Coolidge Effect in Pond Snails: Male Motivation in a Simultaneous Hermaphrodite Jorismkoene*1 and Andries Ter Maat1,2

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Coolidge Effect in Pond Snails: Male Motivation in a Simultaneous Hermaphrodite Jorismkoene*1 and Andries Ter Maat1,2 BMC Evolutionary Biology BioMed Central Research article Open Access Coolidge effect in pond snails: male motivation in a simultaneous hermaphrodite JorisMKoene*1 and Andries Ter Maat1,2 Address: 1Department of Animal Ecology, Faculty of Earth & Life Sciences, Vrije Universiteit, De Boelelaan 1085, 1081 HV Amsterdam, The Netherlands and 2Max-Planck-Institut für Ornithologie, Postfach 1564, 82305 Starnberg, Germany Email: Joris M Koene* - [email protected]; Andries Ter Maat - [email protected] * Corresponding author Published: 6 November 2007 Received: 1 September 2006 Accepted: 6 November 2007 BMC Evolutionary Biology 2007, 7:212 doi:10.1186/1471-2148-7-212 This article is available from: http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2148/7/212 © 2007 Koene and Ter Maat; licensee BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. Abstract Background: The simultaneously hermaphroditic pond snail, Lymnaea stagnalis, can mate in the male and female role, but within one copulation only one sexual role is performed at a time. Previous work has shown that male motivation is determined by the availability of seminal fluid in the prostate gland, which is detected via a nervous connection by the brain area controlling male behaviour. Based on this knowledge, patterns of sexual role alternations within mating pairs can be explained. Results: The data presented here reveal that these snails can donate and receive sperm several times within 24 hours, and that they have increased mating rates in larger groups (i.e. more mating opportunities). For mating pairs we show, by introducing novel mating partners after copulation, that animals do inseminate new partners, while they are no longer motivated to inseminate their original partners. Conclusion: Our findings provide the first direct evidence for higher motivation in a hermaphrodite to copulate when a new partner is encountered. This Coolidge effect seems to be attenuated when mucus trails are excluded, which suggests that a chemical or textural cue may be responsible for mediating this response to sperm competition. Background non of preferentially inseminating a novel partner was Males of many species distribute their sperm over several first demonstrated experimentally in rats [6], and is gener- mates, and often do this in a way that is theoretically pre- ally assumed to be widespread among promiscuous verte- dicted to maximize their lifetime reproductive success brates [4,5,7]. The Coolidge effect is based on familiarity [e.g., [1,2]]. Since the seminal paper by Dewsbury [3], it is with a mating partner, and is often assumed to require the now generally accepted that sperm and seminal fluids are presence of an individual recognition mechanism. How- costly to produce and that males are selected to strategi- ever, individual recognition is not included in the defini- cally allocate their sperm [4]. One behavioural strategy to tion and has, to our knowledge, actually never been achieve the latter is via rekindled sexual motivation upon shown to lay at the basis of this phenomenon. Nonethe- encounter of a new partner, which has been dubbed the less, the Coolidge effect is usually thought to require cog- Coolidge effect by Whalen in 1959 (after an anecdote nitive brain areas involved in learning and memory that about President and Mrs. Coolidge) [5]. This phenome- can integrate the information from different neural sys- Page 1 of 6 (page number not for citation purposes) BMC Evolutionary Biology 2007, 7:212 http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2148/7/212 tems involved in individual discrimination, novelty detec- opening is accessible. This is followed by probing with the tion and memory maintenance, and motivation [e.g., everted preputium (penis carrying organ) and, once the [7,8]]. female gonopore is found, penis intromission and trans- fer of a copious amount of semen [24]. That the male Darwin [9] stated about molluscs, and other species reproductive investment of this simultaneous hermaphro- belonging to the "lower classes", that "it is almost certain dite equals the energetic costs of female reproduction was that these animals have too imperfect senses and much demonstrated by experimentally eliminating male behav- too low mental powers, to appreciate each other's beauty iour, which resulted in a doubling of egg production [27]. or other attractions, or to feel rivalry". However, it is now The above illustrates that these snails are expected to be clear that sexual selection and sperm competition are prudent with their expensive male reserves. It would be widespread among invertebrates [e.g., [10]]. Here, we prudent to use the sperm to inseminate different partners show that the