Gestalt and Shame: the Foundation for a Clearer Understanding of Field Dynamics Robert G

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Gestalt and Shame: the Foundation for a Clearer Understanding of Field Dynamics Robert G 1 1995, The British Gestalt Journal, 4(1), 14-22. Gestalt and Shame: The Foundation for a Clearer Understanding of Field Dynamics Robert G. Lee Abstract. The Gestalt model inherently incorporates an understanding of shame dynamics in its analysis of contact processes. Shame is a field variable, a ground condition modulating the contact boundary, a natural process of retroflection enlisted at times of perceived insufficient support. The shame-support polarity, when functioning optimally, allows the person to be at the edge and to venture beyond old organizations of the field--i.e. to grow. Conversely, with severe or persistent lack of support (e.g., severe or sustained abuse, neglect, or loss), shame becomes internalized and integrated into basic beliefs about the 'self and the possibilities of contact with others. These fixed gestalts (Perls' introjects) which have been learned in a particular field then become blueprints with which to interpret experience and guide behaviour in general, restricting flexibility. Restoring flexibility means facing the shame that holds the fixed gestalts in place. This can only happen in the context of a relationship. One of the chief processes of therapy then becomes supporting awareness of shame in the present field, between therapist and client. Key words: gestalt, Gestalt Therapy, Field theory, shame, support, contact processes, self processes, shame bind, introject, figure and ground. About seven years ago I discovered the work of Tomkins (e.g. 1963) and of Kaufman (e.g. 1980, 1989) on the phenomenon of shame. I was taken with the similarity between what I read and what I had been noticing in my clients. As a result, I became extremely interested in the phenomenon of shame. At that time I did not conceptualize shame as being related to Gestalt theory or therapy. I don't remember exactly when my thinking changed, but as part of the transition, I remember attending a day-long seminar in Boston given by Kaufman five or six years ago. In the first half of the day, Kaufman presented and discussed theory connected with the phenomenon of shame, and in the second half, he conducted an experiential demonstration 2 on the treatment of shame. During the first half of the day, I had the sense of starting to master a new discipline. However, my experience in the second half was completely different. As Kaufman proceeded with his demonstration, I quickly found myself in old, familiar territory. Much of what Kaufman illustrated was very similar to what I was trained to do at the Gestalt Institute of Cleveland and to what I had been doing as a Gestalt therapist for many years. With time I noticed more and more that significant portions of Gestalt theory and therapy concerned the phenomenon of shame without mentioning shame by name. Correspondingly, as I became aware of Gestalt theory'3 inherent incorporation of shame dynamics in its treatment of contact processes, I gained a fuller understanding of the nature of shame. Introjects and Shame Binds To begin with, I noticed (Lee, 1994) that what Gestalt theorists refer to as an introject (conceived of by Perls, 1947, and refined by others, e.g., Polster and Polster, 1973) is what Tomkins (1963) and Kaufman (1989) call a shame-bind. From a Gestalt perspective, this is significant because Perls' idea of 'resistance to contact,' which is central to his conception of neurosis, is centered around his construct of'introjects.' All of Perls' basic resistances to contact (introjection, projection, and retroflection) are organized around an introject at their core. Introjects are 'standards, attitudes, ways of acting and thinking, which are not truly ours' (Perls, 1973, p. 35) yet organize our functioning. Thus, introjects are cognitive, affective, and kinesthetic information from the environment that has been 'swallowed whole,' according to Perls, rather than assimilated (i.e. reorganized to be in harmony with one's needs and one's internal and external resources). In order to live in accordance with introjects, one must disown aspects of one's functioning. The cognitive part of introjects are beliefs (for example, 'shoulds' and 'oughts') that hold within them the piece ofthe self that has been disowned. For example, the cognitive part of an introject may take the form of 'I shouldn't be angry' or 'I shouldn't need help when I'm hurt.' In these cases what is disowned, respectively, is one's anger or one's need for nurturance or assistance when hurt. Perls maintains that introjects form because of over control by the environment, i.e., the person is not allowed to. act in accordance with his or her own needs 3 and sense of self. This is very simUar to how Tomkins (1963) and Kaufman (1989) describe shame binds. Shame binds form when a person's feelings, needs, or