Decomposing the Influence of Mental Processes on Academic Performance

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Decomposing the Influence of Mental Processes on Academic Performance Intelligence 77 (2019) 101404 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Intelligence journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/intell Decomposing the influence of mental processes on academic performance T ⁎ Andreas Demetrioua, , Nikolaos Makrisb, Dimitrios Tachmatzidisb, Smaragda Kazic, George Spanoudisd a University of Nicosia, and Cyprus Academy of Science, Letters, and Arts, Makedonitissas Avenue, P.O. Box 24005, 1700 Nicosia, Cyprus b Democritus University of Thrace, Greece c Panteion University of Social and Political Sciences, Greece d University of Cyprus, Cyprus ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Keywords: We explored relations between academic performance and cognition from 9 to 15 years of age. Participants were Academic performance examined on executive control processes, reasoning in several domains, self-evaluation of performance in these Cognition domains, and language. Structural equation modelling showed that cognitive and language ability highly pre- Language dicted school performance. These relations changed with age; cognitive flexibility, working memory and rea- Cognitive development soning dominated from 9 to 11 and reasoning and language dominated from 13 to 15 years. Self-evaluation was Intelligence related with academic performance only in secondary school, but this relation was masked by reasoning. SES influenced school achievement directly on top of cognitive influences in both primary and secondary school.The implications for cognitive developmental theory and educational implications are discussed. 1. Introduction instance, numerical operations and the mental number line in mathe- matics, mental rotation and mental imagery in spatial reasoning, This study examined how performance at school relates to various sorting and class reasoning in classification, etc. Although the exact dimensions of cognition. We decomposed this relation into specific number, identity, and degree of functional autonomy of the domains processes involved in cognition, such as executive control, reasoning, are still disputed, some domains are recognized across disciplines of language and cognitive self-evaluation, and specified if they relate to psychological research. For instance, spatial, categorical, quantitative, school performance differently at different levels of school education. causal, social, and verbal reasoning emerged as distinct domains in Below we first summarize current research about the organization and differential, cognitive, developmental, and educational psychology development of these mental processes. We then summarize research (Carroll, 1993; Case, 1992; Case, Demetriou, Platsidou, Kazi, 2001; showing how these processes relate to academic achievement. Finally, Demetriou & Spanoudis, 2018; Gardner, 1983; Thurstone, 1973). we state predictions to be tested by our study. At still a higher level, all domains relate to a higher-order factor, general intelligence or g, reflecting the fact that all mental processes 1.1. Intelligence correlate with each other. Although widely accepted, the nature of g is still under strong dispute. Through the years, it has been associated 1.1.1. Organization with three types of domain-independent processes. First is reasoning in The hierarchical interpretation of mental processes dominates in its various manifestations, including inductive, analogical, and deduc- psychometric (Carroll, 1993) and brain models (Haier, 2017) of the tive reasoning (Carroll, 1993; Jensen, 1998; Spearman, 1927). In cur- human mind. According to this interpretation, mental abilities are or- rent psychometric theory, this factor is basically identical with fluid ganized in three major hierarchical levels. At the task level, there are intelligence (gf) (Gustafsson & Undheim, 1996). Later, several processes specific processes related to specific tasks, such as addition inmathe- which reflect efficiency in representing and processing information matics, visualization in space, classifying objects, etc. At this level, were found to independently relate with psychometric g. These include specificities of task content and the context involving the taskmaybe processing speed (Coyle, 2017; Kail, Lervag, & Hulme, 2015), inhibition important. At a higher level, task-specific skills are organized in several and attention control (Arsalidou & Pascaul-Leone, 2016; Blair, 2006; broad domains, identified by mental processes shared by tasks. For Zelazo, 2015), and working memory (Baddeley, 2012; Case, 1992; ⁎ Corresponding author at: University of Nicosia, Makedonitissas Avenue, P.O. Box 24005, 1700 Nicosia, Cyprus. E-mail addresses: [email protected] (A. Demetriou), [email protected] (N. Makris), [email protected] (S. Kazi), [email protected] (G. Spanoudis). