The Emotional Stroop Effect in Anxiety Disorders General Emotionality Or Disorder Specificity?

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Emotional Stroop Effect in Anxiety Disorders General Emotionality Or Disorder Specificity? Anxiety Disorders 15 Q2001) 147±159 The emotional Stroop effect in anxiety disorders General emotionality or disorder specificity? Eni S. Beckera,*, Mike Rincka, JuÈrgen Margraf a, Walton T. Rothb aTU, Dresden, Germany bVA Palo Alto Health Care System, Palo Alto, CA, USA Received 8 January 1999; received in revised form 22 April 1999; accepted 15 October 1999 Abstract Selective attentional biases, often documented with a modified Stroop task, are considered to play an important role in the etiology and maintenance of anxiety. Two competing explanations for these effects are selectivity for highly emotional words in general vs. selectivity for disorder-specific words. We tested these explanations in 32 patients with generalized anxiety disorder QGAD), 29 patients with social phobia QSP), and 31 non-anxious controls. Stimuli were of four kinds: GAD-related words, SP-related words, words with a neutral valence, and words with a positive valence. Different attentional biases were observed: GAD patients were slowed by all types of emotional words, while SP patients were distracted specifically by speech-related words. D 2001 Elsevier Science Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: Attention; Generalized anxiety disorder; Social phobia; Stroop task 1. Introduction Cognitive models of anxiety have received increasing empirical support over the past decade Qe.g., Barlow 1988; Beck, Emery, & Greenberg, 1985; 1992). * Corresponding author. Department of Clinical Psychology, Dresden University of Technology, D-01062 Dresden, Germany. E-mail address: [email protected] QE.S. Becker). 0887-6185/01/$ ± see front matter D 2001 Elsevier Science Inc. All rights reserved. PII: S0887-6185Q01)00055-X 148 E.S. Becker et al. / Anxiety Disorders 15 F2001) 147±159 These models focus on attentional processes because one function of anxiety is the detection of threat, enabling the individual to react quickly. According to cognitive theories Qe.g., Bower, 1981, 1987; Williams, Watts, MacLeod, & Mathews, 1988, 1997), anxiety patients' attention should be biased selectively towards threatening stimuli. To understand cognitive biases of attention, psychological theories and clinical research have increasingly turned to infor- mation processing paradigms derived from experimental cognitive psychology. A classical paradigm adapted to this purpose was originally introduced by Stroop Q1935). The modified version of the Stroop task is the paradigm most frequently used to show attentional biases in anxiety patients. The general approach of this task is to demonstrate selectively biased attention by showing diminished performance in a task condition where threatening stimuli could cause distraction. Specifically, participants are asked to name the ink color of words, disregarding their content. The basic finding is that, compared to other words and other participants, patients with anxiety disorders take longer to name the color of threatening words. The modified Stroop task has been used successfully with a variety of anxiety patients, among them patients with panic disorder QEhlers, Margraf, Davies, & Roth, 1988; McNally, Riemann, & Kim, 1990; McNally, Riemann, Louro, Lukach, & Kim, 1992), obsessive±compulsive disorder QIlai, Shoyer, & Foa, 1991), specific phobia QLavy, van den Hout, & Arntz, 1993), social phobia QSP, Hope, Rapee, Heimberg, & Dombeck, 1990), and posttraumatic stress disorder QFoa, Feske, Murdock, Kozak, & McCarthy, 1991; Kaspi, McNally, & Amir, 1995). Furthermore, there are several studies of patients with generalized anxiety QMartin, Williams, & Clark, 1991; Mathews & MacLeod, 1985; Mogg, Mathews, & Weinman, 1989). Most of these studies found the expected specific distraction effect, i.e., patients were specifically slowed in naming the print color of threat- related words. This effect occurs both with word-by-word and card-by-card presentation of the words, although it seems to be stronger with the blocked card- by-card format QHolle, Heimberg, & Neely, 1997; McNally, Amir, & Lipke, 1996). The color-naming interference does not seem to be due to inter-item priming, repetition of the items, or even more critically, on conscious attention. Four studies QMacLeod & Hagan, 1992; MacLeod & Rutherford, 1992; Mogg, Bradley, Williams, & Mathews, 1993; Mogg, Kentish, & Bradley, 1993) could demonstrate distraction by threatening stimuli presented subliminally. Hence, the interference seems to be a genuine bias of anxiety patients. Mathews and MacLeod Q1985) conducted one of the first studies using the modified Stroop task with anxiety patients. They found highly specific effects: patients who worried mostly about physical harm were particularly slow in naming the color of physical threat words, whereas patients worrying about social threat were especially slow in naming social threat words. Similarly, Hope et al. Q1990) reported that panic patients were slowed by physical threat cues, but not social threat cues, whereas the opposite held for social phobics. Specific, albeit slightly different, effects were also reported by Mogg et al. Q1989). Martin et al. E.S. Becker et al. / Anxiety Disorders 15 F2001) 147±159 149 Q1991), however, criticized earlier studies for confounding emotionality and threat. They presented evidence that anxiety patients were distracted by emo- tional words generally, regardless of their specific content. Thus, there are two competing explanations for the attentional biases observed in the emotional Stroop task: general emotionality Qpositive or negative) vs. schema congruency or specificity. If emotionality