Cues to Vowels in the Aperiodic Phase of English Plosive Onsets Kaj Christian Nyman Mphil University of York Language and Lingui

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Cues to Vowels in the Aperiodic Phase of English Plosive Onsets Kaj Christian Nyman Mphil University of York Language and Lingui 1 Cues to Vowels in the Aperiodic Phase of English Plosive Onsets Kaj Christian Nyman MPhil University of York Language and Linguistic Science September 2011 2 Abstract This thesis addresses the problem of vowel recognition in coarticulatory theory and phonology by assessing how early vowel quality can be recognised from English onset plosives realised with aspiration. Particular attention is paid to aspects of production and perception timing. A gating experiment was used to assess how reliably listeners can recognise English monophthongs. The treatment of coarticulation distinguishes between phonetic and phonological aspects of production and perception, with a clear demarcation between these levels of representation. The results are interpreted through the lens of prosodic phonology, as this framework constrains the grammar more optimally than segmental-phonemic ones and better exemplifies listeners’ sensitivity to the distribution of FPD. Velar and bilabial onsets give rise to significantly more correct responses than alveolars, which require more precise articulations. High vowels are recognised more reliably than low ones. This result is due to their intrinsically shorter duration, making high vowels less variable through time. This perceptual link is proportionate to the total amount of variation in vowel inherent spectral change (VISC), which corresponds to spectro-temporal variation in formant centre frequencies through time in vowel realisations. Nasal rimes give rise to a smaller proportion of correct responses than non-nasal rimes, especially in the context of high and low front vowels: the VISC and changes in vowel height undergone in the context of such articulations, as well as the phonetic consequences of the overall articulatory constellation shape the resulting percept. CVCs with non-nasal rimes give rise to more correct responses than CVVs, despite there being more articulations on-going: the shortness of the vowel in CVCs compensates for this deficit, making perception more robust. Word frequency does not have a significant effect on recognition for any of the syllable types investigated. Overall, a much larger temporal window than the phoneme is required for the robust processing and perceptual integration of speech. Phonemes alone cannot adequately define how the relationship between the phonetic co- extensiveness of different sounds and feature sharing is to be accounted for in speech understanding. Since articulators are in constant motion during production, and consonantal gestures have distinctive coarticulatory influences over vocalic ones, the formant frequencies for both types of sound are in constant flux. This variation reinforces perceptual cohesion and has systematic effects on the mapping of FPD, through which larger structures become audible. 3 Table of Contents Abstract __________________________________________ 2 List of Figures ______________________________________ 8 List of Tables ______________________________________ 15 Preface __________________________________________ 17 Acknowledgements ________________________________ 20 Author’s Declaration _______________________________ 21 1. Background ______________________________________ 22 1.1 Studying the Perception of Coarticulation within the Context of Vowel Recognition from Aspiration ___________________ 22 1.2 Research Questions _______________________________ 27 1.2.1 Research Questions: Generalisations on this Study and its Relationship with Previous Research ________________________ 30 1.3 General Accounts of Coarticulation __________________ 32 1.3.1 A Non-Segmental Structural Definition of Coarticulation: Phonetic vs. Phonological Aspects __________________________ 43 1.4 Influences on this Study and its Theoretical Rationale: Theories and Approaches to Coarticulation _______________ 48 1.4.1 FPA and DP ________________________________________ 48 1.4.2 Polysp ____________________________________________ 51 1.4.3 The Contributions of Previous Theories: Explaining Vowel Timing Non-Segmentally __________________________________ 55 1.5 The Application and Use of Terminology in this