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2019.101404 Received 28 March 2019; Received in revised form 18 September 2019; Accepted 19 September 2019 Available online 06 November 2019 0160-2896/ © 2019 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. A. Demetriou, et al. Intelligence 77 (2019) 101404 Halford, Wilson, & Phillips, 1998; Kyllonen & Christal, 1990; Pascual- carefully and rehearse (Chevalier, Martis, Curran, & Munakata, 2015; Leone, 1970). Finally, recent research suggested that various aspects of Paulus, Tsalas, Proust, & Sodian, 2014; Spanoudis et al., 2015). In metacognition (Demetriou et al., 2018; Efklides, 2008) relate with g. adolescence, they become aware of inferential processes, such as de- These include awareness of mental processes and their mental load ductive and inductive reasoning, and of the constraints underlying their (Demetriou & Kazi, 2006; Kazi, Demetriou, Spanoudis, Zhang, & Wang, validity (Demetriou et al., 2017; Moshman, 2015). Thus, with devel- 2012), awareness of the origins of knowledge (Spanoudis, Demetriou, opment, individuals become increasingly accurate in evaluating their Kazi, Giorgala, & Zenonos, 2015), and self-evaluation of one's own performance and representing their own strengths and weaknesses. performance on cognitive tasks (Demetriou & Efklides, 1989; Makris, Overall, self-evaluations of performance and cognitive self-representa- Tahmatzidis, Demetriou, & Spanoudis, 2017). tions tend to reflect actual performance with relative accuracy since Makris et al. (2017) showed recently that psychometric g is a early adolescence, becoming increasingly stricter and less positive with complex additive function of all of these processes: attention control, attainment of principle-based reasoning (Demetriou et al., 2017; shifting flexibility, working memory, reasoning, and awareness ac- Demetriou, Makris, Kazi, Spanoudis, & Shayer, 2018; Demetriou & counted for 27%, 18%, 27%, 19%, and 7% the variance of g, respec- Spanoudis, 2018). tively, adding up to 98% of total g variance. van der Maas et al. (2006) The developmental patterns outlined suggest that the nature of g proposed that g may reflect the dynamic interaction between these varies in development. Specifically, the relation between reasoning and processes rather than any specific process as such; the relative con- processing and representational efficiency changes with development. tribution of each process in this interaction may vary across different On the one hand, the relation with attention control (−0.52, −0.35, tasks, depending upon their specific demands (van der Maas, Kan, and − 0.17, at 9–11, 11–13, and 13–15 years, respectively) and flex- Marsman, & Stevenson, 2017). Thus, it is important to specify how each ibility (−0.71, −0.38, and − 0.10, for the three age groups, respec- process relates with school performance at different phases of educa- tively) decreases with age, because these processes tend to automate tion. with age; on the other, the relation with working memory (0.06, 0.65, and 0.74, for the three age groups, respectively) and awareness 1.1.2. Development strengthens, because these processes develop until late adolescence All processes above develop from birth to adulthood. Processing (0.25, 0.30, and 0.35, for the three age groups, respectively). These becomes faster with time (Demetriou et al., 2013; Kail et al., 2015). patterns suggest a shift from executive processes related to attention Attention becomes more efficient in focusing on stimuli for the time control to reasoning processes and explicit awareness involved in pro- needed, in resisting distraction until processing is complete, and flex- blem solving and planning (Demetriou et al., 2017; Makris et al., 2017). ibly shifting between stimuli or responses according to needs (Arsalidou & Pascaul-Leone, 2016; Zelazo, 2015). Working memory increases so 1.2. Intelligence, socioeconomic environment, and academic performance that more information may be held in mind and processed (Case, 1992; Pascual-Leone, 1970). The