Coolidge effect is also not restricted to ver- rather than the same partner several times, which we set tebrates, because we find that this motivational strategy is out to test here. used by a hermaphroditic invertebrate. Results There is now abundant evidence that insects are prudent The mean number of inseminations per individual with their expensive sperm and seminal fluids, despite observed during 24 hour observations of Pairs, Quartets, their relatively high mating rates. Gage [11,12] was the and Octets are shown in Figure 1. The overall difference first to show that for the medfly, Ceratitis capitata, the pres- between the groups was significant (Nested ANOVA: F2,18 ence of sperm competition results in the transfer of more = 3.88, p = 0.027). Post-hoc testing showed that Octets sperm per ejaculate. Subsequent studies further focused had a higher insemination frequency than Pairs (Tukey on sperm numbers as a response to the risk or intensity of HSD, p < 0.05), while the Quartets' frequency was in sperm competition [e.g., [13-18]]. Interestingly, we have between but different from neither. only been able to find one insect study, on Tribolium cas- taneum, that specifically looked at whether males prefer to In pairs of snails we subsequently tested experimentally inseminate novel females over familiar females, which whether the observed increase in insemination frequency they had already inseminated [19]. Although that study in larger groups was due to the presence of new partners. provided evidence for a Coolidge effect this connection We used a mixed model (nominal logistic REML proce- was somehow not made at the time, possibly because of dure) to test for the fixed effects of treatment (New/Same the lack of knowledge about the underlying motivational partner) and test (Dirty/Clean aquarium) and the random mechanism. Another study, on Gryllus bimaculatus, effect of replicates (nested in treatment) on whether showed that females preferred to mate with a novel male, insemination occurred. Prior to the treatment, i.e. during indicating that the Coolidge effect is not limited to males the first 6 hours of the experiment, on average 39.25 (± [20]. The great pond snail, L. stagnalis, is a simultaneous her- maphrodite that can mate in the male and female role, 3 but within a single copulation one sexual role is per- b formed (and roles can be swapped afterwards) [21]. These ab snails seem usually prepared to receive sperm [21], they 2 a can receive sperm multiple times within a breeding season [22], and they can store and use the received sperm from different partners [23]. However, they will only donate per individual 1 sperm after several days of sexual isolation [24]. Previous studies have demonstrated that this male motivation is Mean no. of inseminations determined by the amount of seminal fluid in the prostate 0 Pairs Quartets Octets gland [25]. The gland's volume increase is detected, via a nervous connection, by an evolutionarily conserved brain 24andFigure hour 8 snails 1observations of copulation activity in groups of 2, 4 region controlling male behaviour in gastropod molluscs 24 hour observations of copulation activity in groups of 2, 4 [26]. and 8 snails. The mean number of inseminations per individ- ual are shown, the error bars represent standard errors of L. stagnalis' male behaviour comprises a fixed sequence of the mean of the replicates. For clarity the replicates are events. These events include shell mounting and circular pooled. Groups with different letters (a or b) differ signifi- locomotion over the partner's shell to eventually find the cantly from each other. position on the edge of the shell from where the female Page 2 of 6 (page number not for citation purposes) BMC Evolutionary Biology 2007, 7:212 http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2148/7/212 1.71 S.D., N = 4 replicates) of the 48 animals inseminated A: Dirty their partner. For this observation period no differences 50 were found in the number of inseminations (treatment: 2 2 χ < 0.00, df = 1, p = 1.00; test: χ = 0.31, df = 1, p = 0.57; 40 interaction: χ2 = 0.07, df = 2, p = 0.96). In all cases where insemination took place after the treatment, i.e. during 30 the second 6 hours, animals inseminated their partners only once. Significantly more new partners were insemi- nated than familiar partners (χ2 = 6.14, df = 1, p = 0.013). inseminations 20 In a clean aquarium more copulations were observed than nd 2 10 in an uncleaned aquarium (χ = 3.88, df = 1, p = 0.049), % 2 indicating that the presence of mucus is important for this 0 effect. The interaction between the two factors was also 0 significant (χ2 = 6.84, df = 2, p = 0.033), suggesting that the Same partner New partner effect of a new partner depended on the factor Clean/ Dirty. The percentage of animals performing an insemina- tion during the second 6 hours is shown separately for the two replicates in the Dirty (Figure 2A) and the two repli- B: Clean cates in the Clean aquaria (Figure 2B).