is responsible for the interference, then the schema theories proposing that anxious patients have increased accessibility only to threat information are wrong. However, Mathews and Klug Q1993), using threat-related negative and threat-related positive words Qantonyms of the threat-related negative ones, e.g., safe), and negative and positive words unrelated to threat, found evidence against the emotionality hypothesis: In their experiment, patients were slowed by all words related to their fears, regardless of the valence of the words. Furthermore, a study by MacLeod and Rutherford Q1992) found that specificity of effects depended on the level of processing. In their review, Williams, Mathews, and MacLeod Q1996) conclude that the Stroop effect is not due to emotionality per se, but rather to the degree to which words are semantically related to the schema. Furthermore, comparing interference indices over a variety of studies, they find that although ``current concern'' accounts for much Stroop inter- ference, it does not explain all the interference in patient groups. Besides relatedness, the negativity of the material is important in determining how much color naming will be disrupted. The present study employed the modified emotional Stroop task to further clarify the role of current concerns, emotionality, and specificity in color-naming interference. In addition to a group of non-anxious control participants, two different groups of anxiety patients participated in the studies, namely patients with generalized anxiety disorder QGAD) and patients with SP, most of whom were afraid of giving speeches. These anxiety disorders were chosen for two reasons: First, they involve anxiety schemata of differing widths, i.e., a rather broad schema QGAD) vs. a very narrow one QSP). GAD patients typically worry about a wide range of topics, while fears of SP patients in this study concentrated on giving speeches. Thus, GAD patients could be expected to have an attentional bias towards a broader range of stimuli Qmaybe even emotional stimuli in general) than SP patients. The GAD-related words chosen as stimuli were either connected to worry topics or to anxiety symptoms. The SP-related words were connected with giving a speech. 2. Method 2.1. Participants A total of 32 GAD patients, 29 SP patients, and 31 control participants without a history of any psychiatric disorder participated. GAD patients were invited to 150 E.S. Becker et al. / Anxiety Disorders 15 F2001) 147±159 participate in a drug-treatment study conducted by the fourth author at a VA Palo Alto Health Care System Hospital, Palo Alto, CA; patients with SP volunteered for a behavior therapy study at the same place. All patients completed the experiments reported here before receiving treatment. Control participants were paid a modest fee for their participation. All participants were recruited by advertisements in local newspapers. After an initial telephone screening, potential participants were given the ``Structural Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R, UpJohn Version'' QSCID-UP, Spitzer, Williams, & Gibbon, 1987) by trained interviewers. Several potential participants were excluded due to medical illness, substance abuse, and past or current psychotic episodes. Patients with both GAD and SP were excluded, as were patients with both GAD and panic disorder. Comorbidity was low in all participant groups: Four GAD patients, eight SP patients, and three control participants suffered from specific phobia, and one SP patient from panic disorder. The three groups were equated with regard to age, gender, and level of education Qsee Table 2). 2.2. Stroop cards A modified Stroop color-naming task similar to that of Mathews and MacLeod Q1985) was used. Participants were asked to name the ink color of the words on five cards. One card contained only GAD-related words, one contained only speech-related words, one neutral words, and one positive words. The words were selected
Recommended publications
  • Effect of Emotional Arousal 1 Running Head
    Effect of Emotional Arousal 1 Running Head: THE EFFECT OF EMOTIONAL AROUSAL ON RECALL The Effect of Emotional Arousal and Valence on Memory Recall 500181765 Bangor University Group 14, Thursday Afternoon Effect of Emotional Arousal 2 Abstract This study examined the effect of emotion on memory when recalling positive, negative and neutral events. Four hundred and fourteen participants aged over 18 years were asked to read stories that differed in emotional arousal and valence, and then performed a spatial distraction task before they were asked to recall the details of the stories. Afterwards, participants rated the stories on how emotional they found them, from ‘Very Negative’ to ‘Very Positive’. It was found that the emotional stories were remembered significantly better than the neutral story; however there was no significant difference in recall when a negative mood was induced versus a positive mood. Therefore this research suggests that emotional valence does not affect recall but emotional arousal affects recall to a large extent. Effect of Emotional Arousal 3 Emotional arousal has often been found to influence an individual’s recall of past events. It has been documented that highly emotional autobiographical memories tend to be remembered in better detail than neutral events in a person’s life. Structures involved in memory and emotions, the hippocampus and amygdala respectively, are joined in the limbic system within the brain. Therefore, it would seem true that emotions and memory are linked. Many studies have investigated this topic, finding that emotional arousal increases recall. For instance, Kensinger and Corkin (2003) found that individuals remember emotionally arousing words (such as swear words) more than they remember neutral words.