unit of representation changes from reality- School performance is related to cognitive ability. Psychometric referenced representations to relational constructs signifying relations intelligence accounts for about 30% of variance of school performance, at various levels of abstraction (Demetriou & Spanoudis, 2018). although this relation may vary with level of education (Gustafsson & Inference also changes at several levels. For instance, at preschool, Balke, 1993; Roth et al., 2015). Also, academic self-concept (Guay, representations function in blocks largely matching their episodic March, & Boivin, 2003; Johannesson, 2017), self-evaluation (Mabe III & origin rather than inferential links. Toddlers may translate representa- West, 1982), cognitive self-representation, and personality (Demetriou,
Recommended publications
  • A Differential–Developmental Model (DDM): Mental Speed, Attention Lapses, and General Intelligence (G)
    Commentary A Differential–Developmental Model (DDM): Mental Speed, Attention Lapses, and General Intelligence (g) Thomas R. Coyle Department of Psychology, University of Texas at San Antonio, San Antonio, TX 78249, USA; [email protected]; Tel.: +1-210-458-7407; Fax: +1-210-458-5728 Academic Editors: Andreas Demetriou and George Spanoudis Received: 23 December 2016; Accepted: 6 June 2017; Published: 12 June 2017 Abstract: The aim of this paper is to provide a parsimonious account of developmental and individual differences in intelligence (measured as g). The paper proposes a Differential– Developmental Model (DDM), which focuses on factors common to intelligence and cognitive development (e.g., mental speed and attention lapses). It also proposes a complementary method based on Jensen’s box, namely a chronometric device. The device systematically varies task complexity, and separates two components of mental speed that differentially predict intelligence and cognitive development (reaction time and movement time). The paper reviews key assumptions of DDM, preliminary findings relevant to DDM, and future research on DDM. Keywords: cognitive development; differential–developmental model (DDM); Jensen’s box; intelligence; mental speed; attention lapses; reaction time; movement time 1. Introduction All models are wrong, but some are useful. ([1], p. 202). The aim of this paper is to provide a parsimonious account of developmental and individual difference in intelligence (measured as g). 1 To this end, the paper proposes a Differential– Developmental Model (DDM), which focuses on factors common to cognitive development and intelligence (e.g., mental speed and attention lapses). The model attempts to bridge the two disciplines at the heart of the Special Issue: differential psychology, which focuses on individual differences in intelligence, and cognitive development, which focuses on age differences in intelligence.
    [Show full text]
  • Binet's Error: Developmental Change and Individual Differences In
    Journal of Intelligence Article Binet’s Error: Developmental Change and Individual Differences in Intelligence Are Related to Different Mechanisms Mike Anderson School of Psychology & Exercise Science, Murdoch University, Murdoch 6150, Australia; [email protected]; Tel.: +61-8-9360-2186 Academic Editors: Andreas Demetriou and George Spanoudis Received: 23 February 2017; Accepted: 30 May 2017; Published: 9 June 2017 Abstract: In common with most, if not all, papers in this special issue, I will argue that understanding the nature of developmental change and individual differences in intelligence requires a theory of the mechanisms underlying both factors. Insofar as these mechanisms constitute part of the fundamental architecture of cognition, this is also an exercise in unifying the discipline and research on intelligence in both children and adults. However, I argue that a variety of data support a theory suggesting that developmental change is the province of mechanisms commonly regarded as components of executive functioning or cognitive control, whereas individual differences are constrained by the speed of information processing. Perhaps paradoxically, this leads to the conclusion that Binet’s fundamental insight—that children’s increasing ability to solve problems of increasing difficulty could generate a single scale of intelligence—is wrong. Compounding the paradox, this means that mental age and IQ are not simply two different ways of expressing the same thing, but are related to two different dimensions of g itself.