Recommended publications
  • 1. SCIENCE in CARTOONS. in the Cartoon Futurama, the Crustacean Dr
    Bio 312, Fall 2018 Exam 2 ( 1 ) Name:____________________ Please write the first letter of your last name in the box; 5 points will be deducted if your name is hard to read or the box does not contain the correct letter. Written answers should be concise and precise; answers typically have short correct answers. Regrade requests cannot be made for exams completed in pencil for any reason. The exam has 100 points total 1. SCIENCE IN CARTOONS. In the cartoon Futurama, the crustacean Dr. Zoidberg travels back to his home planet, Decapod 10, to participate in their mating ritual. The males of his species construct mounds of sand in a line along the beach and decorate them with shells and seaweed. They also adorn themselves. They then perform a dance to attract females, who tour the beach and watch the males dance. Here he is shown with a potential mate evaluating his performance. (a, 4 pts) Name the specific category of sexual selection Dr. Zoidberg’s behavior represents (b, 4 pts) Describe (don't just name the species) the example from class that is most similar to this scenario. (c, 2 pts) Is this typically considered an example of intrasexual or intersexual selection? (circle one) intrasexual intersexual (d, 4 pts) Later in the episode, we find out that Dr. Zoidberg failed to attract a mate. When his friends attempt to comfort him by mentioning that he can try again next year, Dr. Zoidberg explains that his species mates once, and then dies. What is the technical term for this life history strategy? List the technical term and its opposite.
    [Show full text]
  • Pair Bonding: What Mediates Its Formation and Maintenance?
    Pair bonding: what mediates its formation and maintenance? by Amanda M. Dios A Dissertation submitted to Graduate School-Newark Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Psychology written under the direction of Mei-Fang Cheng and approved by ___________________________________ Mei-Fang Cheng ___________________________________ Michael Shifflet ___________________________________ Michael Stephen J. Hanson ___________________________________Shifflet Mauricio Delgado ___________________________________ Mark Hauber Newark, NJ May 2015 Copyright page: © [2015] Amanda Dios ALL RIGHTS RESERVED ABSTRACT OF THE DISSERTATION Pair bonding: what mediates its formation and maintenance? BY: AMANDA DIOS Dissertation Director: Mei Fang Cheng Pair bonding is an exclusive mating relationship associating the memory of a mate with the potential successful completion of a breeding cycle. In evolutionary biology, pair bonding has been studied as a mating strategy and this biological phenomenon has been associated with survival related reproductive behaviors, however, behaviors that function in the formation of bonding and those that are established as a result of bonding are seldom distinguished, making it difficult to assess how pair bonding is represented in the brain. This thesis assesses pair bonding in ring doves, an animal that has a predictable sequence leading to a successful breeding cycle. Through a series of lesion, immunohistochemistry, and behavior studies we sought to understand the processing and execution of this fitness-critical complex behavior, by assessing the neural basis of pair bonding, understanding which behavioral events initiate the formation of the bond and how bonding affects an animal's decisions involving a mate. We found a neuro- marker for pair bonding that is more accurate than current methods of measuring bonding.
