Direct Versus Indirect Tests of Memory: Directed Forgetting Meets The

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Direct Versus Indirect Tests of Memory: Directed Forgetting Meets The Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2000, 7 (2), 354-359 Direct versus indirect tests ofmemory: Directed forgetting meets the generation effect COUN M. MAcLEOD and KAREN A. DANIELS University ofToronto, Scarborough, Ontario, Canada Subjects read 20 words and generated 20 others from defInitions during a 40-item study phase. Pro­ duction ofeach word was followed by an instruction to remember or to forget that word. In free recall, a direct test ofmemory, words that had been generated were recalled much betterthan words that had been read. The remember-forget instructional manipulation affected read words but not generated words. In speeded word reading, an indirect test of memory, all studied words showed priming, but read words showed more priming than generated words. Here, the effect of remember versus forget instructions appeared only for generated words. These dissociations ofa direct and an indirect test in­ dicate that two powerful encoding manipulations affect separable processes to which these tests are differentially sensitive. The prevalent lay view ofremembering rests on the con­ have come to be called priming effects. As reviewed by scious recollection ofpast episodes. Yet even over 100 years Richardson-Klavehn and Bjork, the combined evidence ago, in his ground-breaking empirical studies, Ebbinghaus from amnesic and normal subjects reveals a number of (1885/1964) distinguished two ways ofremembering: "in­ trends, of which we will highlight two. First, direct tests voluntary recollection" and "voluntary recollection." This generally are more influenced than are indirect tests by se­ distinction can be seen as a precursorto the current concepts mantic elaboration during encoding (Graf & Mandler, ofimplicit and explicit memory (Graf& Schacter, 1985), as 1984; Jacoby & Dallas, 1981), although exceptions do differentiated by two classes ofmemory tests. Indirect tests exist (Graf& Schacter, 1985). Second, a shift in modality reveal the influence ofmemory through facilitation ofper­ from study phase to test phase (e.g., from auditory to vi­ formance in the absence ofconscious recollection; in con­ sual) has little effect on direct measures yet sharply atten­ trast, direct tests require the conscious recollection ofpre­ uates priming on indirect measures (Bassili, Smith, & vious information. As illustrations, intentionally trying to MacLeod, 1989; Roediger & Blaxton, 1987), although ex­ recollect references that we wish to cite in an article consti­ ceptions exist here as well (Jacoby & Dallas, 1981). tutes a direct use of memory, whereas fluently using the It should be noted, however, that despite such strikingdis­ many features ofour word processors without realizing that sociations between direct and indirect tests, there also are memory is being tapped is an indirect use ofmemory. significant parallel effects. As illustrations, repetition of A variety ofdissociations in performance have been ob­ stimuli appears to facilitate performance on both types of served between direct and indirect tests, suggesting that test (Jacoby & Dallas, 1981; but see Challis & Sidhu, 1993), these two ways of using memory are quite different (see and same-context responses are significantly more effec­ Richardson-Klavehn & Bjork, 1988; Roediger & McDer­ tive than are different-context responses in aiding reten­ mott, 1993, for reviews). Studies of amnesia have made tion on both types oftest (Graf& Schacter, 1985). especially important contributions. Despite their frequently Roediger (1990) has offered a transfer-appropriate pro­ catastrophic failure on direct tests ofmemory, amnesics cessing account ofthe pattern ofresults seen on direct ver­ can show normal performance on indirect tests ofmemory sus indirect tests (see also MacLeod & Masson, 1997, in (Graf& Schacter, 1985; Graf, Shimamura,& Squire, 1985; press; Masson & MacLeod, 1992). Here, indirect tests are Warrington & Weiskrantz, 1970). These unconscious ef­ seen as relying on the fluent replaying ofthe encoding of fects in indirect tests-usually expressed as a benefit con­ stimuli, typically emphasizing perceptual components of ferred on subsequent processing by previous processing- their initial analysis. Direct tests typically rely more on any elaborative encoding done during study, emphasizing se­ mantic/conceptual processing that went beyond simply an­ alyzing the stimulus in context. Thus, a modality change This research was supported by Natural Sciences and Engineering Re­ search Council ofCanada Grant A7459, and formed the senior honors from study to test should disrupt recovery of the initial thesis ofK.A.D., who is now at the Georgia Institute ofTechnology. We perceptual analysis, affecting primarily indirect tests, thank Monica Ghosal and Shelley Hodder for their assistance in col­ whereas semantic elaboration at encoding should enhance lecting the data, and Barbara Basden, John Gardiner, Pierre Perruchet, conceptual processing, affecting primarily direct tests, as and Henry Roediger III, for their helpful comments during the review process. Correspondence should be addressed to C. M. MacLeod, Divi­ has been shown repeatedly.l sion of Life Sciences, University of Toronto, Scarborough, ON, MIC Our goal was to explore the direct-indirect test distinc­ IA4 Canada (e-mail: [email protected]). tion further by manipulating two powerful encoding vari- Copyright 2000 Psychonomic Society, Inc. 354 DIRECTED FORGETTING AND GENERATION 355 abies: directed forgetting and generation versus reading. that required subjects to provide words in response to de­ These variables are particularly ofinterest because they per­ finitions-presumably a more