sense of purpose (i.e., the building blocks of ways of being in the world) are seriously unsupported by the environment, through abuse, neglect, or loss. When the experience of shame is severe enough or chronic enough a linkage forms between shame and the feeling, need, or sense of purpose that is unsupported. Thereafter, whenever the person experiences the given feeling, need, or sense of purpose, he or she will automatically also experience shame. And, in more severe cases, with greater unavailability of support, the person will lose awareness of the feeling, need, or sense of purpose and only experience the shame. This is similar to the consequences of an introject in which the need, etc. is disowned. Comparing their similarity further, both shame binds and introjects are formed in the context of relationships where people rely on others, e.g., parent-child, teacher-student, husband-wife, employer-employee, etc. As Kaufman (1980) says, it is when people care that they are vulnerable to shame. It is in this context, when people have needs that must be met for survival or growth, that they are vulnerable to the threat of abandonment which in tum triggers shame. Expressed in Gestalt terms, Tomkins' and Kaufman's understanding of shame is transformed into the following: shame is a regulator of the boundary between 'self and the social field. (I will discuss this further later.) Under these conditions, when there is an aspect of one's functioning that is disapproved of by significant others, one may experience a threat of abandonment and over time, disown the disapproved aspect of one's functioning through shame, thus 'swallowing' an introject. This allows the person to continue to belong in the needed relationship and to continue to have hope of satisfying those needs that he or she perceives as important for survival or growth. The connection between shame binds and introjects continues as we look further at Perls' theory. Perls et al often refer to habitual resistances to contact as neurotic mechanisms. They state: 'every neurotic mechanism is an interruption of some kind of excitement--a prevention of its further development' (Perls, Hefferline, & Goodman, 1951, p. 145). But according to Tomkins (1963), it is shame that regulates excitement. Shame's function is to help the organism 4 pull back from interest-excitement or enjoyment-joy that is perceived to be inappropriate or dangerous. Thus, theoretically, shame is associated with Perls' neurotic mechanisms which in tum have introjects as their basis. As a test of whether or not a relationship exists in fact between introjects and shame, I examined transcripts of Perls' sessions with clients. Ifit is true that Perls' introjects are shame binds, we would expect that as Perls works with client's neurotic mechanisms in sessions, verbal expressions and other signs of shame would appear. These would be associated with client's underlying introjects or with attempts to avoid their associated shame. A reading of the transcripts of Perls' (1973) sessions reveals many such instances. For example, a client who is talking to himself, in the process of re-owning his projection (the projection being that he believes that Perls wants him to 'stretch') states to himself: 'Don, you 're not good enough the way you are so you've got to stretch' (p. 122) (italics added). 'You've got to stretch' is the introject, and 'you're not good enough' is the accompanying shame. Another client says: 'I try to hide my fear' (p. 130) (italics added). The introject here is 'I should not be afraid,' and hiding is an expression of the shame. Another client, in re-owning her power associated with being tall, says to the audience (when Perls suggests that she talk as if she is a giant): 'you might think that's a silly idea. You might laugh at me' (p. 171) (italics added). The introject here is something like 'I should not be powerful.' And imagining that others would think that she was silly and laugh at her, she projects her shame onto others. In another instance, Perls chides a couple for falling into the 'blaming game' (p. 140) (italics added). This is an example ofthe use ofblame (a form of shaming another) to avoid one's own shame. (Lansky, 1980, talks at length about couple's shame cycles that involve blame.) In almost all of the transcripts, Perls presents himself working with a client, and 1 facilitating the impasse , and in each case direct or indirect signs appear that indicate that the client is experiencing shame. While there is much of Perls' theory and practice as a therapist with which I do not agree, some of which I will discuss shortly, this is supportive evidence, both theoretical and practical, that introjects are shame binds. 5 Introjects in the Light of Shame Theory My recognition that introjects are shame binds brought with it a new understanding of the nature of introjects as well as support for the view that Wheeler (1991), among others, has espoused that there are some serious flaws in Perls' theory.