Study ___ 58 2. Literature Review ________________________________ 66 2.1 Vowel Timing ____________________________________ 67 2.1.1 Timing Information: VISC _____________________________ 67 2.1.2 Articulatory-Phonetic Timing _________________________ 72 2.1.3 Functional Timing (Information Encoding) _______________ 75 4 2.1.4 Non-Linearity in Vowel Perception: Order Effects and Perceptual Confusions____________________________________ 77 2.2 Vowel Timing and Aspiration in English CV(V)/Cs _______ 79 2.2.1 The Relationship of Contrast and Representation to Recognition ____________________________________________ 79 2.2.2 Structural Variation and Vowel Recognition _____________ 84 2.2.3 FPD and Coarticulatory Direction Effects ________________ 90 2.2.4 Long-Domain Coarticulation and Airflow in CV(C)s ________ 94 2.3 The Phonological Treatment of Vowel Recognition _____ 107 2.3.1 A Formal Model for Reconciling Inconsistent Findings on Vowel Recognition Timing: Units and Devices Available _______ 107 2.4 An Evaluation of the Methods of Earlier Studies _______ 114 2.5 Secondary Research Questions _____________________ 118 2.6 Hypotheses _____________________________________ 122 3. Methodology __________________________________ 127 3.1. Overview ______________________________________ 127 3.2 Experimental Design and Rationale _________________ 127 3.2.1 Justifications for Choosing the Gating Paradigm ______ 127 3.3 Participants ____________________________________ 131 3.3.1 Speakers _________________________________________ 131 3.3.2 Listeners _________________________________________ 132 3.3.3 Method of Recruiting Participants ____________________ 133 3.4 Materials ______________________________________ 134 3.4.1 Stimuli and Stimulus Structure _______________________ 134 3.5 Implementing the Design and Experiment ____________ 138 3.5.1 Recordings _______________________________________ 138 3.5.2 Stimulus Segmentation _____________________________ 139 3.5.3 Stimulus Presentation and Procedure _________________ 142 3.6 Analysis Method ________________________________ 144 3.6.1 General Phonetic Aspects of Plosives and the Aperiodic Phase _____________________________________________________ 144 5 3.6.2 Plosive-Vowel Transitions in Unaspirated Plosive-Vowel CVs and in Aperiodic Noise __________________________________ 146 3.6.3 On the Phonetic Properties of Aspiration and Accompanying Formant Transitions into Vowels __________________________ 149 3.6.4 Spectro-Temporal Analysis of Production Timing ________ 152 3.6.5 Statistical Methods and External Analyses of Perception Timing ________________________________________________ 156 4. Results ________________________________________ 158 4.1 Overview ______________________________________ 158 4.2. Vowel Timing and Aspiration in CV(V)/C Production ___ 159 4.2.1 Temporal Dynamics and VISC – the Evolution of Spectral Information in Production _______________________________ 160 4.2.2 FPD and Coarticulatory Direction Effects _______________ 173 4.2.3 Long-Domain Coarticulation and Airflow: ______________ 178 Phonetic Exponency and Structure for [ + Nasal ] Stimuli ______ 178 4.3 Vowel Timing and Aspiration in CV(V)/C Perception ____ 182 4.3.1 Temporal Dynamics and VISC: the Evolution of Spectral Information in Vowel Recognition _________________________ 182 4.3.2 FPD and Coarticulatory Direction Effects _______________ 183 4.3.3 Long-Domain Coarticulation and Airflow _______________ 185 4.4 Differences in Vowel Recognition Relating to Nasality __ 186 4.5 Perceptual Confusions and Vowel Length ____________ 190 4.5.1 Overall Values Across Time __________________________ 191 4.5.2 An Examination of the Results between Gates __________ 192 4.6 Lexical Frequency ________________________________ 197 4.7 A Summary of the Results Presented in Chapter 4 ______ 203 4.7.1 Production Results _________________________________ 203 4.7.2 Perception Results _________________________________ 204 5. Recognising and Building Representations for Vowels through Time ____________________________________ 207 5.1 Overview ______________________________________ 207 5.2 Extending Our Understanding of the Perception of Coarticulation and Vowel Recognition __________________ 209 6 5.2.1 A Re-examination of the Hypotheses Presented in Chapter 2 _____________________________________________________ 209 5.2.2 Reconciling the Aims and Results of this Study __________ 212 5.2.3 