    [Show full text]
  • Precopulatory Sexual Selection in the Seed Bug Lygaeus Equestris:A Comparison of Choice and No-Choice Paradigms
    Animal Behaviour 89 (2014) 207e214 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Animal Behaviour journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/anbehav Precopulatory sexual selection in the seed bug Lygaeus equestris:a comparison of choice and no-choice paradigms Liam R. Dougherty*, David M. Shuker School of Biology, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, U.K. article info Mate choice has long been appreciated as a key component of sexual selection. However, how we fi Article history: measure mate choice, both in the eld and in the laboratory, remains problematic. Mating preferences Received 3 August 2013 may be tested using either no-choice or choice tests, but explicit comparisons between these two Initial acceptance 6 September 2013 experimental paradigms remain limited. It has been suggested that preferences may be stronger in Final acceptance 17 December 2013 choice tests as they allow simultaneous comparison, and some studies have indeed found stronger Published online mating preferences in choice tests compared to no-choice tests. Here we explicitly tested the effect of MS. number: 13-00648R experimental choice paradigm on the measurement of sexual selection on male and female morphology in the promiscuous seed bug Lygaeus equestris (Heteroptera, Lygaeidae). We performed mating trials in Keywords: which we varied the amount of choice presented to each sex, giving four choice treatments: no-choice, choice paradigm male choice, female choice and mutual choice. Overall we found evidence for significant positive experimental design directional selection on female body length and stabilizing selection on an overall measure of male body Lygaeidae fi mate choice size. However, we found no signi cant effect of choice paradigm on the patterns of sexual selection for no-choice males or females.
    [Show full text]
  • Intersexual Communication, Male Mate Preference, and Reproductive Energetics of the Polygynous Lizard, Anolis Carolinensis
    INTERSEXUAL COMMUNICATION, MALE MATE PREFERENCE, AND REPRODUCTIVE ENERGETICS OF THE POLYGYNOUS LIZARD, ANOLIS CAROLINENSIS Kimberly S. Orrell Dissertation submitted to the Faculty of the Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Biology Thomas A. Jenssen, Chair Justin D. Congdon Jack Cranford Eric P. Smith Jeffrey Walters June 2002 Blacksburg, Virginia Keywords: behavior, cognition, communication, Coolidge effect, courtship, display behavior, female mimicry, energetics, individual recognition, mate choice, metabolism, reproduction, sexual selection. Copyright 2002, Kimberly S. Orrell MALE MATE PREFERENCE, INTERSEXUAL COMMUNICATION, AND REPRODUCTIVE ENERGETICS OF THE POLYGYNOUS LIZARD, ANOLIS CAROLINENSIS Kimberly S. Orrell ABSTRACT Particularly lacking in the current body of sexual selection literature are studies based on reptile species and intrasexually selected mating systems. Because the life history traits and ecology of reptiles are dramatically different from other animal taxa, current models of sexual selection are insufficient for predicting how sexual selection should influence the behavior and mating systems of lizards. Similarly, intersexually selected mating systems (i.e., based on female choice) are inappropriate predictive models for examining species with intrasexually selected mating systems (i.e., based on consexual contests). I investigated three aspects of Anolis carolinensis behavior and mating system (communication signals, male mate preference, and reproductive energetics) to contribute to a theoretical model for sexual selection based on a lizard with an intrasexually selected, polygynous mating system. In my first study, I quantified the structure and use of signals exchanged by both sexes, compared signal structure and use during heterosexual interactions to that of other social contexts (e.g., male-alone, male-male, female-female), then related signal structure and use to the species mating system.
    [Show full text]
  • The Coolidge Effect, Individual Recognition and Selection For
    ARTICLE IN PRESS Proc. R. Soc. B 1 doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.0375 65 2 66 3 67 4 68 5 The Coolidge effect, individual recognition 69 6 70 7 and selection for distinctive cuticular signatures 71 8 72 9 in a burying beetle 73 10 1,* 1,† 1,2 ¨ 1 74 11 Sandra Steiger , Ragna Franz , Anne-Katrin Eggert and Josef K. Muller 75 12 1Department of Evolutionary Biology and Ecology, Faculty of Biology, University of Freiburg, 76 13 Hauptstrasse 1, 79104 Freiburg, Germany 77 14 2Behavior, Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics (BEES) Section, Department of Biological Sciences, 78 15 Illinois State University, Normal, IL 61790-4120, USA 79 16 80 The ability to recognize individuals is an important aspect of social interactions, but it can also be useful to 17 81 avoid repeated matings with the same individual. The Coolidge effect is the progressive decline in a male’s 18 82 propensity to mate with the same female combined with a heightened sexual interest in new females. 19 83 Although males that recognize previous partners and show a preference for novel females should have a 20 84 selective advantage as they can distribute sperm evenly among the females they encounter, there are few 21 85 invertebrate examples of the Coolidge effect. Here we present evidence for this effect in the burying beetle 22 86 Nicrophorus vespilloides and examine the mechanism underlying the discrimination between familiar and 23 87 novel mates. Burying beetles feed and reproduce on vertebrate carcasses, where they regularly encounter 24 88 conspecifics. Males showed greater sexual interest in novel females (virgin or mated) than in females they 25 89 had inseminated before.