Recommended publications
  • Strategies for Managing Evaluation Anxiety: Toward a Psychology of Program Evaluation
    Strategies for Managing Evaluation Anxiety: Toward a Psychology of Program Evaluation STEWART I. DONALDSON, LAURA E. GOOLER, AND MICHAEL SCRIVEN ABSTRACT Excessive evaluation anxiety (XEA) can be a destructive phenomenon in modern program eval- uation. Some of the negative consequences include: Lack of access to important information and data; compliance and cooperation problems; false reporting; effects on bias and validity; and reduced utilization of evaluation findings. If left alone, XEA can lead stakeholders to behave in ways that destroy the credibility of evaluation findings and evaluators. The purpose of this paper is to examine the sources and signs of XEA in program evaluation, and to provide practicing evaluators with strategies to prevent and manage this common problem. This example of how more than technical skills are required to conduct high quality program evaluations illustrates the need for and begins a broader discussion of the psychology of evaluation. INTRODUCTION Most people experience anxiety when their behavior or achievements are being evaluated. Whether evaluations are formal, as in the case of standardized achievement testing, or informal, such as being picked to be part of a soccer team or cheerleading squad, the experience of being evaluated, critiqued, or judged commonly results in an emotional reaction of uneasiness, uncertainty, or apprehension. In essence, many evaluative situations cause people to fear that they will be found to be deficient or inadequate by others (e.g., supervisors, funding agencies, or evaluators in the program evaluation context). The fear of the prospect of a negative evaluation is probably inherent to being human. Criticism, ridicule, contempt, embarrassment, loss of acceptability, loss of respect, and rejection are unpleasant experiences most people attempt to avoid.
    [Show full text]
  • Incurable Psychopaths?
    Incurable Psychopaths? Marianne Kristiansson, MD Treatment, comprising pharmacotherapy and an educational program based on cognitive behavior therapy, of four psychopathic, criminal men fulfilling the crite- ria for borderline personality disorder and antisocial personality disorder is de- scribed. The diagnoses were made during a forensic psychiatric evaluation. An estimation of the capacity of the central serotonergic system was performed by analysing the platelet monoamine oxidase (MAO) activity. The pharmacotherapy was combined with an educational program involving strategies for developing better impulse control. All four men had earlier been regarded as resistant to conventional therapy. In the present cases, a combined psychosocial and biolog- ical approach seemed to be effective in developing an increased control of im- pulses, leading to improved coping strategies. Controlled studies are needed in order to clarify whether the described treatment program proves beneficial. Psychopathy, as originally described by personality disorder according to DSM- Cleckley,' comprises a set of clinical 111-R.~ characteristics including superficial The possibility of curing or even trying charm, unreliability, untruthfulness, lack to treat criminal psychopathic individuals of remorse or shame, failure to learn by is often looked upon as futile. Most treat- experience, incapacity for love, general ment programs involve various psycho- poverty in major affective relations, and social interventions and little effort has failure to follow any life
    [Show full text]
  • Applying a Discrete Emotion Perspective
    AROUSAL OR RELEVANCE? APPLYING A DISCRETE EMOTION PERSPECTIVE TO AGING AND AFFECT REGULATION SARA E. LAUTZENHISER Bachelor of Science in Psychology Ashland University May 2015 Submitted in partial fulfillment of requirements for the degree MASTER OF ARTS IN PSYCHOLOGY At the CLEVELAND STATE UNIVERSITY May 2019 We hereby approve this thesis For SARA E. LAUTZENHISER Candidate for the Master of Arts in Experimental Research Psychology For the Department of Psychology And CLEVELAND STATE UNIVERSITY’S College of Graduate Studies by __________________________ Eric Allard, Ph.D. __________________________ Department & Date __________________________ Andrew Slifkin, Ph. D. (Methodologist) __________________________ Department & Date __________________________ Conor McLennan, Ph.D. __________________________ Department & Date __________________________ Robert Hurley, Ph. D. __________________________ Department & Date Student’s Date of Defense May 10, 2019 AROUSAL OR RELEVANCE? APPLYING A DISCRETE EMOTION PERSPECTIE TO AGING AND AFFECT REGULATION SARA E. LAUTZENHISER ABSTRACT While research in the psychology of human aging suggests that older adults are quite adept at managing negative affect, emotion regulation efficacy may depend on the discrete emotion elicited. For instance, prior research suggests older adults are more effective at dealing with emotional states that are more age-relevant/useful and lower in intensity (i.e., sadness) relative to less relevant/useful or more intense (i.e., anger). The goal of the present study was to probe this discrete emotions perspective further by addressing the relevance/intensity distinction within a broader set of negative affective states (i.e., fear and disgust, along with anger and sadness). Results revealed that participants reported relatively high levels of the intended emotion for each video, while also demonstrating significant affective recovery after the attentional refocusing task.