Main Findings _____________________________________ 213 5.2.4 The Way Recognition Evolves Through Time ____________ 215 5.2.5 Contrast, Representation and Vowel Recognition ________ 217 5.2.6 FPD and Coarticulatory Direction Effects _______________ 217 5.2.7 Phonological/Syllable Structure ______________________ 218 5.2.8 Long-Domain Coarticulation and Airflow _______________ 218 5.3 General Aspects of a Model of Vowel Recognition _____ 219 5.3 Projecting Vowel and Syllable Structures Step-by-Step Using Incremental Dynamic Information _________________________ 230 5.3.1 Example: abstraction of ‘pea’ ________________________ 231 5.3.2 Abstraction at Time Slot 2 (Burst Transient with 10ms Vowel Resonance) ____________________________________________ 234 5.3.3 Abstraction at Time Slot 3 (Plosive Burst with 20ms Accompanying Vowel Resonance) _________________________ 240 5.3.4 Abstraction at Time slot 4 (burst transient + 30ms vowel resonance) ____________________________________________
Recommended publications
  • LINGUISTICS 221 LECTURE #3 the BASIC SOUNDS of ENGLISH 1. STOPS a Stop Consonant Is Produced with a Complete Closure of Airflow
    LINGUISTICS 221 LECTURE #3 Introduction to Phonetics and Phonology THE BASIC SOUNDS OF ENGLISH 1. STOPS A stop consonant is produced with a complete closure of airflow in the vocal tract; the air pressure has built up behind the closure; the air rushes out with an explosive sound when released. The term plosive is also used for oral stops. ORAL STOPS: e.g., [b] [t] (= plosives) NASAL STOPS: e.g., [m] [n] (= nasals) There are three phases of stop articulation: i. CLOSING PHASE (approach or shutting phase) The articulators are moving from an open state to a closed state; ii. CLOSURE PHASE (= occlusion) Blockage of the airflow in the oral tract; iii. RELEASE PHASE Sudden reopening; it may be accompanied by a burst of air. ORAL STOPS IN ENGLISH a. BILABIAL STOPS: The blockage is made with the two lips. spot [p] voiceless baby [b] voiced 1 b. ALVEOLAR STOPS: The blade (or the tip) of the tongue makes a closure with the alveolar ridge; the sides of the tongue are along the upper teeth. lamino-alveolar stops or Check your apico-alveolar stops pronunciation! stake [t] voiceless deep [d] voiced c. VELAR STOPS: The closure is between the back of the tongue (= dorsum) and the velum. dorso-velar stops scar [k] voiceless goose [g] voiced 2. NASALS (= nasal stops) The air is stopped in the oral tract, but the velum is lowered so that the airflow can go through the nasal tract. All nasals are voiced. NASALS IN ENGLISH a. BILABIAL NASAL: made [m] b. ALVEOLAR NASAL: need [n] c.
    [Show full text]
  • The Violability of Backness in Retroflex Consonants
    The violability of backness in retroflex consonants Paul Boersma University of Amsterdam Silke Hamann ZAS Berlin February 11, 2005 Abstract This paper addresses remarks made by Flemming (2003) to the effect that his analysis of the interaction between retroflexion and vowel backness is superior to that of Hamann (2003b). While Hamann maintained that retroflex articulations are always back, Flemming adduces phonological as well as phonetic evidence to prove that retroflex consonants can be non-back and even front (i.e. palatalised). The present paper, however, shows that the phonetic evidence fails under closer scrutiny. A closer consideration of the phonological evidence shows, by making a principled distinction between articulatory and perceptual drives, that a reanalysis of Flemming’s data in terms of unviolated retroflex backness is not only possible but also simpler with respect to the number of language-specific stipulations. 1 Introduction This paper is a reply to Flemming’s article “The relationship between coronal place and vowel backness” in Phonology 20.3 (2003). In a footnote (p. 342), Flemming states that “a key difference from the present proposal is that Hamann (2003b) employs inviolable articulatory constraints, whereas it is a central thesis of this paper that the constraints relating coronal place to tongue-body backness are violable”. The only such constraint that is violable for Flemming but inviolable for Hamann is the constraint that requires retroflex coronals to be articulated with a back tongue body. Flemming expresses this as the violable constraint RETRO!BACK, or RETRO!BACKCLO if it only requires that the closing phase of a retroflex consonant be articulated with a back tongue body.