    [Show full text]
  • How Emotions Influence Consumers' Perception of Credibility and Trust in CSR Communication
    Department of Informatics and Media Digital Media and Society Two-Year Master’s thesis The Age of Emotionality? – How emotions influence consumers’ perception of credibility and trust in CSR communication Student: Anika Reupsch Supervisor: Göran Svensson Spring 2017 Abstract Companies around the world are using different strategies for their corporate social responsibility (CSR) communication, but finding an appropriate strategy to enhance trust and credibility on the consumer side remains challenging. The constitutive aspect of emotions in CSR communication has long been overlooked. Therefore, this study investigates the influence emotions in CSR communication have on the credibility and trust consumers have in a firm’s CSR. Quantitative research with group division was conducted. The online survey used authentic CSR communication content of a German trading company, which was classified in two categories: low emotional appeal and high emotional appeal. Statistical tests for group differences, correlation, factor analysis and multiple regression were carried out to study the influence of emotions on credibility and trust. The applied test statistics for comparing groups differences have shown a significant difference for guilt and regret, which are reduced for the group exposed to high emotional CSR content. Similarly, comprehension is enhanced, whereas sincerity is reduced for high emotional content. Above that, it was proved that positive emotions have a positive effect on perceived credibility for both text- image and moving image CSR content. However, negative emotions influence perceived credibility only for the text-image content. To the contrary, the analysis for moving image content revealed another dimension formed by interest and compassion, that positively influences perceived credibility.
    [Show full text]
  • The Intermediality of Emotion Representations of Emotionality and Fear in Youtube Vlogs and Beyond
    The Intermediality of Emotion Representations of Emotionality and Fear in YouTube Vlogs and Beyond SILKE JANDL 1. INTRODUCTION Showing emotions is a core component of successful YouTube vloggers’ ca- reers. The overt expression of emotions, i.e. emotionality that suggests to view- ers that the expression of emotion is an outburst of authentic feelings, is a pre- condition for the intricate relationships viewers develop with YouTubers. Thus, conveying genuineness in displaying emotions in vlogs is a highly effective tool. Emotionality regarded as genuine in YouTube vlogs elicits higher view counts and generates an increased number of sympathetic as well as accusatory com- ments. Appreciative as well as derogatory comments ultimately translate into financial gain; this is especially the case on channels that normally produce content that is not centred on emotional outbursts. Perceived emotional authen- ticity aids in the fostering of loyal fanbases that are keen on consuming their content as well as building communities. The communities centred around YouTube personalities are often based on the emotionality that is frequently implied in vlogs, and as David Matsumoto claims, emotional expressions are fundamental building blocks of social interaction.1 Even though bursts of emo- tion “[…] are often produced in a spontaneous and unintentional fashion, some- times defying efforts of expression control or emotion regulations, they can be produced intentionally and strategically, often with the aim of bestowing greater 1 Matsumoto 2009: 175-176. 176 | SILKE JANDL authenticity or naturalness to one’s emotional display”.2 Encouraging the notion of authenticity certainly is of key importance and can determine a YouTube vlogger’s professional success.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Psychology
    COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY PSYCH 126 Acknowledgements College of the Canyons would like to extend appreciation to the following people and organizations for allowing this textbook to be created: California Community Colleges Chancellor’s Office Chancellor Diane Van Hook Santa Clarita Community College District College of the Canyons Distance Learning Office In providing content for this textbook, the following professionals were invaluable: Mehgan Andrade, who was the major contributor and compiler of this work and Neil Walker, without whose help the book could not have been completed. Special Thank You to Trudi Radtke for editing, formatting, readability, and aesthetics. The contents of this textbook were developed under the Title V grant from the Department of Education (Award #P031S140092). However, those contents do not necessarily represent the policy of the Department of Education, and you should not assume endorsement by the Federal Government. Unless otherwise noted, the content in this textbook is licensed under CC BY 4.0 Table of Contents Psychology .................................................................................................................................................... 