Psychoacoustic Evaluation of Musical Sounds

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Psychoacoustic Evaluation of Musical Sounds Perception & Psychophysics 1978, Vol. 23 (6), 483-492 Psychoacoustic evaluation of musical sounds ERNST TERHARDT InstituteofElectroacoustics, Technical University ofMunchen Arcisstrasse 21, D-8000 Miinchen 2, WestGermany A review is given of several basic aspects of musical sounds, i.e., the perception of dura­ tion, the perception of sound impulses as events within temporal patterns, timbre, the equiv­ alence of sensational intervals, roughness, and the pitch qualities of complex sounds. Selected examples illustrate how psychoacoustic results can contribute to the evaluation of musical sounds and to the understanding of the perception of music. Music is a "language" whose acoustic realization DURATION OF TONES usually is prescribed much more precisely and rigidly than is the case with other auditory signals, including A simple parameter of a single musical sound is speech. In particular, the frequencies of musical its duration; corresponding to the physical duration tones and the time pattern in which they are realized is a psychoacoustic attribute, called subjective are essential and significant carriers of information, duration. The relation between these two parameters But the ultimate receiver of this information is the is not as simple as may be expected at first sight auditory system. It is not the physical sound param­ (cf. Zwicker, 1970). See Figure 1 for an example in eters as such (e.g., frequency and temporal envelope some experimental results ofBurghardt (1973). of tones) that are the decisive criteria of musical per­ In one experiment, Burghardt presented alternately formance, but the corresponding auditory qualities a I-kHz tone and a 200-Hz tone in endless succession, (e.g., subjective duration, rhythm, timbre, roughness, and made the subjects adjust the physical duration of pitch, harmony). Hence, the quantitative relations one of the tones in such a way that successive tones between stimulus parameters and auditory sensations were perceived as having the same apparent duration. are of particular importance in the theory as well as Care was taken to eliminate methodological effects. the realization of music. It is particularly in the elec­ The relation between the physical durations of the tronic realization of music, where the physical two tones (T, kHz and Tf, with f = 200 Hz) under parameters of musical stimuli in many cases are the condition of equal subjective durations is shown specified numerically without auditory feedback, by the curve marked f = 200 Hz. The departure of that it is desirable to know as precisely as possible this curve from the dashed line indicates that the how acoustic stimuli are transformed into auditory 200-Hz tone has to be presented with a distinctly sensations. Moreover, careful analysis of those greater physical duration than the I-kHz tone to be psychoacoustic transformations may provide val­ perceived with the same apparent duration. uable theoretical insight into such basic musical In another experiment, a I-kHz tone and a 3,200 Hz phenomena as, e.g., consonance and harmony. tone were matched in duration in the same way. The The present contribution reflects the author's result is shown also in Figure 1. Clearly, subjective endeavor to proceed into that part of musical per­ duration of pure tones depends not only on physical ception. Several examples of psychoacoustic evalua­ duration, but also on frequency. When the physical tion, selected from the author's and his colleqgues' duration of a pure tone is held constant, its subjective own work, will be discussed. The study may be useful duration is distinctly diminished by decreasing fre­ as an introduction into an interesting aspect of quency. It should be noticed that the range of dura­ psychophysics, a guide to many experimental results, tions in which the effect is significant is almost iden­ and a stimulus for further research. It is not intended tical with the range of durations covered by the to present a comprehensive review of contemporary majority of musical tones (durations of less than results. about 800 msec). The essential content of this contribution was orally presented With some caution, these results may be applied at the Workshop on Physical and Neuropsychological Foundations to real musical tones, which usually are complex of Music, Ossiach/Austria, July 18·22, 1977. The described tones, i.e., tones composed of several harmonics. research of the present author was carried out in the Sonderfor­ Experimental data on the subjective duration of schungsbereich Kybernetik, Munchen, supported by the Deutsche complex tones are not available as yet, but one may Forschungsgemeinschaft. The author is indebted to H. Fastl, who read the manuscript and made valuable suggestions. Also, the infer rather safely that the subjective duration of a helpful comments given by the two referees of Perception and complex tone will be determined by its dominant Psychophysics on a first draft are gratefully acknowledged. harmonics. For example, it may be predicted that a 483 484 TERHARDT .1000.