    [Show full text]
  • Action to End Child Sexual Abuse and Exploitation
    ACTION TO END CHILD SEXUAL ABUSE AND EXPLOITATION Published by UNICEF Child Protection Section Programme Division 3 United Nations Plaza New York, NY 10017 Email: [email protected] Website: www.unicef.org © United Nations Children’s Fund (UNICEF) December 2020. Permission is required to reproduce any part of this publication. Permission will be freely granted to educational or non-profit organizations. For more information on usage rights, please contact: [email protected] Cover photo: © UNICEF/UNI303881/Zaidi Design and layout by Big Yellow Taxi, Inc. Suggested citation: United Nations Children’s Fund (2020) Action to end child sexual abuse and exploitation, UNICEF, New York This publication has been produced with financial support from the End Violence Fund. However, the opinions, findings, conclusions, and recommendations expressed herein do not necessarily reflect those of the End Violence Fund. Click on section bars to navigate publication CONTENTS 1. Introduction ............................................3 6. Service delivery ...................................21 2. A Global Problem...................................5 7. Social & behavioural change ................27 3. Building on the evidence .................... 11 8. Gaps & challenges ...............................31 4. A Theory of Change ............................13 Endnotes .................................................32 5. Enabling National Environments ..........15 1 Ending Child Sexual Abuse and Exploitation: A Review of the Evidence ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
    [Show full text]
  • On the Deep Structure of Social Affect: Attitudes, Emotions, Sentiments, and the Case of “Contempt”
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2017), Page 1 of 63 doi:10.1017/S0140525X16000352,e0 On the deep structure of social affect: Attitudes, emotions, sentiments, and the case of “contempt” Matthew M. Gervais School of Human Evolution and Social Change, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ 85287-2402; Center for Human Evolutionary Studies, Department of Anthropology, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, NJ 08901-1414 [email protected] www.matthewgervais.net Daniel M. T. Fessler Center for Behavior, Evolution, and Culture, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095-1553; Department of Anthropology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095-1553 [email protected] www.danielmtfessler.com Abstract: Contempt is typically studied as a uniquely human moral emotion. However, this approach has yielded inconclusive results. We argue this is because the folk affect concept “contempt” has been inaccurately mapped onto basic affect systems. “Contempt” has features that are inconsistent with a basic emotion, especially its protracted duration and frequently cold phenomenology. Yet other features are inconsistent with a basic attitude. Nonetheless, the features of “contempt” functionally cohere. To account for this, we revive and reconfigure the sentiment construct using the notion of evolved functional specialization. We develop the Attitude–Scenario–Emotion (ASE) model of sentiments, in which enduring attitudes represent others’ social-relational value and moderate discrete emotions across scenarios. Sentiments are functional networks of attitudes and emotions. Distinct sentiments, including love, respect, like, hate, and fear, track distinct relational affordances, and each is emotionally pluripotent, thereby serving both bookkeeping and commitment functions within relationships. The sentiment contempt is an absence of respect; from cues to others’ low efficacy, it represents them as worthless and small, muting compassion, guilt, and shame and potentiating anger, disgust, and mirth.