    [Show full text]
  • Mapping Mind-Brain Development: Towards a Comprehensive Theory
    Journal of Intelligence Review Mapping Mind-Brain Development: Towards a Comprehensive Theory George Spanoudis 1,* and Andreas Demetriou 2,3 1 Psychology Department, University of Cyprus, 1678 Nicosia, Cyprus 2 Department of Psychology, University of Nicosia, 1700 Nicosia, Cyprus; [email protected] 3 Cyprus Academy of Science, Letters, and Arts, 1700 Nicosia, Cyprus * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +357-22892969 Received: 20 January 2020; Accepted: 20 April 2020; Published: 26 April 2020 Abstract: The relations between the developing mind and developing brain are explored. We outline a theory of intellectual development postulating that the mind comprises four systems of processes (domain-specific, attention and working memory, reasoning, and cognizance) developing in four cycles (episodic, realistic, rule-based, and principle-based representations, emerging at birth, 2, 6, and 11 years, respectively), with two phases in each. Changes in reasoning relate to processing efficiency in the first phase and working memory in the second phase. Awareness of mental processes is recycled with the changes in each cycle and drives their integration into the representational unit of the next cycle. Brain research shows that each type of processes is served by specialized brain networks. Domain-specific processes are rooted in sensory cortices; working memory processes are mainly rooted in hippocampal, parietal, and prefrontal cortices; abstraction and alignment processes are rooted in parietal, frontal, and prefrontal and medial cortices. Information entering these networks is available to awareness processes. Brain networks change along the four cycles, in precision, connectivity, and brain rhythms. Principles of mind-brain interaction are discussed. Keywords: brain; mind; development; intelligence; mental and brain changes; brain networks 1.
    [Show full text]
  • 07. Cristiano Mauro Assis Gomes . OK.Indd
    ISSN 1678-7153 Psychology/Psicologia Refl exão e Crítica, 27(3), 472-480. – DOI: 10.1590/1678-7153.201427307 Self-reports on Students’ Learning Processes are Academic Metacognitive Knowledge Autorrelatos de Estudantes sobre seus Processos de Aprendizagem são Conhecimento Metacognitivo Acadêmico Cristiano Mauro Assis Gomes*, a & Hudson Fernandes Golinoa, b aUniversidade Federal de Minas Gerais, Belo Horizonte, Minas Gerais, Brasil & bFaculdade Independente do Nordeste, Vitória da Conquista, Bahia, Brasil Abstract The current study postulates that students’ self-reported perceptions on their academic processes are a type of metacognition: academic metacognitive knowledge (AMcK). We investigated, using Structural Equation Modeling (SEM), three hypotheses: (a) AMcK explains the variance of factor scores of students’ learning approaches (SLA) and academic motivation (AM); (b) AMcK is distinct from working metacognition (WMC); and (c) AMcK has incremental validity, beyond WMC, on the explanation of general academic achievement (GAA) variance. Two tests (indicators of WMC) and two scales (indicators of AMcK) were administered to 684 ten-to-eighteen-year-old Brazilian children and adolescents. Annual grades in Math, Portuguese, Geography and History were used as indicators of GAA. The results show that none of the three hypotheses can be refuted. Keywords: Metacognition, approaches to learning, motivation, intelligence. Resumo Este trabalho postula que percepções autorrelatadas de estudantes sobre seus processos acadêmicos são um tipo de metacognição: conhecimento metacognitivo acadêmico (AMcK). Usando o Modelo de Equações Estruturais, investigamos três hipóteses: (a) AMcK explica a variância dos escores fatoriais das abordagens de aprendizagem (SAL) e da motivação à aprendizagem (MAL) dos estudantes; (b) AMcK é distinto da hipercognição de trabalho (WMC); e (c) AMcK possui validadeincremental, além de WMC, na explicação do desempenho acadêmico geral (GAA).