----,-----,--------.. tions (5-400 msec), and combinations of envelope shapes; the tone frequency was around 3 kHz. Under the condition of subjective equability, the ms time interval between the onsets of two successive tones was in many cases not T12 as might be expected, but systematically different from this value by a cer­ r 100 tain departure M, as indicated and defined in Figure 2. With rectangularly shaped tones of different dura­ tion (as in Figure 2A), M was maximally found to be 7f 20 msec. With tone combinations similar to the 10 example in Figure 2B, the average M value attained about 60 msec. It was found that these values were almost independent of T. Moreover, the results were almost independent of sound pressure level and tone 21L.------'-------'--------' (or sound) spectrum (cf. also Schutte, 1978a, 1978b). 2 10 100 ms 1000 Similar results, which agree well with these observa­ 71kHz- tions, have recently been published by Morton, Figure 1. Duration T, of a tone with frequency f = 200 and Marcus, & Frankish (1976). 3,200 Hz, respectively, as a function of the durauon of T1kHz of a I-kHz tone, for equal subjective duration; SPL 60 dB (from TIMBRE OF SLIGHTLY Burghardt, 1973). INHARMONIC COMPLEX TONES complex tone of a certain physical duration, with The timbre of a steady sound is to some extent 200-Hz fundamental frequency, and many strong dependent on the spectral envelope (i.e., the gross harmonics up to about the 20th will possess approxi­ spectral distribution of intensity), for example the mately the subjective duration typical of a high pure third-octave-band spectrum (cf., e.g., Plomp, Pols, tone (frequency of some kHz) instead of the (shorter) & Van de Geer, 1967; von Bismarck, 1974). But a subjective duration typical of a pure 200-Hz tone. considerable part of timbre is not specified by the spectral envelope. Rather, the precise frequencies of EQUABILITY OF TONE SEQUENCES the part tones covered by a given envelope also play a role. In electronic organs, for example, synthesis In music, the rhythmic relations between successive of complex tones by superposition of pure tones tones are an important (and usually carefully pre­ which are taken from the equally tempered scale is scribed) feature. A tone sequence is defined as being a usual method. In these synthesized complex tones, subjectively equable when the successive events only those partials which are one or more octaves which are determined by the single tones are per­ above the fundamental are "true" harmonics; the ceived as being uniformly arranged in time. It is other partials are slightly inharmonic. For example, usual to consider a tone sequence as "equable" or "uniform" when the physical onsets of the single impulses are arranged equidistantly on the physical 1-------- T time scale. This is an arbitrary definition of equabil­ Tl2~T12 ity, which is fairly compatible with common auditory f experience. Yet it was considered as sensible to check its psychoacoustic validity (Terhardt & Schutte, 1976). Figure 2 illustrates the experimental procedure with two examples (A, B). A sequence of tones was presented, alternately composed of two tones with different durations and/or temporal envelopes. In case A, both successive tones were rectangularly shaped, but different in duration. In case B, the two tones additionally were different in shape of envelope. The time interval between each pair of identical tones, T, was constant. The time interval between the suc­ cessive (i.e., nonidentical) tones was varied until the Figure 2. Two examples (A, B) of tone sequences which are perceived as equable (uniform); schematic amplitude-time pattern, criterion of subjective equability was fulfilled. The The sequences are periodic with period T. The departure lit experiments were carried out with various T values indicates that perceived equability is not identical with equal time (0.2 sec ~ T ~ 2 sec), combinations of tone dura- intervals between physical onsets. PSYCHOACOUSTIC EVALUATION OF MUSICAL SOUNDS 485 the 3rd partial differs from the true 3rd harmonic there is no timbre difference, because both complex by - 2 cents, the 5th partial from the true 5th har­ tones of pair A are identical in this case. The inhar­ monic by + 14 cents. Though the relative amplitude monicity effect comes into play when n = 3. With of each partial may be controlled in a wide range, n > 3, the difference increases rapidly, attaining thus providing many distinctly different spectral saturation. envelopes, the sound of those electronic organ tones In electronic organs, there usually is n ~ 6. With is somewhat uniform and specific. The timbre of a n = 6, the effect of inharmonicity has reached complex tone which is synthesized in the described almost its maximally possible magnitude, which is way is more or less different from that of a truly equivalent to the timbre difference between a single harmonic complex tone comprising the same number pure tone and the same tone "enriched" by six addi­ of partials with the same amplitudes.