    [Show full text]
  • About Emotions There Are 8 Primary Emotions. You Are Born with These
    About Emotions There are 8 primary emotions. You are born with these emotions wired into your brain. That wiring causes your body to react in certain ways and for you to have certain urges when the emotion arises. Here is a list of primary emotions: Eight Primary Emotions Anger: fury, outrage, wrath, irritability, hostility, resentment and violence. Sadness: grief, sorrow, gloom, melancholy, despair, loneliness, and depression. Fear: anxiety, apprehension, nervousness, dread, fright, and panic. Joy: enjoyment, happiness, relief, bliss, delight, pride, thrill, and ecstasy. Interest: acceptance, friendliness, trust, kindness, affection, love, and devotion. Surprise: shock, astonishment, amazement, astound, and wonder. Disgust: contempt, disdain, scorn, aversion, distaste, and revulsion. Shame: guilt, embarrassment, chagrin, remorse, regret, and contrition. All other emotions are made up by combining these basic 8 emotions. Sometimes we have secondary emotions, an emotional reaction to an emotion. We learn these. Some examples of these are: o Feeling shame when you get angry. o Feeling angry when you have a shame response (e.g., hurt feelings). o Feeling fear when you get angry (maybe you’ve been punished for anger). There are many more. These are NOT wired into our bodies and brains, but are learned from our families, our culture, and others. When you have a secondary emotion, the key is to figure out what the primary emotion, the feeling at the root of your reaction is, so that you can take an action that is most helpful. .
    [Show full text]
  • A Study of Emotional Intelligence and Frustration Tolerance Among Adolescent
    ADVANCE RESEARCH JOURNAL OF SOCIAL SCIENCE RESEARCH ARTICLE Volume 6 | Issue 2 | December, 2015 | 173-180 e ISSN–2231–6418 DOI: 10.15740/HAS/ARJSS/6.2/173-180 Visit us : www.researchjournal.co.in A study of emotional intelligence and frustration tolerance among adolescent Archana Kumari* and Sandhya Gupta The IIS University, JAIPUR (RAJASTHAN) INDIA (Email: [email protected]; [email protected]) ARTICLE INFO : ABSTRACT Received : 14.07.2015 In every sphere of life whether it is education, academic or personal, adolescents feel Revised : 22.10.2015 lots of obstacles on the way of their goals in life. Sometimes they are able to deal with Accepted : 03.11.2015 them rationally but sometimes they deal with it emotionally. In case if they are incapable KEY WORDS : to deal with these obstacles they get frustrated. To cope up with frustration the adolescents need to be emotionally intelligent that means they should have flexibility, Emotional intelligence, Frustration, optimist thought and skilled to control impulses. The present study attempted to Tolerance, Adolescent correlate frustration tolerance with emotional intelligence. A total of 120 adolescents were selected from Jaipur city in the age group of 12- 19 years of age. Out of 120 HOW TO CITE THIS ARTICLE : Kumari, Archana and Gupta, Sandhya adolescents, 60 were girls and 60 were boys. For data collection Emotional intelligence (2015). A study of emotional intelligence scale and Frustration tolerance tool was used. A positive correlation was found between and frustration tolerance among emotional intelligence and frustration tolerance of adolescents. Girls were found to adolescent. Adv.
