-0-9 Also available as an eBook. To order, visit: http://www.TerraNat.com The Terra Explorer series is copyrighted © 2009 by William James Davis. All rights reserved. Copyrights of individual stories included in the last section of the book, “Adventures in the field,” belong to their respective authors. No part of this book may be used or reproduced in any manner whatsoever without written permission from the publisher and respective authors, except in the case of brief quotations embedded in critical articles and reviews. Contact the publisher to request permis- sion by visiting www.terranat.com. Photos on the front and back covers (Black Skimmer and Cuban Anole, respec- tively) by Karen Anthonisen Finch. For Linda Jeanne Mealey, who inspired my dreams to explore the natural world. Also by William James Davis Australian Birds: A guide and resource for interpreting behavior TableTable of of contents contents Introduction Evolution of a concept The challenge Book’s organization and video projects Participating in the Terra Explorer Project Behavioral profiles 8 Common Loon 11 American White Pelican 14 Anhinga 17 Cattle Egret 20 Mallard 23 Bald Eagle 26
    [Show full text]
  • The Cognitive Animal Empirical and Theoretical Perspectives on Animal Cognition
    This PDF includes a chapter from the following book: The Cognitive Animal Empirical and Theoretical Perspectives on Animal Cognition © 2002 Massachusetts Institute of Technology License Terms: Made available under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International Public License https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ OA Funding Provided By: The open access edition of this book was made possible by generous funding from Arcadia—a charitable fund of Lisbet Rausing and Peter Baldwin. The title-level DOI for this work is: doi:10.7551/mitpress/1885.001.0001 Downloaded from http://direct.mit.edu/books/edited-volume/chapter-pdf/677490/9780262268028_c001600.pdf by guest on 29 September 2021 17 Spatial and Social Cognition in Corvids: An Evolutionary Approach Russell P. Balda and Alan C. Kamil research plan using controlled laboratory ex- Research Questions periments and captive birds. Fortunately, nut- crackers are quite willing to cache and recover The central research questions that have guided seeds in laboratory settings and do so with a high our studies since 1981 combine issues and tech- degree of accuracy, both in a sandy floor indoors niques from both comparative psychology and (Balda 1980; Balda and Turek 1984) or out of avian ecology. Most of our questions originate doors (Vander Wall 1982), as well as in a room from the cognitive implications of extensive field with a raised floor containing sand-filled cups as studies on the natural history, ecology, and potential cache sites (Kamil and Balda 1985). behavior of seed-caching corvids. Because our The ability to study caching and cache recovery questions have evolved as our studies progressed, under controlled laboratory conditions allowed we have chosen to give a historical perspective us to test hypotheses on how the nutcrackers find outlining the progression of our ideas and ques- their caches.
    [Show full text]
  • Clark's Nutcracker Factsheet 6: Population Trends
    United States Department of Agriculture D E E Forest Service A Monitoring Challenge: P R A R U TM U LT ENT OF AGRIC Pacific Northwest Region Clark’s Nutcracker Population Trends 2011 ARE CLARK’S nutcrackers declining? Many resource What’s the current status How reliable are the managers think so, yet long-term national surveys say no. of Clark’s nutcrackers? surveys? Problem is, these birds are so hard to monitor. How can we improve on methods to accurately detect changes in Clark’s Breeding Bird Surveys These annual bird counts nutcracker populations? (conducted nationwide have limitations for projecting each May since 1966) show population trends in Clark’s a significant range-wide nutcrackers for several BACKGROUND increase in numbers of reasons: We investigated habitat use, caching behavior, and Clark’s nutcrackers from 1966 • Most routes are along migratory patterns in Clark’s nutcrackers in the Pacific through 2007. Christmas Bird established roads to Northwest using radio telemetry. Over 4 years (2006– Counts (done in December facilitate access by 2009), we captured 54 adult nutcrackers at 10 sites in the across the country since 1900) volunteer surveyors; Cascade and Olympic Mountains in Washington State. show fairly strong population species occupying remote We fitted nutcrackers with a back-pack style harness. fluctuations, but no overall terrain, like Clark’s The battery life on the radio tags was 450 days, and trend (either increasing or nutcrackers, might be we tracked nutcrackers year-round, on foot (to obtain decreasing). Data from these poorly sampled. behavior observations) and via aircraft (to obtain point annual surveys (shown in the • Clark’s nutcrackers breed locations).