1 126 ................................................................................................................................................................ 1 Chapter 1 - History of Cognitive Psychology ............................................................................................. 7 Definition of Cognitive Psychology
    [Show full text]
  • Emotionality and Second Language Writers: Expressing Fear Through Narrative in Thai and in English
    L2 Journal, Volume 5 (2013), pp. 59-75 http://repositories.cdlib.org/uccllt/l2/vol5/iss1/art4/ Emotionality and Second Language Writers: Expressing Fear through Narrative in Thai and in English PISARN BEE CHAMCHARATSRI University of New Mexico E-mail: [email protected] Writing to express emotions can be a challenging task for second language (L2) writers, especially because it tends to be a process that is less addressed in language classrooms. This paper aims to expand thinking on L2 literacy and writing by exploring how L2 writers can express emotion (fear) through narratives both in their first language (L1) and second language (L2). With a small number of participants, the study reports that narrative writing can be helpful in creating venues for L2 writers to become aware of linguistic and cultural aspects of their first (Thai) and second (English) languages. By providing personally significant writing prompts, L2 writers can reflect on their personal experiences and gain understanding about themselves linguistically, culturally, and emotionally. The paper concludes with pedagogical suggestions for how writing teachers can introduce both positive and negative emotions in L2 classrooms. _______________ INTRODUCTION Where is emotion in the pedagogies of second language writing? In the work of researchers such as Hanauer (2010), Pavlenko (2005) and Kramsch (2009), there is recognition that the personal histories and emotional lives of language learners are significant for second language learning. But language teaching research tends to avoid emotionality as a result of an overreliance on the belief that language learning is solely a cognitive activity (Hanauer, 2012). In contrast to this mainstream position, Hanauer (2012) states that learning another language is “a significant, potentially life-changing, event” (p.
    [Show full text]
  • A Review of Alexithymia and Emotion Perception in Music, Odor, Taste, and Touch
    MINI REVIEW published: 30 July 2021 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.707599 Beyond Face and Voice: A Review of Alexithymia and Emotion Perception in Music, Odor, Taste, and Touch Thomas Suslow* and Anette Kersting Department of Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, University of Leipzig Medical Center, Leipzig, Germany Alexithymia is a clinically relevant personality trait characterized by deficits in recognizing and verbalizing one’s emotions. It has been shown that alexithymia is related to an impaired perception of external emotional stimuli, but previous research focused on emotion perception from faces and voices. Since sensory modalities represent rather distinct input channels it is important to know whether alexithymia also affects emotion perception in other modalities and expressive domains. The objective of our review was to summarize and systematically assess the literature on the impact of alexithymia on the perception of emotional (or hedonic) stimuli in music, odor, taste, and touch. Eleven relevant studies were identified. On the basis of the reviewed research, it can be preliminary concluded that alexithymia might be associated with deficits Edited by: in the perception of primarily negative but also positive emotions in music and a Mathias Weymar, University of Potsdam, Germany reduced perception of aversive taste. The data available on olfaction and touch are Reviewed by: inconsistent or ambiguous and do not allow to draw conclusions. Future investigations Khatereh Borhani, would benefit from a multimethod assessment of alexithymia and control of negative Shahid Beheshti University, Iran Kristen Paula Morie, affect. Multimodal research seems necessary to advance our understanding of emotion Yale University, United States perception deficits in alexithymia and clarify the contribution of modality-specific and Jan Terock, supramodal processing impairments.