    [Show full text]
  • Intelligence As a Developing Function: a Neuroconstructivist Approach
    Journal of Intelligence Article Intelligence as a Developing Function: A Neuroconstructivist Approach Luca Rinaldi 1,2,* and Annette Karmiloff-Smith 3 1 Department of Brain and Behavioural Sciences, University of Pavia, Pavia 27100, Italy 2 Milan Center for Neuroscience, Milano 20126, Italy 3 Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck, London WC1E 7HX, UK; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +39-02-6448-3775 Academic Editors: Andreas Demetriou and George Spanoudis Received: 23 December 2016; Accepted: 27 April 2017; Published: 29 April 2017 Abstract: The concept of intelligence encompasses the mental abilities necessary to survival and advancement in any environmental context. Attempts to grasp this multifaceted concept through a relatively simple operationalization have fostered the notion that individual differences in intelligence can often be expressed by a single score. This predominant position has contributed to expect intelligence profiles to remain substantially stable over the course of ontogenetic development and, more generally, across the life-span. These tendencies, however, are biased by the still limited number of empirical reports taking a developmental perspective on intelligence. Viewing intelligence as a dynamic concept, indeed, implies the need to identify full developmental trajectories, to assess how genes, brain, cognition, and environment interact with each other. In the present paper, we describe how a neuroconstructivist approach better explains why intelligence can rise or fall over development, as a result of a fluctuating interaction between the developing system itself and the environmental factors involved at different times across ontogenesis. Keywords: intelligence; individual differences; development; neuroconstructivism; emergent structure; developmental trajectory 1.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Andreas Demetriou Professor of Psychology
    ANDREAS DEMETRIOU PROFESSOR OF PSYCHOLOGY MINISTER OF EDUCATION AND CULTURE He was born in Strongylo, Famagusta in 1950. He is married to Julia Tsakalea and he has two sons, Pantelis and Demetris. Studies 1975: BA in Psychology and Education, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece. 1978: Graduate Studies in Psychology, University of New South Wales, Austarlia. 1983: PhD in Psychology, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece. Academic History 1975-1996: Member of the academic staff of the Department of Psychology of the Aristotle University of Thessaloniki—full professor of developmental psychology from 1992-1996. 1996-present: Professor of psychology, University of Cyprus. He taught as Visiting Fellow or Professor at several Universities, including the Universities of Porto, Portugal, Lublijana and Marribor, Slovenia, Fribourg, Swirzerland, and the University of Melboune, Australia, and lectured as invited lecturer for technical seminars on cognitive development at more than 50 Universities all over the world, including the Universities of Stanford, Harvard, Yale, McGill, Toronto, Munich, Leiden, Gothenburg, Wurtzburgh, Edinburgh, Leuven, etc. He is a member of many learned societies, including the European Association for Research on Learning and Instruction, the International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development, the Society for Research in Child Development, and the American Psychological Society. He served as editor or board member of many journals, including the Advances in Learning and Instruction, Learning and Instruction: The Journal of the European Association for Research on Learning and Instruction, Developmental Science, Psychology: The Journal of the Hellenic Psychological Society. Administrative Service 1992-1996: Chairman of the Department of Psychology, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece.
    [Show full text]
  • Dr. Andreas Demetriou
    Andreas Demetriou Andreas Demetriou is a Greek Cypriot developmental psychologist and former Minister of Education and Culture of Cyprus. He received a PhD in psychology in 1983. He was a professor of Developmental Psychology at the Aristotle University of Thessaloniki until 1996. He then moved to the University of Cyprus, where he was a professor of psychology until he became the Minister of Education and Culture. Currently he is professor of psychology and President of the University of Nicosia Research Foundation. He is also President of the Pancyprian Association of Psychologists. Research His research focuses on cognitive development. From the very beginning he attempted to develop a comprehensive theory of cognitive development aiming to integrate the empirically valid aspects of Piaget's theory with psychometric and cognitive theory. That is, the theory aimed to describe and explain intellectual development through the life span, individual differences in the rate and directions of intellectual development, and the cognitive mechanisms underlying development and individual differences. According to this theory, the human mind is organised in three functional levels. The first is the level of processing potentials, which involves information processing mechanisms underlying the ability to attend to, select, represent, and operate on information. The other two levels involve knowing processes, one oriented to the environment and another oriented to the self. The level oriented to the environment includes thought processes and functions that specialise in the representation and processing of information coming from different domains of the environment. Six domains are specified: Categorical, quantitative, causal, spatial, propositional, and social thought. The self-oriented level includes functions and processes oriented to monitoring, representing, and regulating processing potentials and the environment-oriented systems.