    [Show full text]
  • English Phonetic Vowel Shortening and Lengthening As Perceptually Active for the Poles
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE Title: English phonetic vowel shortening and lengthening as perceptually active for the Poles Author: Arkadiusz Rojczyk Citation style: Rojczyk Arkadiusz. (2007). English phonetic vowel shortening and lengthening as perceptually active for the Poles. W: J. Arabski (red.), "On foreign language acquisition and effective learning" (S. 237-249). Katowice : Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Śląskiego Phonological subsystem English phonetic vowel shortening and lengthening as perceptually active for the Poles Arkadiusz Rojczyk University of Silesia, Katowice 1. Introduction Auditory perception is one of the most thriving and active domains of psy­ cholinguistics. Recent years have witnessed manifold attempts to investigate and understand how humans perceive linguistic sounds and what principles govern the process. The major assumption underlying the studies on auditory perception is a simple fact that linguistic sounds are means of conveying meaning, encoded by a speaker, to a hearer. The speaker, via articulatory gestures, transmits a message encoded in acoustic waves. This is the hearer, on the other hand, who, using his perceptual apparatus, reads acoustic signals and decodes them into the meaning. Therefore, in phonetic terms, the whole communicative act can be divided into three stages. Articulatory - the speaker encodes the message by sound production. Acoustic - the message is transmitted to the hearer as acous­ tic waves. Auditory - the hearer perceives sounds and decodes them into the meaning. Each of the three stages is indispensable for effective communication. Of the three aforementioned stages of communication, the auditory percep­ tion is the least amenable to precise depiction and understanding.
    [Show full text]
  • Part 1: Introduction to The
    PREVIEW OF THE IPA HANDBOOK Handbook of the International Phonetic Association: A guide to the use of the International Phonetic Alphabet PARTI Introduction to the IPA 1. What is the International Phonetic Alphabet? The aim of the International Phonetic Association is to promote the scientific study of phonetics and the various practical applications of that science. For both these it is necessary to have a consistent way of representing the sounds of language in written form. From its foundation in 1886 the Association has been concerned to develop a system of notation which would be convenient to use, but comprehensive enough to cope with the wide variety of sounds found in the languages of the world; and to encourage the use of thjs notation as widely as possible among those concerned with language. The system is generally known as the International Phonetic Alphabet. Both the Association and its Alphabet are widely referred to by the abbreviation IPA, but here 'IPA' will be used only for the Alphabet. The IPA is based on the Roman alphabet, which has the advantage of being widely familiar, but also includes letters and additional symbols from a variety of other sources. These additions are necessary because the variety of sounds in languages is much greater than the number of letters in the Roman alphabet. The use of sequences of phonetic symbols to represent speech is known as transcription. The IPA can be used for many different purposes. For instance, it can be used as a way to show pronunciation in a dictionary, to record a language in linguistic fieldwork, to form the basis of a writing system for a language, or to annotate acoustic and other displays in the analysis of speech.
    [Show full text]
  • SSC: the Science of Talking
    SSC: The Science of Talking (for year 1 students of medicine) Week 3: Sounds of the World’s Languages (vowels and consonants) Michael Ashby, Senior Lecturer in Phonetics, UCL PLIN1101 Introduction to Phonetics and Phonology A Lecture 4 page 1 Vowel Description Essential reading: Ashby & Maidment, Chapter 5 4.1 Aim: To introduce the basics of vowel description and the main characteristics of the vowels of RP English. 4.2 Definition of vowel: Vowels are produced without any major obstruction of the airflow; the intra-oral pressure stays low, and vowels are therefore sonorant sounds. Vowels are normally voiced. Vowels are articulated by raising some part of the tongue body (that is the front or the back of the tongue notnot the tip or blade) towards the roof of the oral cavity (see Figure 1). 4.3 Front vowels are produced by raising the front of the tongue towards the hard palate. Back vowels are produced by raising the back of the tongue towards the soft palate. Central vowels are produced by raising the centre part of the tongue towards the junction of the hard and soft palates. 4.4 The height of a vowel refers to the degree of raising of the relevant part of the tongue. If the tongue is raised so as to be close to the roof of the oral cavity then a close or high vowel is produced. If the tongue is only slightly raised, so that there is a wide gap between its highest point and the roof of the oral cavity, then an open or lowlowlow vowel results.