    [Show full text]
  • Practice and Colour-Word Integration in Stroop Interference
    Psicológica (2015), 36, 37-67. Practice and colour-word integration in Stroop interference Amara Gul1 and Glyn W. Humphreys2 1The University of Birmingham, UK. 2Oxford University, UK. Congruency effects were examined using a manual response version of the Stroop task in which the relationship between the colour word and its hue on incongruent trials was either kept constant or varied randomly across different pairings within the stimulus set. Congruency effects were increased in the condition where the incongruent hue-word relations were maintained. This effect was observed even when only a single letter was coloured rather than the whole word. The data suggest that the Stroop interference effect can increase when words and hues become bound by learning, which can make it more difficult to ignore the word when responding to the hue. The implications for understanding Stroop interference are discussed. Stroop colour naming is one of the most widely studied paradigms for examining human selective attention (Stroop, 1935; see MacLeod, 1991, for a review). In the classic version of the test, participants are asked to name the colour of the ink in which a word is written while ignoring the word. Typically, reaction times (RTs) to name the ink colour are slower when word‘s name is incongruent (word RED in blue ink) relative to when it is congruent with the colour name (RED in red ink). In contrast, at least for naming tasks, the effects of color congruence on word naming are small (White, 1969). This interference effect from the word on naming its colour demonstrates that we have problems in attending selectively to one component of a multidimensional stimulus and ignoring a highly over 1 Acknowledgement: Preparation of this article was supported by the IUB-FDP scholarship to the first author.
    [Show full text]
  • Critical Emotionality: the Unspoken Pain of Latinx Womxn of Color Undergraduates in Higher Education Through Their Community-Engaged Service
    Critical Emotionality: The Unspoken Pain of Latinx Womxn of Color Undergraduates in Higher Education Through their Community-Engaged Service By Gema Cardona A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Education and the Designated Emphasis in Women, Gender and Sexuality in the Graduate Division of the University of California, Berkeley Committee in charge: Professor Zeus Leonardo, Chair Professor Lisa Garcia Bedolla Professor Nikki Jones Spring 2021 Abstract Critical Emotionality: The Unspoken Pain of Latinx Womxn of Color Undergraduates in Higher Education Through their Community-Engaged Service by Gema Cardona Doctor of Philosophy in Education University of California, Berkeley Professor Zeus Leonardo, Chair Research suggests that a college degree leads to greater civic engagement. However, less is known about the ways that civic engagement while enrolled at an institution of higher education influences college navigation, particularly for self-identified Latinx womxn of color undergraduates. Yet, this body of literature tends to emphasize the cultural and social benefits of the college student. That is, how community engagement is transformative for the individual student, rather than placing a greater focus on the communities they engage with through the service-learning program. Thereby the question guiding this investigation is as follows: How do Latinx womxn of color undergraduates make meaning of community engagement in various forms of both institutionalized and non-institutionalized forms of service learning they participate in, both as experienced and conceptually? To answer this question, I employ a testimonio and pláticas methodology to collect and analyze the collective understanding of intersectional forms of oppression experienced directly in the communities as witnessed by the Latinx womxn.
    [Show full text]
  • Perception Without Awareness: Further Evidence from a Stroop Priming Task
    Perception & Psychophysics 2002, 64 (8), 1316–1324 Perception without awareness: Further evidence from a Stroop priming task M. TERESA DAZA and JUAN J. ORTELLS University of Almería, Almería, Spain and ELAINE FOX University of Essex, Essex, England In the present research,we examined the influence of prime–targetstimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) on Stroop-priming effects from masked words. Participants indicated the color of a central target, which was preceded by a 33-msec prime word followed either immediately or after a variable delay by a pattern mask. The prime word was incongruent or congruent with the target color on 75% and 25% of the trials, respectively. The words followed by an immediate mask produced reliable Stroop interfer- ence at SOAs of 300 and 400 msec but not at SOAs of 500 and 700 msec. The words followed by a de- layedmask produced a reversed(i.e., facilitatory)Stroop effect, which reached significance at an SOA of 400 msec or longer, but never at the shorter 300-msec SOA. Such an differential time course of both types of Stroop priming effects provides further evidence for the existence of qualitative differences between conscious and nonconscious perceptual processes. The question of whether perception can occur without scious experience is indexed by some behavioral measure awareness has been the focus of considerableresearch and that demonstrates an observer’s inability to discriminate discussionfor many years (for recent reviews, see Merikle between alternative stimulus states (see, e.g., Balota, & Daneman, 1998; Merikle, Smilek, & Eastwood, 2001). 1983; Fowler, Wolford, Slade, & Tassinary, 1981; Green- The logic followed by the majority of studies within ex- wald, Draine, & Abrams, 1996; Kunst-Wilson & Zajonc, perimental psychologyhas been to demonstrate a dissoci- 1980; Marcel, 1983a, 1983b).
    [Show full text]
  • Does the Modified Stroop Effect Exist in PTSD?