    [Show full text]
  • The Perplexing Pinyon Jay
    University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Papers in Behavior and Biological Sciences Papers in the Biological Sciences 1998 The Ecology and Evolution of Spatial Memory in Corvids of the Southwestern USA: The Perplexing Pinyon Jay Russell P. Balda Northern Arizona University,, [email protected] Alan Kamil University of Nebraska - Lincoln, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/bioscibehavior Part of the Behavior and Ethology Commons Balda, Russell P. and Kamil, Alan, "The Ecology and Evolution of Spatial Memory in Corvids of the Southwestern USA: The Perplexing Pinyon Jay" (1998). Papers in Behavior and Biological Sciences. 17. https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/bioscibehavior/17 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Papers in the Biological Sciences at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in Papers in Behavior and Biological Sciences by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. Published (as Chapter 2) in Animal Cognition in Nature: The Convergence of Psychology and Biology in Laboratory and Field, edited by Russell P. Balda, Irene M. Pepperberg, and Alan C. Kamil, San Diego (Academic Press, 1998), pp. 29–64. Copyright © 1998 by Academic Press. Used by permission. The Ecology and Evolution of Spatial Memory in Corvids of the Southwestern USA: The Perplexing Pinyon Jay Russell P. Balda 1 and Alan C. Kamil 2 1 Department of Biological Sciences, Northern
    [Show full text]
  • Reproductive Success and Nest-Site Selection in a Cooperative Breeder: Effect of Experience and a Direct Benefit of Helping
    TheAuk 116(2):355-363, 1999 REPRODUCTIVE SUCCESS AND NEST-SITE SELECTION IN A COOPERATIVE BREEDER: EFFECT OF EXPERIENCE AND A DIRECT BENEFIT OF HELPING B. J. HATCHWELL,• A. E RUSSELL,M. K. FOWLIE,AND D. J. Ross Departmentof Animal and Plant Sciences, University of Sheffield,Sheffield S10 2TN, UnitedKingdom ABSTRACT.--Wedetermined whether nest-site characteristics influence reproductive suc- cessand whetherexperience influences nest-site selection in a populationof cooperatively breedingLong-tailed Tits (Aegithaloscaudatus). Nest predationwas high; only 17%of breed- ing attemptsresulted in fledgedyoung. The heightof nestswas an importantdeterminant of success;low nestswere significantlymore successfulthan high nests.A breeder'sage, natal nestsite, and breedingexperience had no significanteffect on nest-siteselection. How- ever,failed breeders who helped at thesuccessful nests of conspecificsbuilt subsequentnests lowerthan nestsbuilt prior to their helpingexperience. Failed breeders who did not help showedno reductionin tlseheight of subsequentnests. Moreover, the subsequentrepro- ductivesuccess of failed breederswho helped was significantlyhigher than that of failed breederswho did not help.We concludethat helpersgain informationon nest-sitequality throughtheir helping experience and thus gain a directfitness benefit from their cooperative behavior.We suggest that experience as a helperoffers a morereliable cue to nest-sitequality thanbreeding experience because helpers are associatedwith nestsonly during the nestling phasewhen few nestsare depredated.In contrast,although successful breeders may expe- riencesuccess with a low nest,they are evenmore likely to haveexperienced the failureof low nestsbecause of the high rate of nestpredation. Received 26 December1997,accepted 28 July1998. A MAJORDETERMINANT of reproductivesuc- acteristics,then no consistentselection may ex- cessfor manyorganisms is the abilityof breed- ist for choiceof particularnest sites. However, ers to protecttheir offspringfrom predation.