    [Show full text]
  • The Effects of Incidental Anger, Contempt, and Disgust on Hostile Language and Implicit Behaviors David Matsumoto1, Hyisung C
    Journal of Applied Social Psychology 2016, 46, pp. 437–452 The effects of incidental anger, contempt, and disgust on hostile language and implicit behaviors David Matsumoto1, Hyisung C. Hwang1, Mark G. Frank2 1Department of Psychology, San Francisco State University and Humintell, LLC 2University at Buffalo, State University of New York Correspondence concerning this addressed to Abstract David Matsumoto, Department of Psychology, San Francisco State University, 1600 Holloway Recent studies have suggested that the combination of the emotions anger- Avenue, San Francisco, CA 94132, USA. contempt-disgust (ANCODI) is associated with intergroup hostility. This study E-mail: [email protected] examined if incidental elicitation of this emotion combination causally produces hostile cognitions, language, and behaviors. Members of political groups were doi: 10.1111/jasp.12374 primed with either ANCODI, fear 1 sadness, or no emotion, and then engaged in creativity task in relation to their opponent or a non-opponent outgroup. The ANCODI mix produced more hostile cognitions, language, and implicit behaviors associated with hostility, in some cases specifically toward their opponent outgroups, than individuals primed with other emotions. Multiple mediation analyses indicated that the three emotions and their interactions mediated many of the effects. Despite the relatively robust research examining emotion and assessed. Leaders of groups that eventually committed acts of interpersonal aggression (see review in Anderson & Bush- aggression expressed words, language-based metaphorical man, 2002), there has been surprisingly little research on the appraisals, and nonverbal behaviors related to anger, con- role of emotions in intergroup aggression. Two studies in this tempt, and disgust toward their opponent outgroups; other area point to an important role for anger, and to a lesser emotions did not differentiate groups that committed acts of extent, fear.
    [Show full text]
  • Bodily Maps of Emotions
    Bodily maps of emotions Lauri Nummenmaaa,b,c,1, Enrico Glereana, Riitta Harib,1, and Jari K. Hietanend aDepartment of Biomedical Engineering and Computational Science and bBrain Research Unit, O. V. Lounasmaa Laboratory, School of Science, Aalto University, FI-00076, Espoo, Finland; cTurku PET Centre, University of Turku, FI-20521, Turku, Finland; and dHuman Information Processing Laboratory, School of Social Sciences and Humanities, University of Tampere, FI-33014, Tampere, Finland Contributed by Riitta Hari, November 27, 2013 (sent for review June 11, 2013) Emotions are often felt in the body, and somatosensory feedback independence of bodily topographies across emotions. We pro- has been proposed to trigger conscious emotional experiences. pose that consciously felt emotions are associated with culturally Here we reveal maps of bodily sensations associated with different universal, topographically distinct bodily sensations that may emotions using a unique topographical self-report method. In five support the categorical experience of different emotions. n = experiments, participants ( 701) were shown two silhouettes of Results bodies alongside emotional words, stories, movies, or facial expres- sions. They were asked to color the bodily regions whose activity We ran five experiments, with 36–302 participants in each. In they felt increasing or decreasing while viewing each stimulus. experiment 1, participants reported bodily sensations associated “ ” “ ” Different emotions were consistently associated with statistically with six basic and seven nonbasic ( complex ) emotions, as separable bodily sensation maps across experiments. These maps well as a neutral state, all described by the corresponding emo- were concordant across West European and East Asian samples. tion words. Fig. 2 shows the bodily sensation maps associated Statistical classifiers distinguished emotion-specific activation maps with each emotion.
    [Show full text]
  • Literature Review: Is the Emotional Expression of Contempt
    Literature Review: Is the Emotional Expression of Contempt Recognized Universally or Culturally? Julianna Phoukhao University of California, Merced Keywords: Contempt, Facial Expression, Forced Choice, Freely Labeling, Universal, Cultural, Emotion. 1 Abstract The universal facial expression of contempt is often described as one lip corner raised and tightened. This literature reviews whether or not this expression is recognized universally. After examining theories and methods, low agreement of this expression recognized as contempt was found across cultures. Evidence so far is not sufficient enough to support the unilateral lip corner as an universal expression for contempt. The expression and recognition of contempt is highly dependent on culture and context. 2 Literature Review Contempt is an emotion described as annoyance (Alvarado, 1996), ignoring or belittling a person (Fischer & Roseman, 2007), and involving another person’s negative actions and feelings of superiority (Wagner, 2000). This emotion is usually initiated when one perceives the inability to change or correct another person’s behavior. The social function of contempt is to end a relationship through rejection and exclusion (Fischer & Roseman, 2007). The concept of emotions serving functions dates back to Darwin’s days (1876) when he proposed the physiological component and facial expressions of basic emotions as innate because they serve towards a goal in one’s environment. He used his observations of evolution as evidence of the universality of emotions. Ekman (1971) challenged Darwin’s theory by conducting his own research in a remote village of Papua New Guinea. Ekman discovered emotions as innate and recognizable through facial expression. His research has also suggested the universality of a contempt expression (Ekman, 1988).