Recommended publications
  • Virtual Pitch and Pitch Shifts in Church Bells
    Open Journal of Acoustics, 2017, 7, 52-68 http://www.scirp.org/journal/oja ISSN Online: 2162-5794 ISSN Print: 2162-5786 Virtual Pitch and Pitch Shifts in Church Bells William A. Hibbert, Shahram Taherzadeh, David B. Sharp School of Engineering and Innovation, Open University, Milton Keynes, UK How to cite this paper: Hibbert, W.A., Abstract Taherzadeh, S. and Sharp, D.B. (2017) Virtual Pitch and Pitch Shifts in Church It is well established that musical sounds comprising multiple partials with Bells. Open Journal of Acoustics, 7, 52-68. frequencies approximately in the ratio of small integers give rise to a strong https://doi.org/10.4236/oja.2017.73006 sensation of pitch even if the lowest or fundamental partial is missing—the so-called virtual pitch effect. Experiments on thirty test subjects demonstrate Received: August 3, 2017 Accepted: September 5, 2017 that this virtual pitch is shifted significantly by changes in the spacing of the Published: September 8, 2017 constituent partials. The experiments measured pitch by comparison of sounds of similar timbre and were automated so that they could be performed Copyright © 2017 by authors and remotely across the Internet. Analysis of the test sounds used shows that the Scientific Research Publishing Inc. This work is licensed under the Creative pitch shifts are not predicted by Terhardt’s classic model of virtual pitch. The Commons Attribution International test sounds used were modelled on the sounds of church bells, but a further License (CC BY 4.0). experiment on seventeen test subjects showed that changes in partial ampli- http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ tude only had a minor effect on the pitch shifts observed, and that a pitch shift Open Access was still observed when two of the lowest frequency partials were removed, so that the effects reported are of general interest.
    [Show full text]
  • Cell Phones Silent Clickers on Remember - Learning Team – You Can Email/Skype/Facetime/Zoom in Virtual Office Hours
    This is PHYS 1240 - Sound and Music Lecture 11 Professor Patricia Rankin Cell Phones silent Clickers on Remember - Learning Team – you can email/skype/facetime/zoom in virtual office hours Graduate student Teaching Assistant Tyler C Mcmaken - online 10-11am Friday Undergraduate Learning Assistants Madeline Karr Online 6-7pm Monday Rishi Mayekar Online 11-12noon Thur Miles Warnke Online 3-4pm Wed Professor Patricia Rankin Online 2-3pm Wed Physics 1240 Lecture 11 Today: Timbre, Fourier, Sound Spectra, Sampling Size Next time: Sound Intensity, Intervals, Scales physicscourses.colorado.edu/phys1240 Canvas Site: assignments, administration, grades Homework – HW5 Due Wed February 19th 5pm Homelabs – Hlab3 Due Monday Feb 24th 5pm Debrief – Last Class(es) Waves in pipes Open-open n=1,2,3,4 Closed-open n = 1,3,5 Pressure nodes at open ends, pressure antinodes at closed ends Displacement nodes/antinodes opposite to pressure ones. Open-open 푓푛=푛∙푣푠/2퐿 So, fundamental doesn’t depend just on length of pipe Closed-open 푓푛=푛∙푣푠/4퐿 Homework hints Check your units You can use yx key to find 2n Be aware – you may need to do some manipulation e.g. Suppose question asks you to find the mass/unit length of a string given velocity, tension…. You know F 2 퐹 , 퐹 v 푠표, 푣푡 = 휇 = 2 t 휇 푣푡 Timbre – the tone color/voice of an instrument Timbre is why we can recognize different instruments from steady tones. Perception Physics Pitch Frequency Consonance Frequencies are ratios of small integers Loudness Overall amplitude Timbre Amplitudes of a harmonic series of notes Superposition We can add/superimpose sine waves to get a more complex wave profile Overall shape (Timbre) depends on the frequency spectra (the frequencies of waves added together), and the amplitudes of the waves Ohm's acoustic law, sometimes called the acoustic phase law or simply Ohm's law (but another Ohm’s law in Electricity and Magnetism), states that a musical sound is perceived by the ear as the fundamental note of a set of a number of constituent pure harmonic tones.