    [Show full text]
  • Cottontail Rabbits
    Cottontail Rabbits Biology of Cottontail Rabbits (Sylvilagus spp.) as Prey of Golden Eagles (Aquila chrysaetos) in the Western United States Photo Credit, Sky deLight Credit,Photo Sky Cottontail Rabbits Biology of Cottontail Rabbits (Sylvilagus spp.) as Prey of Golden Eagles (Aquila chrysaetos) in the Western United States U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service Regions 1, 2, 6, and 8 Western Golden Eagle Team Front Matter Date: November 13, 2017 Disclaimer The reports in this series have been prepared by the U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service (Service) Western Golden Eagle Team (WGET) for the purpose of proactively addressing energy-related conservation needs of golden eagles in Regions 1, 2, 6, and 8. The team was composed of Service personnel, sometimes assisted by contractors or outside cooperators. The findings and conclusions in this article are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the views of the U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service. Suggested Citation Hansen, D.L., G. Bedrosian, and G. Beatty. 2017. Biology of cottontail rabbits (Sylvilagus spp.) as prey of golden eagles (Aquila chrysaetos) in the western United States. Unpublished report prepared by the Western Golden Eagle Team, U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service. Available online at: https://ecos.fws.gov/ServCat/Reference/Profile/87137 Acknowledgments This report was authored by Dan L. Hansen, Geoffrey Bedrosian, and Greg Beatty. The authors are grateful to the following reviewers (in alphabetical order): Katie Powell, Charles R. Preston, and Hillary White. Cottontails—i Summary Cottontail rabbits (Sylvilagus spp.; hereafter, cottontails) are among the most frequently identified prey in the diets of breeding golden eagles (Aquila chrysaetos) in the western United States (U.S.).
    [Show full text]
  • Informes Individuales IUCN 2018.Indd
    IUCN SSC Lagomorph Specialist Group 2018 Report Andrew Smith Hayley Lanier Co-Chairs Mission statement Targets for the 2017-2020 quadrennium Andrew Smith (1) To promote the conservation and effective Assess (2) Hayley Lanier sustainable management of all species of Red List: (1) improve knowledge and assess- lagomorph through science, education and ment of lagomorph systematics, (2) complete Red List Authority Coordinator advocacy. all Red List reassessments of all lagomorph Charlotte Johnston (1) species. Projected impact for the 2017-2020 Research activities: (1) improve knowledge of Location/Affiliation quadrennium Brachylagus idahoensis; (2) examine popula- (1) School of Life Sciences, Arizona State The Lagomorph Specialist Group (LSG) is tion trends of all lagomorphs in the western University, Tempe, Arizona, US “middle-sized” – not a single species, nor United States; (3) improve knowledge of Lepus (2) Sam Noble Museum, University of Oklahoma, composed of hundreds of species. We have callotis; (4) improve knowledge of Lepus fagani, Norman, Oklahoma, US slightly less than 100 species in our brief. L. habessinicus, and L. starcki in Ethiopia; However, these are distributed around the (5) improve knowledge of Lepus flavigularis; Number of members globe, and there are few similarities among (6) improve knowledge of all Chinese Lepus; 73 any of our many forms that are Red List clas- (7) improve knowledge of Nesolagus netscheri; sified as Threatened. Thus, we do not have a (8) improve knowledge of Nesolagus timminsi; Social networks single programme or a single thrust; there is no (9) improve knowledge of Ochotona iliensis; Website: one-size-fits-all to our approach. LSG members (10) improve surveys of poorly-studied www.lagomorphspecialistgroup.org largely work independently in their region, and Ochotona in China; (11) understand the role the Co-Chairs serve more as a nerve centre.