    [Show full text]
  • Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2021 J
    T The Coolidge Effect reproductive tract. Another proposed adaptive benefit of the Coolidge effect is that it allows Gavin Vance1 and Todd K. Shackelford2 males to copulate with a new female more quickly 1Oakland University, Rochester, MI, USA after finishing copulation with a previous female, 2Department of Psychology, Oakland University, as the male does not risk displacing his own Rochester, MI, USA semen from the new female. The Coolidge effect has been observed in a number of species. The first instance of this effect Synonyms was observed in rats (Beach and Jordan 1956) and has been confirmed in more recent research which Male sexual refractory period; Parental invest- showed that, in addition to a shortened refractory ment theory; Sperm competition period, male rats presented with novel sexual stimuli showed increased dopaminergic activity in the nucleus accumbens (Fiorino et al. 1997). Definition This effect has also been observed in non- mammalian species such as fish (Jordan and A phenomenon occurring in the males of some Brooks 2010), invertebrates (Steiger et al. 2008), sexually reproducing species wherein post- and hermaphroditic pond snails adopting the male copulatory sexual arousal may be reignited if the role during copulation (Koene and Ter Maat male is presented with new sexual partners. 2007). The Coolidge effect is a phenomenon observed The Coolidge effect may also extend to differ- in some sexually reproducing animals in which ential sperm allocation. Research has found evi- sexual arousal is reignited by novel sexual stimuli. dence that males of some species, such as The term was first coined by Frank A. Beach European bitterlings, will allocate sperm differen- (Beach and Jordan 1956) and refers to a story tially, such that males will produce higher-quality involving US President Calvin Coolidge and ejaculates during copulation with novel females his wife.
    [Show full text]
  • Frontiers in Zoology Biomed Central
    Frontiers in Zoology BioMed Central Research Open Access No effect of mate novelty on sexual motivation in the freshwater snail Biomphalaria glabrata Ines K Häderer†, Johanna Werminghausen†, Nico K Michiels, Nadine Timmermeyer and Nils Anthes*† Address: Animal Evolutionary Ecology, Institute for Evolution and Ecology, Eberhard Karls-Universität Tübingen, Auf der Morgenstelle 28, 72076 Tübingen, Germany Email: Ines K Häderer - [email protected]; Johanna Werminghausen - [email protected]; Nico K Michiels - [email protected]; Nadine Timmermeyer - [email protected]; Nils Anthes* - [email protected] * Corresponding author †Equal contributors Published: 12 October 2009 Received: 16 July 2009 Accepted: 12 October 2009 Frontiers in Zoology 2009, 6:23 doi:10.1186/1742-9994-6-23 This article is available from: http://www.frontiersinzoology.com/content/6/1/23 © 2009 Häderer et al; licensee BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. Abstract Background: When mating effort (e.g. via ejaculates) is high, males are expected to strategically allocate their resources depending on the expected fitness gains from a given mating opportunity. One mechanism to achieve strategic mating is the Coolidge effect, where male sexual motivation declines across repeated encounters with a familiar partner, but resuscitates when encountering a novel female. Experimental tests of male mate choice via mechanisms such as the Coolidge effect, however, remain scarce. Moreover, it is untested to date whether the Coolidge effect occurs in a sex-specific manner in simultaneous hermaphrodites, where the motivation to mate with a familiar partner may vary with previous mating activity in the male or female role.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolutionary Pyschology 5(2): 275-288
    Evolutionary Psychology www.epjournal.net – 2007. 5(2): 275-288 ¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯ Original Article A Common, Conceptual Framework for Behavioral Ecology and Evolutionary Psychology Donald W. White, Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC, Canada. Lawrence M. Dill, Behavioural Ecology Research Group, Department of Biological Sciences, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC, Canada. E-Mail: [email protected]. (Corresponding author) Charles B. Crawford, Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC, Canada. Abstract: Since evolutionary psychology and behavioral ecology have much in common despite their using different objects for their study, one might expect these disciplines to share a common conceptual framework with associated definitions. Unfortunately, such agreement does not entirely exist. To address the problem, we