    [Show full text]
  • The Physics of Sound 1
    The Physics of Sound 1 The Physics of Sound Sound lies at the very center of speech communication. A sound wave is both the end product of the speech production mechanism and the primary source of raw material used by the listener to recover the speaker's message. Because of the central role played by sound in speech communication, it is important to have a good understanding of how sound is produced, modified, and measured. The purpose of this chapter will be to review some basic principles underlying the physics of sound, with a particular focus on two ideas that play an especially important role in both speech and hearing: the concept of the spectrum and acoustic filtering. The speech production mechanism is a kind of assembly line that operates by generating some relatively simple sounds consisting of various combinations of buzzes, hisses, and pops, and then filtering those sounds by making a number of fine adjustments to the tongue, lips, jaw, soft palate, and other articulators. We will also see that a crucial step at the receiving end occurs when the ear breaks this complex sound into its individual frequency components in much the same way that a prism breaks white light into components of different optical frequencies. Before getting into these ideas it is first necessary to cover the basic principles of vibration and sound propagation. Sound and Vibration A sound wave is an air pressure disturbance that results from vibration. The vibration can come from a tuning fork, a guitar string, the column of air in an organ pipe, the head (or rim) of a snare drum, steam escaping from a radiator, the reed on a clarinet, the diaphragm of a loudspeaker, the vocal cords, or virtually anything that vibrates in a frequency range that is audible to a listener (roughly 20 to 20,000 cycles per second for humans).
    [Show full text]
  • A Comparison of Viola Strings with Harmonic Frequency Analysis
    University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Student Research, Creative Activity, and Performance - School of Music Music, School of 5-2011 A Comparison of Viola Strings with Harmonic Frequency Analysis Jonathan Paul Crosmer University of Nebraska-Lincoln, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/musicstudent Part of the Music Commons Crosmer, Jonathan Paul, "A Comparison of Viola Strings with Harmonic Frequency Analysis" (2011). Student Research, Creative Activity, and Performance - School of Music. 33. https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/musicstudent/33 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Music, School of at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in Student Research, Creative Activity, and Performance - School of Music by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. A COMPARISON OF VIOLA STRINGS WITH HARMONIC FREQUENCY ANALYSIS by Jonathan P. Crosmer A DOCTORAL DOCUMENT Presented to the Faculty of The Graduate College at the University of Nebraska In Partial Fulfillment of Requirements For the Degree of Doctor of Musical Arts Major: Music Under the Supervision of Professor Clark E. Potter Lincoln, Nebraska May, 2011 A COMPARISON OF VIOLA STRINGS WITH HARMONIC FREQUENCY ANALYSIS Jonathan P. Crosmer, D.M.A. University of Nebraska, 2011 Adviser: Clark E. Potter Many brands of viola strings are available today. Different materials used result in varying timbres. This study compares 12 popular brands of strings. Each set of strings was tested and recorded on four violas. We allowed two weeks after installation for each string set to settle, and we were careful to control as many factors as possible in the recording process.