    [Show full text]
  • Biology, Legal Status, Control Materials, and Directions for Use
    VERTEBRATE PEST CONTROL HANDBOOK - MAMMALS BIOLOGY, LEGAL STATUS, CONTROL MATERIALS, AND DIRECTIONS FOR USE Rabbits – Black-tailed Jackrabbit, Cottontail Rabbit, Brush Rabbit Family: Leporidae Fig. 1. Black-tailed jackrabbit (Lepus californicus) Fig. 2. Desert cottontail rabbit (Sylvilagus audobonii) Fig. 3. Brush rabbit (S. bachmani) Introduction: Three rabbits are common to California: the black-tailed jackrabbit, the cottontail, and the brush rabbit. Of these, the jackrabbit is the most destructive because of its greater size and occurrence in agricultural areas. Cottontails are common pests in landscaped areas. Hereinafter ‘rabbits’ shall refer to all three species unless distinguished otherwise. Rabbits can be destructive and eat a wide variety of plants, grasses, grains, alfalfa, vegetables, fruit trees, vines, and many ornamentals. They also cause damage to plastic irrigation through their gnawing activities. Identification: The black-tailed jackrabbit (Fig. 1) is about the size of a house cat, 17 to 22 inches long. It has long ears, short front legs, and long hind legs. They populate open or semi open lands in valleys and foothills. Desert cottontail (Fig. 2) and brush rabbits (Fig. 3) are smaller (14.5 to 15.5, and 12.0 to 14.5 inches in length, respectively) and have shorter ears. Brush rabbits can be distinguished from cottontails by their smaller, inconspicuous tail and uniformly colored ears that lack a black tip. They inhabit brushy areas where cover is dense; landscaped areas provide excellent habitat. They can also be found beneath junipers and other large, low-growing evergreen shrubs. 262 VERTEBRATE PEST CONTROL HANDBOOK - MAMMALS Legal Status: Black-tailed jackrabbits, desert cottontails, and brush rabbits are classed as nongame animals in most states, but are considered game mammals by the California Fish and Game Code.
    [Show full text]
  • Pinyon Jay Movement, Nest Site Selection, Nest Fate
    PINYON JAY MOVEMENT, NEST SITE SELECTION, NEST FATE, AND RENESTING IN CENTRAL NEW MEXICO By MICHAEL COULTER NOVAK Bachelor of Science in Zoology University of California, Santa Barbara Santa Barbara, California 2010 Submitted to the Faculty of the Graduate College of the Oklahoma State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE December, 2019 PINYON JAY MOVEMENT, NEST SITE SELECTION, NEST FATE, AND RENESTING IN CENTRAL NEW MEXICO Thesis Approved: Dr. Loren M. Smith Thesis Adviser Dr. Scott T. McMurry Dr. Craig A. Davis ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I want to thank my advisor, Dr. Loren Smith, for his expert assistance guiding me through my thesis. Through his mentorship I learned to be a more effective writer and scientist. Thank you to my committee members, Dr. Scott McMurry and Dr. Craig Davis, for their valuable input on my research. Much thanks to Oklahoma State University for providing excellent academic resources and being a great place to develop professional skills required for today’s workforce. I want to thank my parents, Michael Novak and Kathleen Murphy, who have always encouraged me to work hard and be happy. I’m especially grateful for their understanding and encouragement throughout my transient career as a seasonal field biologist. Thanks, Dad, for encouraging me to find a job where they pay me to travel. Thanks, Mom, for encouraging me to pursue higher education and for keeping me on track. Thanks to my sister, Marisol Novak, for her siblingship and being a willing ear to vent my problems to. My family has always believed in me and they inspire me to continue putting one foot in front of the other, even when things are difficult.