    [Show full text]
  • Word Study Guide for Parents
    Descriptions of Word Study Patterns/Concepts Purpose of Word Study • It teaches students to examine words to discover the regularities, patterns, and conventions of the English language in order to read and spell. • It increases specific knowledge of words – the spelling and meaning of individual words. 3 Layers of word study • Alphabet – learning the relationship between letters and sounds • Pattern – learning specific groupings of letters and their sounds • Meaning – learning the meaning of groups of letters such as prefixes, suffixes, and roots. Vocabulary increases at this layer. Sorting – organizing words into groups based on similarities in their patterns or meaning. Oddballs – words that cannot be grouped into any of the identified categories of a sort. Students should be taught that there are always words that “break the rules” and do not follow the general pattern. Sound marks / / - Sound marks around a letter or pattern tell the student to focus only on the sound rather than the actual letters. (example: the word gem could be grouped into the /j/ category because it sounds like j at the beginning). Vowel (represented by V) – one of 6 letters causing the mouth to open when vocalized (a, e, i, o, u, and usually y). A single vowel sound is heard in every syllable of a word. Consonants (represented by C) – all letters other than the vowels. Consonant sounds are blocked by the lips, tongue, or teeth during articulation. Concepts in Order of Instruction Initial and final consonants – students are instructed to look at pictures and identify the beginning sound. They then connect the beginning sound to the letter that makes that sound.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 9 Consonant Substitution in Child Language (Ikwere) Roseline I
    Chapter 9 Consonant substitution in child language (Ikwere) Roseline I. C. Alerechi University of Port Harcourt The Ikwere language is spoken in four out of the twenty-three Local Government Areas (LGAs) of Rivers State of Nigeria, namely, Port Harcourt, Obio/Akpor, Emohua and Ikwerre LGAs. Like Kana, Kalabari and Ekpeye, it is one of the major languages of Rivers State of Nigeria used in broadcasting in the electronic media. The Ikwere language is classified asan Igboid language of the West Benue-Congo family of the Niger-Congo phylum of languages (Williamson 1988: 67, 71, Williamson & Blench 2000: 31). This paper treats consonant substi- tution in the speech of the Ikwere child. It demonstrates that children use of a language can contribute to the divergent nature of that language as they always strive for simplification of the target language. Using simple descriptive method of data analysis, the paper identifies the various substitutions of consonant sounds, which characterize the Ikwere children’s ut- terances. It stresses that the substitutions are regular and rule governed and hence implies the operation of some phonological processes. Some of the processes are strengthening and weakening of consonants, loss of suction of labial implosives causing them to become labial plosives, devoicing of voiced consonants, etc. While some of these processes are identical with the adult language, others are peculiar to children, demonstrating the relationships between the phonological processes in both forms of speech. It is worthy of note that high- lighting the relationships and differences will make for effective communication between children and adults. 1 Introduction The Ikwere language is spoken in four out of the twenty-three Local Government Areas (LGAs) of Rivers State of Nigeria, namely, Port Harcourt, Obio/Akpor, Emohua and Ik- werre LGAs.