    Journal of Anxiety Disorders 23 (2009) 650–655 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Anxiety Disorders Does the modified Stroop effect exist in PTSD? Evidence from dissertation abstracts and the peer reviewed literature Matthew O. Kimble a,*, B. Christopher Frueh b, Libby Marks a a Department of Psychology, Middlebury College, Middlebury, VT 05753, United States b Department of Psychology, University of Hawai‘i at Hilo, Hilo, HI, United States ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Article history: The modified Stroop effect (MSE), in which participants show delayed colour naming to trauma-specific Received 26 June 2008 words, is one of the most widely cited findings in the literature pertaining to cognitive bias in Received in revised form 28 January 2009 posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). The current study used a novel approach (Dissertation Abstract Accepted 2 February 2009 Review; DAR) to review the presence of the MSE in dissertation abstracts. A review of dissertations that used the modified Stroop task in a PTSD sample revealed that only 8% of the studies found delayed Keywords: reaction times to trauma-specific words in participants with PTSD. The most common finding (75%) was Modified Stroop for no PTSD-specific effects in colour naming trauma-relevant words. This ratio is significantly lower Attentional bias than ratios found in the peer reviewed literature, but even in the peer reviewed literature only 44% of PTSD Trauma controlled studies found the modified Stroop effect. These data suggest that a reevaluation of the MSE in Dissertation Abstract Review PTSD is warranted. ß 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Modified versions of the original Stroop (1935) task have been depressive, anxiety, and eating disorders (Dagleish & Power, widely used to demonstrate biases in the processing of information 2004; MacLeod, 2005).
    [Show full text]
  • The Cultural Dimension of Emotional Intelligence and Its Clinical Implications
    Eastern Illinois University The Keep Masters Theses Student Theses & Publications 1-1-2008 The ulturC al Dimension Of Emotional Intelligence And Its Clinical Implications Koyeli Sengupta Eastern Illinois University This research is a product of the graduate program in Psychology at Eastern Illinois University. Find out more about the program. Recommended Citation Sengupta, Koyeli, "The ulturC al Dimension Of Emotional Intelligence And Its Clinical Implications" (2008). Masters Theses. 601. http://thekeep.eiu.edu/theses/601 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Theses & Publications at The Keep. It has been accepted for inclusion in Masters Theses by an authorized administrator of The Keep. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Page 1 of 1 THESIS REPRODUCTION CERTIFICATE .TO: Graduate Degree Candidates (who have written formal theses) SUBJECT: Permission to Reproduce Theses The University Library is receiving a number of request from other institutions asking permission to reproduce dissertations for inclusion in their library holdings. Although no copyright laws are involved, we feel that professional courtesy demands that permission be obtained from the author before we allow these to be copied. • PLEASE SIGN ONE OF THE FOLLOWING STATEMENTS: Booth Library of Eastern Illinois University has my permission to lend my thesis to a reputable college or .. university for the purpose of copying it for inclusion in that institution's library or research holdings. o 7/14 /20D:F ~ I 7 Date .0:1 respectfully request Booth Library of Eastern Illinois University NOT allow my thesis to be reproduced because: ,.,.~. Author's Signature Date This fonn must be submitted in duplicate.
    [Show full text]
  • Paying Attention to the Stroop Effect?
    Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society (1996), 2,467-470. Copyright © 1996 INS. Published by Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. LETTER TO THE EDITOR Paying attention to the Stroop effect? AVISHAI HENIK Department of Behavioral Sciences, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva, Israel (RECEIVED November 15, 1995; REVISED January 8, 1996; ACCEPTED February 21, 1996) Studies of attention commonly use conflict situations, such (RCNd). On average, subjects were 2.3 s slower to read color as the Stroop task (Stroop, 1935), in which subjects are asked names in the incongruent condition than when the same to attend to an object or a dimension and simultaneously names were printed in black. Stroop concluded that this ef- ignore other objects or dimensions. Because of its reliabil- fect was not reliable, and further experiments have repli- ity and robustness, the Stroop task is included in many neuro- cated this finding (MacLeod, 1991). That is, colors do not psychology batteries for investigating attentional processing reliably interfere with reading of words. in a variety of populations. Unfortunately, despite its wide The second experiment measured interference of words application, the task is sometimes misapplied because some on naming colors. This experiment was also composed of studies are not motivated by a conceptual framework, but two conditions: (1) squares of colors (e.g., a square of red only by empirical diagnostic criteria. The data resulting from color), and (2) an incongruent condition similar to that men- some studies are either uninterpretable or, worse, mislead- tioned for the first experiment. Subjects were asked to name ing.
    [Show full text]