    [Show full text]
  • A Researcher Speaks to Ombudsmen About Workplace Bullying LORALE IGH KEASHLY
    Journal of the International Ombudsman Association Keashly Some Things You Need to Know but may have been Afraid to Ask: A Researcher Speaks to Ombudsmen about Workplace Bullying LORALE IGH KEASHLY ABSTRACT In the early 1990’s, I became interested in understand- ing persistent and enduring hostility at work. That Workplace bullying is repeated and prolonged hostile interest was spurred by a colleague’s experience at mistreatment of one or more people at work. It has the hands of her director. He yelled and screamed tremendous potential to escalate, drawing in others at her (and others), accusing her of not completing beyond the initial actor-target relationship. Its effects assignments, which she actually had. He lied about can be devastating and widespread individually, her and other subordinates. He would deliberately organizationally and beyond. It is fundamentally a avoid when staff needed his input and then berate systemic phenomenon grounded in the organization’s them for not consulting with him. At other times, he culture. In this article, I identify from my perspective was thoughtful, apologetic, and even constructive. My as a researcher and professional in this area current colleague felt like she was walking on eggshells, never thinking and research findings that may be useful for sure how he would be. Her coworkers had similar ombudsmen in their deliberations and investigations experiences and the group developed ways of coping as well as in their intervention and management of and handling it. For example, his secretary would these hostile behaviors and relationships. warn staff when it was not a good idea to speak with him.
    [Show full text]
  • Emotion Classification Based on Biophysical Signals and Machine Learning Techniques
    S S symmetry Article Emotion Classification Based on Biophysical Signals and Machine Learning Techniques Oana Bălan 1,* , Gabriela Moise 2 , Livia Petrescu 3 , Alin Moldoveanu 1 , Marius Leordeanu 1 and Florica Moldoveanu 1 1 Faculty of Automatic Control and Computers, University POLITEHNICA of Bucharest, Bucharest 060042, Romania; [email protected] (A.M.); [email protected] (M.L.); fl[email protected] (F.M.) 2 Department of Computer Science, Information Technology, Mathematics and Physics (ITIMF), Petroleum-Gas University of Ploiesti, Ploiesti 100680, Romania; [email protected] 3 Faculty of Biology, University of Bucharest, Bucharest 030014, Romania; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +40722276571 Received: 12 November 2019; Accepted: 18 December 2019; Published: 20 December 2019 Abstract: Emotions constitute an indispensable component of our everyday life. They consist of conscious mental reactions towards objects or situations and are associated with various physiological, behavioral, and cognitive changes. In this paper, we propose a comparative analysis between different machine learning and deep learning techniques, with and without feature selection, for binarily classifying the six basic emotions, namely anger, disgust, fear, joy, sadness, and surprise, into two symmetrical categorical classes (emotion and no emotion), using the physiological recordings and subjective ratings of valence, arousal, and dominance from the DEAP (Dataset for Emotion Analysis using EEG, Physiological and Video Signals) database. The results showed that the maximum classification accuracies for each emotion were: anger: 98.02%, joy:100%, surprise: 96%, disgust: 95%, fear: 90.75%, and sadness: 90.08%. In the case of four emotions (anger, disgust, fear, and sadness), the classification accuracies were higher without feature selection.
    [Show full text]