conceptual indirect test­ mit us to investigate the separate effects ofinitial percep­ although only under the item method. Thus, the most gen­ tual/conceptual encoding and of subsequent rehearsal on eral conclusion emerging now from the directed forgetting direct versus indirect tests. In what follows, we will con­ literature is that there ordinarily is not an R-F difference on sider the literature relating to each ofthese issues in tum indirect tests ofmemory, although as yet there are only a before describing in detail the purpose ofour study. few relevant studies. It also appears to be the case that in­ direct tests can be very sensitive to what otherwise might Directed Forgetting seem to be minor changes over experiments. Over the past 30 years, it has been well established that Two other points regarding directed forgetting studies stimuli accompanied by an instruction to forget ordinarily are especially relevant. First, as Basden et al. (1993) have show reduced retention relative to those accompanied by argued (see also MacLeod, 1998, 1999), the operative mech­ an instruction to remember (for recent reviews, see John­ anism in directed forgetting appears to differ as a function son, 1994; MacLeod, 1998). Known as directed forget­ of the method of delivery of the instructions. In the list ting, this phenomenon can be seen as a proper subset of method, F words appear to be encoded as well as R words, the more general phenomenon of intentional forgetting, but inhibited from retrieval. In contrast, in the item method, which spans social psychology, psychology and the law, F words appear to be rehearsed less and hence are not as animal cognition, and a variety ofother domains (see the well encoded. In our study, we used the item procedure recent book edited by Golding & MacLeod, 1998). Yet by because we wished to focus on directed forgetting due to far the majority ofthe directed forgetting work in the mem­ selective rehearsal. ory literature has exclusively examined performance on Second, in a study by Golding, Long, and MacLeod direct tests of memory, primarily recall and recognition. (1994) that examined recall only, we found that the ability It is worth noting at the outset that directed forgetting to forget information on cue was a function of the relat­ can be realized in either of two ways. Under the item edness ofstudied items. Subjects studied a list containing method, each individual item presented for study is as­ some semantically related successive words (e.g., crab­ signed its own instruction, usually displayed just after the leg) and some unrelated successive words (e.g., cheese­ item. Under the list method, a single instruction is pre­ fire, constructed from the related pairs cheese-cake and sented after each halfofthe list ofitems, with one halfof camp-fire). Using the item method, successive words the list to be forgotten and the other half to be remem­ sometimes received the same instruction, sometimes dif­ bered. The item method produces a larger remember­ ferent instructions. Although recall was best when both forget (R-F) difference than does the list method in both words in a pair were given a remember cue (i.e., the R-R recall and recognition; indeed, the list method generally condition), we also found good recall ofsemantically re­ does not produce a reliable R-F difference in recognition lated word pairs in the R-F condition, much better than (see, e.g., MacLeod, 1999). when the first word ofthe pair was an F word. We inter­ Does directed forgetting affect performance on indirect preted this finding as suggesting that compliance with an tests as well? The picture is not clear: The answer seems instruction to forget a word is difficult when that word im­ to depend both on the particular indirect test employed mediately follows an R word to which it is semantically and the method ofdelivering the instructions to
Recommended publications
  • Memory Control: Investigating the Consequences and Mechanisms of Directed Forgetting in Working Memory
    Memory Control: Investigating the Consequences and Mechanisms of Directed Forgetting in Working Memory by Sara B. Festini A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy (Psychology) in the University of Michigan 2014 Doctoral Committee: Professor Patricia A. Reuter-Lorenz, Chair Professor John Jonides Associate Professor Cindy A. Lustig Associate Professor Rachael D. Seidler © Sara B. Festini 2014 Dedication To my family. Thank you for all of your love, support, and encouragement! ii Acknowledgments First, I would like to thank my primary advisor, Patti Reuter-Lorenz, for her continued guidance, support, and mentorship throughout my time at the University of Michigan. I am extremely grateful for all of the knowledge and advice she provided, which has made me a better scientist, writer, and thinker. Every experiment in my dissertation benefited from Patti’s wisdom, and I look forward to our continued academic collaboration in years to come. I would also like to thank the members of my committee, John Jonides, Cindy Lustig, and Rachael Seidler, for their valuable feedback on my dissertation. Each committee meeting strengthened my dissertation and opened my eyes to new and important viewpoints. I would like to especially thank John for taking the time to read and provide comments on all of the manuscripts comprising this dissertation. His expertise in cognitive control considerably strengthened the arguments and interpretation of my research, for which I am extremely appreciative. Moreover, I want to thank Rachael Seidler for welcoming me into her lab to conduct additional independent research projects. I greatly valued being exposed to different types of research paradigms and analyses, including the wonderful opportunity to analyze resting state functional connectivity data and to pursue my interest in motor learning.