    [Show full text]
  • Bridging the Twenty-First Century Gap in Education – History, Causation, and Solutions
    Part 1 Educational Transitions in Context Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. IP address: 170.106.35.229, on 27 Sep 2021 at 15:19:29, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1062798720000873 European Review, Vol. 28, No. S1, S7–S27 © 2020 Academia Europaea. This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/ licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. doi:10.1017/S1062798720000873 Cognitive Context Bridging the Twenty-first Century Gap in Education – History, Causation, and Solutions ANDREAS DEMETRIOU Cyprus Academy of Science, Letters, and Arts, Faneromenis 60-68, 1010 Nicosia, Cyprus. Email: [email protected] This article discusses the gaps in the cognitive demands of education as higher levels of education became socially and culturally necessary. These gaps are related to major transitions between education levels, such as the transition from preschool to primary school, from primary to secondary, or from secondary to tertiary educa- tion. Gaps reflect deviations between the concepts and skills prescribed for learning by a specific population and the readiness of this population to cope with the demands of the task within the time frame prescribed. The history and the cognitive developmental profile of the gaps is outlined. This article focuses on the gap between secondary and tertiary education. It is explained that a major reason for the gap is the vast expansion in the population of youth attending university studies.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Development Are Discussed in the Concluding Part of the Paper
    CORE Metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk Provided by Cognitive Sciences ePrint Archive LOGICAL AND PSYCHOLOGICAL PARTITIONING OF MIND: DEPICTING THE SAME MAP? Philip V. Kargopoulos Aristotelian University of Thessaloniki, Greece & Andreas Demetriou University of Cyprus, Cyprus Correspondence: Andreas Demetriou, Department of Education, University of Cyprus, P.O. Box 20537, 1678 Nicosia, Cyprus. Tel: +357 2 338881; Fax: +357 2 339064; email: [email protected] 1 Abstract The aim of this paper is to demonstrate that empirically delimited structures of mind are also differentiable by means of systematic logical analysis. In the sake of this aim, the paper first summarizes Demetriou's theory of cognitive organization and growth. This theory assumes that the mind is a multistructural entity that develops across three fronts: the processing system that constrains processing potentials, a set of specialized structural systems (SSSs) that guide processing within different reality and knowledge domains, and a hypecognitive system that monitors and controls the functioning of all other systems. In the second part the paper focuses on the SSSs, which are the target of our logical analysis, and it summarizes a series of empirical studies demonstrating their autonomous operation. The third part develops the logical proof showing that each SSS involves a kernel element that cannot be reduced to standard logic or to any other SSS. The implications of this analysis for the general theory of knowledge and cognitive development are discussed in the concluding part of the paper. 2 LOGICAL AND PSYCHOLOGICAL PARTITIONING OF MIND: DEPICTING THE SAME MAP? This paper, co-authored by a logician (Kargopoulos) and a cognitive developmentalist (Demetriou), is intended as a contribution to what we might call developmental cognitive science.