    [Show full text]
  • Locutour Guide to Letters, Sounds, and Symbols
    LOCUTOUROUR® Guide to Letters, Sounds, and Symbols LOCUTOUR ABEL LASSIFICATION LACE OR RTICULATION PELLED AS XAMPLES S PELLINGP E L L I N G L C P A IPA S E Consonants p bilabial plosive voiceless lips /p/ p, pp pit, puppy b bilabial plosive voiced lips /b/ b, bb bat, ebb t lingua-alveolar plosive voiceless tongue tip + upper gum ridge /t/ t, ed, gth, th, tt, tw tan, tipped, tight, thyme, attic, two d lingua-alveolar plosive voiced tongue tip + upper gum ridge /d/ d, dd, ed dad, ladder, bagged k lingua-velar plosive voiceless back of tongue and soft palate /k/ c, cc, ch, ck cab, occur, school, duck que, k mosque, kit g lingua-velar plosive voiced back of tongue and soft palate /g/ g, gg, gh, gu, gue hug, bagged, Ghana, guy, morgue f labiodental fricative voiceless lower lip + upper teeth /f/ f, ff , gh, ph feet, fl u uff, enough, phone v labiodental fricative voiced lower lip + upper teeth /v/ v, vv, f, ph vet, savvy, of, Stephen th linguadental fricative voiceless tongue + teeth /T/ th thin th linguadental fricative voiced tongue + teeth /D/ th, the them, bathe s lingua-alveolar fricative voiceless tongue tip + upper gum ridge or /s/ s, ss, sc, ce, ci, sit, hiss, scenic, ace, city tongue tip + lower gum ridge cy, ps, z cycle, psychology, pizza z lingua-alveolar fricative voiced tongue tip + upper gum ridge or /z/ z, zz, s, ss, x, cz Zen, buzz, is, scissors, tongue tip + lower gum ridge Xerox, czar sh linguapalatal fricative voiceless tongue blade and hard palate /S/ sh, ce, ch, ci, sch, si, sheep, ocean, chef, glacier, kirsch ss, su,
    [Show full text]
  • Vowel Articulation
    1 Linguistics 580 Phonetics Vowel Articulation Goals (1) to develop some familiarity with the relationship between articulatory parameters relevant to the production of vowels and their acoustic consequences. (2) to learn how to distinguish particular vowels in terms of articulatory parameters. Model We will use a model of the vocal tract called CASY (Configurable Articulatory Synthesis) to adjust the positions of vocal tract articulators. From those positions, a 3D vocal tract shape is calculated (2D midsagittal version is plotted), the resonances of that tube are calculated (from the physics), a filter function is calculated and is used to filter a glottal-like buzz. The resulting sound can be played. The articulators that can be moved are shown by little '+' markers on the Vocal Tract Display. They are labelled the figure below. 2 The articulators can be moved by positioning the mouse pointer over one of these markers, then holding down the button and dragging it to a new position. As you are dragging, the articulator will move and the vocal tract shape will be updated appropriately. A new filter function and formants will also be displayed, and a new sound can be played. The articulator positions are defined relative to one another in a dependence hierarchy. The lower lip, tongue body, and tongue tip are dependent on the position of the jaw, so when the jaw moves, these articulators more along with it. The tongue tip is dependent on the position of the tongue body. The upper lip is dependent on the position of the lower lip. For this exercise with vowels, you should be able to produce decent approximations to English vowels by manipulating only the jaw, tongue body, and lips.