    [Show full text]
  • Musical Acoustics - Wikipedia, the Free Encyclopedia 11/07/13 17:28 Musical Acoustics from Wikipedia, the Free Encyclopedia
    Musical acoustics - Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia 11/07/13 17:28 Musical acoustics From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia Musical acoustics or music acoustics is the branch of acoustics concerned with researching and describing the physics of music – how sounds employed as music work. Examples of areas of study are the function of musical instruments, the human voice (the physics of speech and singing), computer analysis of melody, and in the clinical use of music in music therapy. Contents 1 Methods and fields of study 2 Physical aspects 3 Subjective aspects 4 Pitch ranges of musical instruments 5 Harmonics, partials, and overtones 6 Harmonics and non-linearities 7 Harmony 8 Scales 9 See also 10 External links Methods and fields of study Frequency range of music Frequency analysis Computer analysis of musical structure Synthesis of musical sounds Music cognition, based on physics (also known as psychoacoustics) Physical aspects Whenever two different pitches are played at the same time, their sound waves interact with each other – the highs and lows in the air pressure reinforce each other to produce a different sound wave. As a result, any given sound wave which is more complicated than a sine wave can be modelled by many different sine waves of the appropriate frequencies and amplitudes (a frequency spectrum). In humans the hearing apparatus (composed of the ears and brain) can usually isolate these tones and hear them distinctly. When two or more tones are played at once, a variation of air pressure at the ear "contains" the pitches of each, and the ear and/or brain isolate and decode them into distinct tones.
    [Show full text]
  • Musical Acoustics Timbre / Tone Quality I
    Musical Acoustics Lecture 13 Timbre / Tone quality I Musical Acoustics, C. Bertulani 1 Waves: review distance x (m) At a given time t: y = A sin(2πx/λ) A time t (s) -A At a given position x: y = A sin(2πt/T) Musical Acoustics, C. Bertulani 2 Perfect Tuning Fork: Pure Tone • As the tuning fork vibrates, a succession of compressions and rarefactions spread out from the fork • A harmonic (sinusoidal) curve can be used to represent the longitudinal wave • Crests correspond to compressions and troughs to rarefactions • only one single harmonic (pure tone) is needed to describe the wave Musical Acoustics, C. Bertulani 3 Phase δ $ x ' % x ( y = Asin& 2π ) y = Asin' 2π + δ* % λ( & λ ) Musical Acoustics, C. Bertulani 4 € € Adding waves: Beats Superposition of 2 waves with slightly different frequency The amplitude changes as a function of time, so the intensity of sound changes as a function of time. The beat frequency (number of intensity maxima/minima per second): fbeat = |fa-fb| Musical Acoustics, C. Bertulani 5 The perceived frequency is the average of the two frequencies: f + f f = 1 2 perceived 2 The beat frequency (rate of the throbbing) is the difference€ of the two frequencies: fbeats = f1 − f 2 € Musical Acoustics, C. Bertulani 6 Factors Affecting Timbre 1. Amplitudes of harmonics 2. Transients (A sudden and brief fluctuation in a sound. The sound of a crack on a record, for example.) 3. Inharmonicities 4. Formants 5. Vibrato 6. Chorus Effect Two or more sounds are said to be in unison when they are at the same pitch, although often an OCTAVE may exist between them.
    [Show full text]
  • (Autumn 2004) Pitch Perception: Place Theory, Temporal Theory, and Beyond
    EE 391 Special Report (Autumn 2004) Pitch Perception: Place Theory, Temporal Theory, and Beyond Advisor: Professor Julius Smith Kyogu Lee Center for Computer Research in Music and Acoustics (CCRMA) Music Department, Stanford University [email protected] Abstract to explain are easily explained by a temporal theory, and vice versa. Hence it would be fairer to call them complementary than competing, which may be proved by later hybrid models. Two competing theories on pitch perception are reviewed with brief history and several significant works that contributed to building such theories. Fundamental concepts behind these 2 Place Theory two theories - place theory and temporal theory - are briefly described, and the further steps their successors took are pre- The place theory has a long history which may hark back sented, followed by possible future directions. to the days of Helmholtz (von Helmholtz 1954) although most ideas can be traced back far beyond him.(de Cheveigne´ 2004). According to his resonance-place theory of hearing, the inner 1 Introduction ear acts like a frequency analyzer, and the stimulus reach- ing our ear is decomposed into many sinusoidal components, Pitch is one of the most important attributes of audio sig- each of which excites different places along the basilar mem- nals such as speech and music. In speech, pitch can greatly brane, where hair cells with distinct characteristic frequencies improve speech intelligibility and thus can be very useful are linked with neurones. He also suggested that the pitch of in speech recognition systems. Sound source separation is a stimulus is related to the pattern of the excitation produced another application where pitch information is critical espe- by the stimulus along the basilar membrane.