propose a common, conceptual framework, the Adaptive Behavioral System (ABS), which organizes behavior within an evolutionary framework around an organism’s life history tasks. An ABS includes strategies that use decision rules and employs tactics administered by a hypothesized construct, the Evolved Processing Unit (EPU). The ABS also includes observed or predicted behavior which can be tested experimentally – the ultimate test of construct validity. Use of the proposed framework should help the two disciplines focus on their common, core business of behavior and, ultimately, be to the benefit of both. Keywords: Strategy, Tactics, Decision Rule, Adaptive Behavioral System, Fitness ¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯ Introduction Evolutionary psychology and behavioral ecology share many things – despite their choosing humans or non-humans respectively as the objects for their study (Hutchinson and Gigerenzer, 2005). Both accept the principles that organisms obtain information from their environment and, on the basis of that information and within the limits imposed by their genotypes and other constraints, produce a response (Giske et al., 2003).
    [Show full text]
  • Calvin Coolidge a Tale of Two Coolidges
    Calvin Coolidge A tale of two Coolidges EPISODE TRANSCRIPT Listen to Presidential at http://wapo.st/presidential This transcript was run through an automated transcription service and then lightly edited for clarity. There may be typos or small discrepancies from the podcast audio. LILLIAN CUNNINGHAM: Calvin Coolidge was born on the Fourth of July, 1872, in Plymouth Notch, Vermont. He died in 1933, just a few years after leaving the presidency. And he's buried on top of a hill in his small hometown near the Green Mountains. His body rests silently beside many others in his family, including his 16-year-old son who died in the White House. WILLIAM JENNEY: This is William Jenney, the regional administrator with the Vermont Division for Historic Preservation. And we are standing in front of President Coolidge's gravesite here at the Plymouth Notch cemetery. It is a very simple grave site. It's on a steep hill. This land was too steep to farm, so they put it to another practical use. Notice on the gravestone itself, it says his name with his birth and death dates, but nothing else to indicate anything special other than the presidential seal at the top. Some people are surprised to see such a simple gravesite for a president. But it's very appropriate for him. He did say in his autobiography that, 'We draw our presidents from the people. I came from them. I wished to be one of them again.' LILLIAN CUNNINGHAM: For this episode, I have a special tour guide. So, with me in the studio is Steve Pearlstein.
    [Show full text]
  • Strategic Sperm Allocation and a Coolidge Effect in an Externally Fertilizing Species
    Behavioral Ecology doi:10.1093/beheco/ars138 Advance Access publication 17 September 2012 Original Article Strategic sperm allocation and a Coolidge effect in an externally fertilizing species Rowena Spence,a Martin Reichard,b and Carl Smitha a b School of Biology, University of St. Andrews, St. Andrews, Fife, KY16 8LB, UK and Institute of Downloaded from Vertebrate Biology, Academy of Sciences of the Czech Republic, Květná 8, 603 65 Brno, Czech Republic Because sperm is costly and limiting, males are predicted to allocate sperm differentially across matings, according to the level http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/ of sperm competition, female reproductive quality, and female novelty. We investigated sperm allocation in the European bitter- ling (Rhodeus amarus), an externally fertilizing species of fish that spawns and incubates its eggs in the gills of freshwater mussels. We predicted that males would allocate sperm differentially according to the quality and novelty of mussels. Dominant males responded to rivals by increasing both sperm investment and aggression, whereas subordinates responded chiefly through sperm investment. Dominant males invested more sperm in novel mussels in accordance with predictions for a Coolidge effect, the mussel representing a new fertilization opportunity. However, subordinate males were not influenced by mussel novelty but were responsive to order of mating. Males did not allocate sperm according to mussel quality, suggesting that certainty of pater- nity is a more important variable than offspring survival in shaping bitterling sperm allocation strategies. This study demon- strates strategic ejaculate expenditure in an externally fertilizing species, analogous to that in internal fertilizers, but with sperm investment operating on the level of the site of fertilization rather than the female.