    [Show full text]
  • SIA Border Cameras Report
    Sky Island Alliance’s Border Camera Summary JUNE 28 2019 SKY ISLAND ALLIANCE by: Meagan Bethel 1 Sky Island Alliance’s Border Camera Summary This report summarizes the data collected by Sky Island Alliance’s remote sensing wildlife cameras over 23 months of sampling effort starting July 2017 and running to the most recent data of May 2019. While Sky Island Alliance maintains more than thirty cameras, 24 cameras are within 35 miles of the United States-Mexico border. Those 24 cameras along the border contributed photo data to this report. 2 Total number of photos for all border cameras by month 4000 3500 3000 2500 2000 Number of Photos 1500 1000 500 0 Figure 1 The total number of unique wildlife photos collected by Sky Island Alliance’s border cameras that spans 23 months. Two peaks can be seen in October 2017 and May 2018, however the total number of photos collected has been declining over time. 3 Table 1 A summary table of all species detected by the remote cameras with in sampling period. There were 45,886 photos with animals in them. Humans and domestic animals are separated from wildlife. Wildlife Number of Photos Humans and Number of Photos Domestic animals White-tailed deer 23994 Cattle 12001 Javelina 1915 Human hiker 1093 White-nosed coati 1159 Domestic dog 292 Mule deer 923 Vehicle 1000 Mountain lion 827 Horseback rider 90 Coyote 758 Border patrol 87 Raccoon 619 Cat 11 Turkey 398 Black Bear 366 Spotted skunk Grey fox 242 Bobcat 224 Hooded skunk 162 Desert cottontail 100 Bird species 86 Striped skunk 78 Mexican jay 68 Antelope
    [Show full text]
  • Desert Cottontails
    Name: ____________________________________________ Desert Cottontails The desert cottontail is a type of rabbit that lives in the dry, dusty areas of New Mexico, Arizona, and Western Texas. They can also live as far north as Montana and as far west as California. This type of cottontail is similar to other rabbit species because it has an easily-noticed white fluffy tail, but it has larger ears that usually stand straight up. They have the potential to weigh up to 3.3 pounds (1. 5 kg) and grow to 20 inches (51 cm) in length. Desert cottontails tend to be most active during the morning and late afternoon hours. During the day they avoid the hot desert sun, and seek shelter to keep themselves cool. Desert cottontails will eat almost any type of plant, and sometimes they’ll munch on small bugs as well. Because the desert plants they eat usually contain moisture, desert cottontails rarely need to drink water. The cottontail has many predators, including bobcats, mountain lions, owls, eagles, rattlesnakes, and coyotes. Sometimes they are able to outsmart the large predators by running very fast and diving into small holes where bigger animals cannot go. They usually run in a zigzag pattern so they’re harder to catch. Cottontails are one of the few species of rabbits that are also able to climb trees when they’re trying to escape a predator. When she is ready to have babies, a female cottontail will make a nest of grass and fur. They can have as many as ten baby cottontails at one time! And sometimes they can have four litters in a single year.
    [Show full text]
  • Cottontails and Jackrabbits by Jim Stanley
    Cottontails and Jackrabbits: It Wouldn’t Be Texas Without Them Two of my favorite native animals are cottontails and jackrabbits. As a kid in West Texas, I could always find at least one or the other in a five-minute walk into the pasture. They are nowhere near that populous now. Cottontails are rabbits, but despite the name, jackrabbits are not. Jackrabbits are hares. Rabbits are born naked, blind and almost helpless. Hares are born fully furred, eyes open and able to hop. Neither rabbits nor hares are rodents, they belong to the order Lagomorpha and the family Leporidae. Three of the four species of rabbits in Texas have ranges that overlap in our part of the Hill Country, the Swamp rabbit (Sylvilagus aquaticus), the Desert cottontail (Sylvilagus audubonii), and the Eastern cottontail (Sylvilagus floridanus). The Swamp rabbit is slightly larger than the other two with slightly longer ears. As the name suggests, it mainly inhabits areas along rivers and creeks or marshes. It very seldom ventures out in the daytime, unless flushed from its bed. All rabbits can swim, but this one is the only one to do so on its own without being chased. Its main range is the eastern part of the Hill Country and further east. The Desert cottontail has very slightly longer ears, relative to its foot length, than the Eastern cottontail, a difference too slight to tell by simply watching the animal. Its main range is the western Hill Country and further west. Since telling the difference among these three in the field is very difficult, and since the most widespread rabbit in Texas is the Eastern cottontail, it is the only rabbit I will discuss further.
    [Show full text]