    [Show full text]
  • Harmonic Template Neurons in Primate Auditory Cortex Underlying Complex Sound Processing
    Harmonic template neurons in primate auditory cortex underlying complex sound processing Lei Fenga,1 and Xiaoqin Wanga,2 aLaboratory of Auditory Neurophysiology, Department of Biomedical Engineering, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21205 Edited by Dale Purves, Duke University, Durham, NC, and approved December 7, 2016 (received for review May 10, 2016) Harmonicity is a fundamental element of music, speech, and animal role in harmonic integration in processing complex sounds. Pa- vocalizations. How the auditory system extracts harmonic structures tients with unilateral temporal lobe excision showed poorer embedded in complex sounds and uses them to form a coherent performance in a missing fundamental pitch perception task than unitary entity is not fully understood. Despite the prevalence of normal listeners but performed as normal in the task when the f0 sounds rich in harmonic structures in our everyday hearing envi- was presented (21). Auditory cortex lesions could impair complex ronment, it has remained largely unknown what neural mechanisms sound discrimination, such as animal vocalizations, and vowel-like are used by the primate auditory cortex to extract these biologically sounds without affecting responses to relative simple stimuli, like important acoustic structures. In this study, we discovered a unique pure tones (22–24). class of harmonic template neurons in the core region of auditory Primate auditory cortex is subdivided into a “core” region, cortex of a highly vocal New World primate, the common marmoset consisting of A1 and the neighboring primary-like rostral (R) and (Callithrix jacchus), across the entire hearing frequency range. Mar- rostral–temporal areas, surrounded by a belt region, which contains mosets have a rich vocal repertoire and a similar hearing range to multiple distinct secondary fields (Fig.
    [Show full text]
  • Interpretations of Frequency Domain Analyses of Neural Entrainment: Periodicity, Fundamental Frequency, and Harmonics
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by MPG.PuRe HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY published: 06 June 2016 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00274 Interpretations of Frequency Domain Analyses of Neural Entrainment: Periodicity, Fundamental Frequency, and Harmonics Hong Zhou 1, Lucia Melloni 2,3, David Poeppel 4,5 and Nai Ding 1,6,7* 1 College of Biomedical Engineering and Instrument Sciences, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China, 2 Department of Neurology, New York University Langone Medical Center, New York, NY, USA, 3 Department of Neurophysiology, Max-Planck Institute for Brain Research, Frankfurt, Germany, 4 Neuroscience Department, Max-Planck Institute for Empirical Aesthetics, Frankfurt, Germany, 5 Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, USA, 6 Interdisciplinary Center for Social Sciences, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China, 7 Neuro and Behavior EconLab, Zhejiang University of Finance and Economics, Hangzhou, China Brain activity can follow the rhythms of dynamic sensory stimuli, such as speech and music, a phenomenon called neural entrainment. It has been hypothesized that low-frequency neural entrainment in the neural delta and theta bands provides a potential mechanism to represent and integrate temporal information. Low-frequency neural entrainment is often studied using periodically changing stimuli and is analyzed in the frequency domain using the Fourier analysis. The Fourier analysis decomposes a periodic signal into harmonically related sinusoids. However, it is not intuitive how these harmonically related components are related to the response waveform. Here, we explain the interpretation of response harmonics, with a special focus on very low- Edited by: frequency neural entrainment near 1 Hz.