    [Show full text]
  • Odor Communication and Mate Choice in Rodents
    biology Review Odor Communication and Mate Choice in Rodents Michael H. Ferkin Department of Biological Sciences, University of Memphis, Memphis, TN 38017, USA; [email protected] Received: 14 December 2017; Accepted: 22 January 2018; Published: 25 January 2018 Abstract: This paper details how chemical communication is affected by ecological challenges such as finding mates. I list several conditions that affect the decision to attract mates, the decision to respond to the signals of potential mates and how the response depends on context. These mate-choice decisions and their outcomes will depend on the life history constraints placed on individuals such as their fecundity, sex, lifespan, opportunities to mate in the future and age at senescence. Consequently, the sender’s decision to scent mark or self-groom as well as the receiver’s choice of response represents a tradeoff between the current costs of the participant’s own survival and future reproduction against that of reproducing now. The decision to scent mark and the response to the scent mark of opposite-sex conspecifics should maximize the fitness of the participants in that context. Keywords: life history strategies; mate choice; odor communication; over-marks; scent marks; rodents 1. Introduction and Overview Identification of suitable mates is probably a universal need for all species that reproduce sexually. Chemical signals often provide crucial information about the sex and reproductive condition of conspecifics. In many species of terrestrial mammals, individuals are more attracted to odors of sexually receptive, opposite-sex conspecific than those of same-sex conspecifics [1,2]. Often, males are not attracted to females that are not sexually receptive [3,4].
    [Show full text]
  • Comparative Psychology (PSYO 6160)
    Topics in Behavioural Biology (NESC and PSYO 4160) & Comparative Psychology (PSYO 6160) Winter term 2021 Lectures: Online, synchronous, using Microsoft Teams, Tuesdays, 14:30 to 16:30 (Halifax time). Instructor: Instructor Office # Office hours email† Phone # Dr. Simon LSC by appointment: Zoom or [email protected] 902–494– Gadbois 3326 Teams 8848 † e-mail communications: When sending an email to Dr. Gadbois please add “4160” or "6160” to the subject line. Course Description and Course Prerequisites For undergraduate students: PSYO/NESC 4160 Topics in Behavioural Biology CREDIT HOURS: 3 SIGNATURE REQUIRED CROSSLISTED: NESC 4160.03 and PSYO 4160.03 FORMATS: Seminar For graduate students: PSYO 6160 Comparative psychology CREDIT HOURS: 3 FORMAT: Seminar Course Objectives/Learning Outcomes The seminar will focus on topics and research in fundamental and applied animal behaviour relating mostly (but not exclusively) to conceptual breakthroughs and new trends in both behavioural biology (sensory ecology, cognitive ecology/ethology, neuroecology/neuroethology, socioecology, etc.) and comparative psychology (animal psychophysics, animal cognition with the exception of animal learning, etc.). For students (most likely undergraduate students) that took “Advanced Animal Behaviour” (PSYO/NESC 3162) with Dr. Gadbois, you can consider this class a natural follow-up to that course, but it also goes beyond the focus of 3162 (social behaviour and communication). The topics will flexible and adapted to the interests of the class. Topics will be chosen by the students from the following list: 1. The science of animal domestication: Physiological, behavioural and psychological changes. 2. Biological clocks: Circannual patterns and/or Zeitgebers 3. Eusociality (in mammals, insects, etc.) 4.
    [Show full text]