    [Show full text]
  • Wolk DA, Schacter DL, Berman AR, Holcomb PJ, Daffner KR, Budson
    Neuropsychologia 43 (2005) 1662–1672 Patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease attribute conceptual fluency to prior experience David A. Wolk a, b, ∗, Daniel L. Schacter c, Alyssa R. Berman b, Phillip J. Holcomb d, Kirk R. Daffner a, b, Andrew E. Budson b, e a Harvard Medical School, 25 Shattuck Street, Boston, MA 02115, USA b Division of Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, 1620 Tremont Street, 221 Longwood Ave., Boston, MA 02115, USA c Department of Psychology, Harvard University, 33 Kirkland Street, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA d Department of Psychology, Tufts University, Medford, MA 02155, USA e Geriatric Research Education Clinical Center, Edith Nourse Rogers Memorial Veterans Hospital, 200 Springs Road, Bedford MA 01730, USA Received 26 October 2004; received in revised form 12 January 2005; accepted 13 January 2005 Available online 23 February 2005 Abstract Patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) have been found to be relatively dependent on familiarity in their recognition memory judgments. Conceptual fluency has been argued to be an important basis of familiarity. This study investigated the extent to which patients with mild AD use conceptual fluency cues in their recognition decisions. While no evidence of recognition memory was found in the patients with AD, enhanced conceptual fluency was associated with a higher rate of “Old” responses (items endorsed as having been studied) compared to when fluency was not enhanced. The magnitude of this effect was similar for patients with AD and healthy control participants. Additionally, ERP recordings time-locked to test item presentation revealed preserved modulations thought critical to the effect of conceptual fluency on test performance (N400 and late frontal components) in the patients with AD, consistent with the behavioral results.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive and Neuropsychological Aspects of Age-Associated Memory Dysfunction
    COGNITIVE AND NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL ASPECTS OF AGE-ASSOCIATED MEMORY DYSFUNCTION A k a d e m is k a v h a n d l in g som för avläggande av filosofie doktorsexamen med vederbörligt tillstånd av rektorsämbetet vid Umeå universitet framlägges för offentlig granskning vid Psykologiska institutionen, Umeå Universitet, Seminarierum 2, fredagen den 24 januari 1992, klockan 10.15 AV Th o m a s K a r l s s o n Psykologiska institutionen, Umeå Universitet, Umeå COGNITIVE AND NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL ASPECTS OF AGE- ASSOCIATED MEMORY DYSFUNCTION BY Thom as K arlsson Doctoral Dissertation Department of Psychology, University of Umeå, Umeå, Sweden ABSTRACT Memory dysfunction is common in association with the course of normal aging. Memory dysfunction is also obligatory in age-associated neurological disorders, such as Alzheimer’s disease. However, despite the ubiquitousness of age-related memory decline, several basic questions regarding this entity remain unanswered. The present investigation addressed two such questions: (1) Can individuals suffering from memory dysfunction due to aging and amnesia due to Alzheimer’s disease improve memory performance if contextual support is provided at the time of acquisition of to-be- remembered material or reproduction of to-be-remembered material? (2) Are memory deficits observed in ‘younger’ older adults similar to the deficits observed in ‘older’ elderly subjects, Alzheimer’s disease, and memory dysfunction in younger subjects? The outcome of this investigation suggests an affirmative answer to the first question. Given appropriate support at encoding and retrieval, even densely amnesic patients can improve their memory performance. As to the second question, a more complex pattern emerges.