    [Show full text]
  • Lecture 38 Quantum Theory of Light
    Lecture 38 Quantum Theory of Light 38.1 Quantum Theory of Light 38.1.1 Historical Background Quantum theory is a major intellectual achievement of the twentieth century, even though we are still discovering new knowledge in it. Several major experimental findings led to the revelation of quantum theory or quantum mechanics of nature. In nature, we know that many things are not infinitely divisible. Matter is not infinitely divisible as vindicated by the atomic theory of John Dalton (1766-1844) [221]. So fluid is not infinitely divisible: as when water is divided into smaller pieces, we will eventually arrive at water molecule, H2O, which is the fundamental building block of water. In turns out that electromagnetic energy is not infinitely divisible either. The electromag- netic radiation out of a heated cavity would obey a very different spectrum if electromagnetic energy is infinitely divisible. In order to fit experimental observation of radiation from a heated electromagnetic cavity, Max Planck (1900s) [222] proposed that electromagnetic en- ergy comes in packets or is quantized. Each packet of energy or a quantum of energy E is associated with the frequency of electromagnetic wave, namely E = ~! = ~2πf = hf (38.1.1) where ~ is now known as the Planck constant and ~ = h=2π = 6:626 × 10−34 J·s (Joule- second). Since ~ is very small, this packet of energy is very small unless ! is large. So it is no surprise that the quantization of electromagnetic field is first associated with light, a very high frequency electromagnetic radiation. A red-light photon at a wavelength of 700 −19 nm corresponds to an energy of approximately 2 eV ≈ 3 × 10 J ≈ 75 kBT , where kBT denotes the thermal energy.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychoacoustic Foundations of Contextual Harmonic Stability in Jazz Piano Voicings
    Journal of Jazz Studies vol. 7, no. 2, pp. 156–191 (Fall 2011) Psychoacoustic Foundations Of Contextual Harmonic Stability In Jazz Piano Voicings James McGowan Considerable harmonic variation of both chord types and specific voicings is available to the jazz pianist, even when playing stable, tonic-functioned chords. Of course non-tonic chords also accommodate extensive harmonic variety, but they generally cannot provide structural stability beyond the musical surface. Many instances of tonic chords, however, also provide little or no sense of structural repose when found in the middle of a phrase or subjected to some other kind of “dissonance.” The inclusion of the word “stable” is therefore important, because while many sources are implicitly aware of the fundamental differences between stable and unstable chords, little significant work explicitly accounts for an impro- vising pianist’s harmonic options as associated specifically with harmonic stability— or harmonic “consonance”—in tonal jazz. The ramifications are profound, as the very question of what constitutes a stable tonic sonority in jazz suggests that underlying precepts of tonality function differ- ently in improvised jazz and common-practice music. While some jazz pedagogical publications attempt to account for the diverse chord types and specific voicings employed as tonic chords, these sources have not provided a distinct conceptual framework that explains what criteria link these harmonic options. Some music theorists, meanwhile, have provided valuable analytical models to account for the sense of resolution to stable harmonic entities. These models, however, are largely designed for “classical” music and are problematic in that they tend to explain pervasive non-triadic harmonies as aberrant in some way.1 1 Representative sources that address “classical” models of jazz harmony include Steve Larson, Analyzing Jazz: A Schenkerian Approach, Harmonologia: studies in music theory, no.
    [Show full text]
  • Application of Virtual Pitch Theory in Music Analysis
    Application of virtual pitch theory in music analysis Llorenç Balsach Abstract In the course of this article a model of harmonic analysis is worked out based on certain properties of the auditory system, which I think will shed new light on the study of cadences and local harmonic resolutions. This model consists basically of extracting the two main fundamentals (roots) which are to be found in 93.3% of chords of less than 6 notes. To do this I apply the basic concepts of virtual pitch but taking into account only those harmonics which are in a prime position in the first seven, which in our thesis include the rest of the harmonics as far as the human auditory is concerned. With this model we found new information about the "internal" harmonic tension which is created by every single note and the tendencies of chords towards resolutions. I compare and discuss other models which apply the theory of virtual pitch in harmonic analysis. Introduction E. Terhardt's concept of "virtual pitch" is used in psycho-acoustics as a method of extracting pitch(es) from (harmonic) complex tone signals (see Terhardt, Stoll & Seewann (1982b, 1982c), Meddis & Hewitt (1991), van Immerseel & Martens (1992), Leman (1995)). The concept could be briefly described as the pitch which the auditory system perceives from a sound or group of sounds. There have been some attempts to bring the acoustic concept of virtual pitch into musical theory. Terhardt himself (1982a) applied his theory to extract the root(s) of a chord. R. Parncutt (1988) saw that the results obtained from Terhardt's model did not fit in sufficiently with conventional musical theory; he worked out a revised version of the model, producing significantly different results from Terhardt's original.
    [Show full text]