    [Show full text]
  • Key Topics in Education
    ROUTLEDGE . TAYLOR & FRANCIS Key Topics in Education A Chapter Sampler www.routledge.com/education Contents 1.What is feedback? From: Visible Learning Feedback, by John Hattie, Shirley Clark 2. Think right From: Great Minds and How to Grow Them, by Wendy Berliner, Deborah Eyre 3. Awareness and knowledge about the mind From: Growing Minds, by Andreas Demetriou, George Spanoudis 4. Pervasive Misunderstandings About Learning: How They Arise, and What We Can Do From: Understanding How We Learn, by Yana Weinstein, Megan Sumeracki, Oliver Caviglioli 5. Theories and theorists From: Early Childhood Studies, by Jane Johnston, Lindy Nahmad-Williams, Ruby Oates, Val Wood 6. The Idea of Evolution From: Evolution of the Learning Brain, by Paul Howard-Jones 20% Discount Available You can enjoy a 20% discount across our entire range of Routledge books. Simply add the discount code F010 at the checkout. Please note: This discount code cannot be combined with any other discount or offer and is only valid on print titles purchased directly from www.routledge.com. Valid until 31st December 2019. www.routledge.com/education CHAPTER Copyright Material - Taylor & Francis 1 What is feedback? This chapter summarizes the key points about the nature and development of feed- back thinking by educators and academics, laying the foundation for the related issues to be explored throughout the rest of this book. The subsequent chapters are closely tied to the life of a teacher and a student in the day to day structure of lessons, as outlined in the introduction: culture, learning strategies, in lesson feedback and post- lesson feedback.
    [Show full text]
  • Mastery Motivation in Early Childhood: Development
    Mastery motivation in early childhood All children possess a motive to ‘master’ the various tasks and problems that they face. Without mastery motivation, it is doubtful whether children would make progress in cognitive, social, communicative and other domains. Although all children possess this motivation, it will vary according to inherited dispositions and to environmental experiences. This makes mastery motivation a key factor in understanding later developmental and educational achievement. Concentrating on pre-school children, the volume brings together current research work and thinking concerned with mastery motivation. New ideas are presented about the way mastery is related to other developmental processes such as self-concepts and attention. There are discussions and findings about innovations in the methods of assessing mastery. Another important theme present in this volume, is the way in which features of social interaction, attachment and the environment influence the development of mastery motivation. With a broad range of international contributors, Mastery Motivation in Early Childhood will be of interest to developmental psychologists and educationalists, and advanced students in these fields. David Messer is Reader in Developmental Psychology at the University of Hertfordshire. International Library of Psychology Editorial adviser, Developmental psychology. Peter K.Smith University of Sheffield Neo-Piagetian Theories of Cognitive Development Edited by Andreas Demetriou, Michael Shayer and Anastasia Efklides New Perspectives in Early Communicative Development Edited by Jacqueline Nadel and Luigia Camaioni Classroom Nonverbal Communication Sean Neill Mastery motivation in early childhood Development, measurement and social processes Edited by David Messer London and New York First published 1993 by Routledge 11 New Fetter Lane, London EC4P 4EE This edition published in the Taylor & Francis e-Library, 2003.
    [Show full text]
  • Introduction the What, How and Why of Developmental Change: the Emergence of a New Paradigm
    Cambridge University Press 0521825792 - Cognitive Developmental Change: Theories, Models and Measurement Edited by Andreas Demetriou and Athanassios Raftopoulos Excerpt More information Introduction The what, how and why of developmental change: the emergence of a new paradigm Andreas Demetriou and Athanassios Raftopoulos This book presents current theory and research on cognitive change. Chapter authors were invited to discuss cognitive change from the perspective of its three main aspects. Its object (what changes in the mind during development?), its nature (how does change occur?), and its causes (why does change occur, or, in other words, what are the factors, internal and external, that are responsible for cognitive change?). Obviously, these are both old and fundamental questions and all theories of development attempted to answer one or more of them. Piaget was the first to provide a full set of answers to all three of these questions. His answer to these questions can rather easily be summarized as follows: opera- tional structures (what?) change through reflecting abstraction, which organizes the results of assimilation and accommodation (how?), because of maturation, cultural influences, and self-organization (why?) (Piaget 1970, 2001). The rate of change during the course from birth to maturity varies systematically, accel- erating and slowing down at different phases, so that cognitive development appears to be stage-like. In a sense, this summary of Piaget’s theory is also an accurate summary of the present volume. If this had been the whole story it would have been nice because our task of writing an introduction to the book would have finished here. Fortunately, for the field at least, this is not the whole story.
    [Show full text]