    [Show full text]
  • Introduction to "Implicit Memory: Multiple Perspectives"
    Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 1990. 28 (4). 338-340 Introduction to "Implicit memory: Multiple perspectives" DANIEL L. SCHACTER University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona Psychological studies of memory have traditionally assessed retention of recently acquired in­ formation with recall and recognition tests that require intentional recollection or explicit memory for a study episode. During the past decade, there has been a growing interest in the experimen­ tal study of implicit memory: unintentional retrieval of information on tests that do not require any conscious recollection of a specific previous experience. Dissociations between explicit and implicit memory have been established, but theoretical interpretation ofthem remains controver­ sial. The present set of papers examines implicit memory from multiple perspectives-cognitive, developmental, psychophysiological, and neuropsychological-and addresses key methodological, empirical, and theoretical issues in this rapidly growing sector of memory research. The papers that follow were presented at a symposium Delaney, 1990; Tulving & Schacter, 1990), it is also pos­ entitled "Implicit memory: Multiple perspectives," held sible to adopt a unitary or single-system view of memory at the annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society in At­ and still make use of the implicit/explicit distinction (e.g., lantaonNovember 17,1989. The symposium represents Roediger, Weldon, & Challis, 1989; Witherspoon & something of a milestone, in the sense that if someone Moscovitch, 1989). had suggested organizing an implicit memory symposium The distinction between implicit and explicit memory a decade ago, he or she probably would have been met is also closely related to the distinction between "direct" with a blank stare. That stare would have reflected two and "indirect" tests of memory (Johnson & Hasher, important facts: first, because the term "implicit 1987).
    [Show full text]
  • An Effect of Age on Implicit Memory That Is Not Due to Explicit Contamination
    Running head: AN EFFECT OF AGE ON IMPLICIT MEMORY 1 An effect of age on implicit memory that is not due to explicit contamination: Implications for single and multiple-systems theories Emma V. Ward, David R. Shanks, and Christopher, J. Berry University College London Author Note Emma V. Ward, David R. Shanks, and Christopher, J. Berry, Department of Cognitive, Perceptual and Brain Sciences, University College London, England. This research was supported by an ESRC studentship award. We would like to thank Sara Garcia for help with data collection in Experiment 3, and Qizhang Sun for data collection in Experiments 4b and 5. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Emma V. Ward, Department of Cognitive, Perceptual and Brain Sciences, UCL, 26 Bedford Way, London WC1H 0AP. E-mail: [email protected] Running head: AN EFFECT OF AGE ON IMPLICIT MEMORY 2 Abstract Implicit memory tests often reveal facilitated processing of previously encountered stimuli even when they cannot be explicitly retrieved. For example, despite decrements in recognition memory, priming in healthy older adults is often comparable in magnitude to that in young individuals. Such observations are commonly taken as evidence for independent explicit and implicit memory systems. On a picture version of the continuous identification with recognition task (CID-R), we found a reliable age-related reduction in recognition memory while the effect on priming did not reach statistical significance (Experiment 1). Experiment 2 replicated these observations using separate priming (CID) and recognition (R) phases. However, when data are combined across experiments to increase power, we find that the reduction in priming reaches statistical significance.