    [Show full text]
  • Studies in African Linguistics Volume 21, Number 3, December 1990
    Studies in African Linguistics Volume 21, Number 3, December 1990 CONTEXTUAL LABIALIZATION IN NA WURI* Roderic F. Casali Ghana Institute of Linguistics Literacy and Bible Translation and UCLA A spectrographic investigation into the non-contrastive labialization of consonants before round vowels in Nawuri (a Kwa language of Ghana) sup­ ports the notion that this labialization is the result of a phonological, feature­ spreading rule and not simply an automatic transitional process. This as­ sumption is further warranted in that it allows for a more natural treatment of some other phonological processes in the language. The fact that labial­ ization before round vowels is generally not very audible is explained in terms of a principle of speech perception. A final topic addressed is the question of why (both in Nawuri and apparently in a number of other Ghanaian languages as well) contextual labialization does tend to be more perceptible in certain restricted environments. o. Introduction This paper deals with the allophonic labialization of consonants before round vowels in Nawuri, a Kwa language of Ghana.! While such labialization is gener­ ally not very audible, spectrographic evidence suggests that it is strongly present, * The spectrograms in this study were produced at the phonetics lab of the University of Texas at Arlington using equipment provided through a grant of the Permanent University Fund of the University of Texas system. I would like to thank the following people for their valuable comments and suggestions: Joan Baart, Don Burquest, Mike Cahill, Jerry Edmondson, Norris McKinney, Bob Mugele, Tony Naden, and Keith Snider. I would also like to express my appreciation to Russell Schuh and an anonymous referee for this journal for their helpful criticism of an earlier version, and to Mary Steele for some helpful discussion concerning labialization in Konkomba.
    [Show full text]
  • Bradley 2015 Labialization and Palatalization in Judeo-Spanish
    LABIALIZATION AND PALATALIZATION IN JUDEO-SPANISH PHONOLOGY* TRAVIS G. BRADLEY University of California, Davis Judeo-Spanish (JS) presents a number of phonological processes involving secondary articulations. This paper establishes novel descriptive generalizations based on labialization and palatalization phenomena across different JS dialects. I show how these generalizations are part of a broader cross-linguistic typology of secondary articulation patterns, which would remain incomplete on the basis of non-Sephardic Spanish alone. I propose an analysis in Optimality Theory (OT) that accounts for this variation using the same universal constraints that are active in languages beyond Ibero-Romance. This paper demonstrates the utility of OT as an analytical framework for doing JS phonology and in turn highlights the importance of JS to phonological theory by bringing new generalizations and data to bear on the analysis of secondary articulations. 1. Introduction Judeo-Spanish (henceforth, JS) refers to those varieties of Spanish preserved by the Sephardic Jews who were expelled from Spain in 1492 and have emigrated throughout Europe, North Africa, the Middle East, and later, the United States. JS is intriguing to study from both diachronic and synchronic perspectives. The linguistic system retains many archaic features, which gives us a unique, albeit indirect, window into the structure of Old Spanish (henceforth, OS). Having evolved in relative isolation from the linguistic norms of the Iberian Peninsula and often in contact with other
    [Show full text]
  • Third Guttural Weak Verbs
    Chapter Twenty-Five Third Guttural Weak Verbs Vocabulary [:l'B to swallow, be consumed [:g'P to meet, intercede h'm'B high place (f) x:t'P to open [:q'B to split open #Eq end H:b'G to be high, proud [:r'q to tare [:r'z to sow x:c'r to kill [:r'K to knell, bow down !'x.lUv table x:v'm to anoint v,m,v sun x:l's to forgive tAd.lAT generations (f) Weak Verb Designations III II I ← Modern designation Modern Description l :[ 'P ← Traditional 1 d :m '[ Pe Guttural I - Guttural Guttural in 1st root position 2 l :a 'v ‘Ayin Guttural II - Guttural Guttural in 2nd root position 3 x :l 'v Lamed Guttural III - Guttural Guttural in 3rd root position 4 l :k 'a Pe ‘Alef I – ‘Alef ‘Alef in 1st root position 5 a 'c 'm Lamed ‘Alef III- ‘Alef ‘Alef in 3rd root position 6 h 'n 'B Lamed He III - He He in 3rd root position 7 l :p 'n Pe Nun I - Nun Nun in 1st root position 8 b W v ‘Ayin Vav or Yod II – Vav or Yod II – Vav/Yod 9 b :v 'y Pe Vav or Yod I - Vav or Yod I – Vav/Yod 10 b :b 's Double ‘Ayin Geminate Third Guttural Weak Verbs A Third Guttural verb designated as Lamed Guttural or III-Guttural is one whose third or final root consonant is one of the limited gutturals: H, x, or [. Final a, h , or r do not act as gutturals when placed in the final root consonant.
    [Show full text]