    [Show full text]
  • Linguistic Communication: Perspectives for Research; Report of the Study Group on Linguistic Communication to the National Institute of Education
    i. DOCUMENT RESUME ED 097 622 CS 001 353 AUTHOR Miller, George A., Ed. TITLE Linguistic Communication: Perspectives for Research; Report of the Study Group on Linguistic Communication to the National Institute of Education. INSTITUTION International Reading Association, Newark, Del. SPONS AGENCY National Inst. of Education (DHEW), Washington, D C PUB DATE 74 NOTE 53p.; Report of a Study Group on Linguistic Communication in Hyannis area of MassachusettsQ August 13-24, 1973 AVAILABLE FROMInternational Reading Association, 800 Barksdale Road, Newark, Delaware 19711 (Stock No. 929, $2.00 nonmember, $1.50 member) EDRS PRICE MP-$0.75 HC-$3.15 PLUS POSTAGE DESCRIPTORS Communication (Thought Transfer); *Educational Objectives; *Educational Research; Educational Strategies; *Linguistics; Reading Comprehension; Second Languages; Social Influences ABSTRACT This publication, which is divided into three parts, contains the report of a study group of the NationalInstitute of Education which met to investigate some of the problemsof linguistic communication. Part 1 summarizes the general point ofview of the group. Part 2 discussesobjectives, strategies, current status, and time scale. Part 3 describes research activitieswhich cover the social and developmental context with influencesoutside the classroom, characteristics of teachers and classroom,characteristics of the reader, influence on reading of dialectalvariation, and the processes of reading and writing,especially basic literacy, comprehension of language, writing, and second languagelearning. The study group recommends a program of research anddevelopment on learning and instruction in the elements of linguistic communication--reading, writing, listening, speaking--including interactions among these elements. (SW) N.1 S !f. PAP T MI NT OF HEALTH. I01.3( A.1.. hi a WELFARE o.,,s 'Nod NSMUTE OF 1/4.0 t..RfION '.
    [Show full text]
  • Prospective Remembering: Perceptually Driven Or Conceptually Driven Processes?
    Memory & Cognition 1998.26 (/).121-/34 Prospective remembering: Perceptually driven or conceptually driven processes? MARK A. McDANIEL University ofNew Mexico, Albuquerque, New Mexico BRIDGETROBINSON-RIEGLER Augsburg College, Minneapolis, Minnesota and GILLES O. EINSTEIN Furman University, Greenville, South Carolina Converging experimental operations and several prospective memory tasks were used across three experiments to determine the extent to which prospective remembering is supported by data­ driven versus conceptually driven processes. In all experiments, subjects were asked to perform an action when a target item later occurred. When the semantic context changed from encoding to test, prospective memory significantly declined (Experiment 1). When the target event (the item, which in its subsequent appearance in the experiment was the signal to perform the action) was presented as a word (relative to picture presentation, Experiment 2) or was encoded nonsemantically (relative to semantic encoding, Experiment 3), there was a decline in prospective memory performance. Di­ viding attention during prospective memory retrieval substantially reduced prospective memory per­ formance (Experiment 3). The results of this research indicated that prospective memory is largely conceptually driven, and it behaves more similarly to direct rather than indirect conceptual tests. We suggest that prospective remembering of the type studied here is mediated by a reflexive episodic a£ociative memory system as proposed by Moscovitch (1994). The processes that support remembering past events explicit retrospective memory tests direct a person to be in (i.e., retrospective memory) have been well studied in the a retrieval mode (see also Craik, Govoni, Naveh-Benjamin, experimental literature and extensively considered by the­ & Anderson, 1996). Unlike for these retrospective mem­ oreticians.
    [Show full text]
  • Memory Illusions
    JOURNAL OF MEMORY AND LANGUAGE 35, 76±100 (1996) ARTICLE NO. 0005 Memory Illusions HENRY L. ROEDIGER III Rice University Memory illusions may be de®ned as cases in which a rememberer's report of a past event seriously deviates from the event's actual occurrence. This article introduces the special issue of the Journal of Memory and Language that is devoted to memory illusions by grounding their study in the context of perceptual illusions. Perceptual illusions have been investigated since the 1850s, whereas memory illusions have been systematically investigated only since the late 1960s or early 1970s (despite some pioneering research and writing before this time). I suggest possible reasons for this discrepancy in research activity, sketch a brief history of the study of memory illusions, and then consider the variety of memory illusions that are studied in contemporary psychology. The papers composing the special issue are introduced during this brief cataloging of memory illusions. Related areas of research are discussed in the concluding remarks. q 1996 Academic Press, Inc. Illusions have fascinated philosophers, recall of a past experience.'' Even though both physicists, and psychologists throughout re- perceptual and memory illusions were men- corded history. Whenever our experience of tioned in this de®nition, by 1964 there had events is shown to be at odds with the actual been hundreds of studies of perceptual illu- nature of the events, our curiosity is piqued. sions but, even counting generously, there The word illusion is derived from the Latin probably existed fewer than 30 publications word illudere, meaning to mock. Illusions directed speci®cally at memory errors, with mock our belief that what we perceive, re- the term memory illusion rarely arising at all.
    [Show full text]
  • Implicit and Explicit Memory for Novel Visual Objects in Older and \Bunger Adults
    Psychology and Aging Copyright 1992 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1992, Vol. 7, No. 2, 299-308 0882-7974/92/53.00 Implicit and Explicit Memory for Novel Visual Objects in Older and \bunger Adults Daniel L. Schacter Lynn A. Cooper Harvard University Columbia University Michael Valdiserri University of Arizona Two experiments examined effects of aging on implicit and explicit memory for novel visual ob- jects. Implicit memory was assessed with an object decision task in which subjects indicated whether briefly exposed drawings represented structurally possible or impossible objects. Explicit memory was assessed with a yes-no recognition task. On the object decision task, old and young subjects both showed priming for previously studied possible objects and no priming for impossible objects; the magnitude of the priming effect did not differ as a function of age. By contrast, the elderly were impaired on the recognition task. Results suggest that the ability to form and retain structural descriptions of novel objects may be spared in older adults. Explicit memory refers to conscious recollection of previous 1988; Davis et al, 1990; Hultsch, Masson, & Small, 1991). These experiences, as expressed on recall and recognition tests, latter findings are difficult to interpret, however, because they whereas implicit memory refers to facilitations of performance, may well reflect "contamination" of implicit task performance often referred to as priming effects, on tests that do not require by explicit retrieval strategies, strategies that are more benefi- any conscious or intentional retrieval of previous experiences cial to young than to old subjects (cf. Graf, 1990; Howard, 1991; (Graf & Schacter, 1985; Schacter, 1987).
    [Show full text]
  • Long-Term Recency in Anterograde Amnesia
    RESEARCH ARTICLE Long-Term Recency in Anterograde Amnesia Deborah Talmi1,2*, Jeremy B. Caplan3,4, Brian Richards5, Morris Moscovitch1,3 1 Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, 2 School of Psychological Sciences, University of Manchester, Manchester, United Kingdom, 3 Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, 4 Psychology Department and Centre for Neuroscience, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada, 5 Department of Psychology, Baycrest Centre, Toronto, Ontario, Canada * [email protected] Abstract Amnesia is usually described as an impairment of a long-term memory (LTM) despite an in- tact short-term memory (STM). The intact recency effect in amnesia had supported this view. Although dual-store models of memory have been challenged by single-store models based on interference theory, this had relatively little influence on our understanding and treatment of amnesia, perhaps because the debate has centred on experiments in the neu- OPEN ACCESS rologically intact population. Here we tested a key prediction of single-store models for free Citation: Talmi D, Caplan JB, Richards B, recall in amnesia: that people with amnesia will exhibit a memory advantage for the most re- Moscovitch M (2015) Long-Term Recency in cent items even when all items are stored in and retrieved from LTM, an effect called long- Anterograde Amnesia. PLoS ONE 10(6): e0124084. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0124084 term recency. People with amnesia and matched controls studied, and then free-recalled, word lists with a distractor task following each word, including the last (continual distractor Academic Editor: Efthimios M. C. Skoulakis, Alexander Fleming Biomedical Sciences Research task, CDFR).
    [Show full text]
  • An Examination of Adult Age Differences in Implicit and Explicit Memory for Prescription Drug Advertisements
    Mississippi State University Scholars Junction Theses and Dissertations Theses and Dissertations 1-1-2008 An Examination Of Adult Age Differences In Implicit And Explicit Memory For Prescription Drug Advertisements L Ty Abernathy Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarsjunction.msstate.edu/td Recommended Citation Abernathy, L Ty, "An Examination Of Adult Age Differences In Implicit And Explicit Memory For Prescription Drug Advertisements" (2008). Theses and Dissertations. 530. https://scholarsjunction.msstate.edu/td/530 This Dissertation - Open Access is brought to you for free and open access by the Theses and Dissertations at Scholars Junction. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Scholars Junction. For more information, please contact [email protected]. AN EXAMINATION OF ADULT AGE DIFFERENCES IN IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY FOR PRESCRIPTION DRUG ADVERTISEMENTS By Larry Ty Abernathy A Dissertation Submitted to the Faculty of Mississippi State University in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Cognitive Science in the Department of Psychology Mississippi State, Mississippi December 2008 Copyright by Larry Ty Abernathy 2008 AN EXAMINATION OF ADULT AGE DIFFERENCES IN IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT MEMORY FOR PRESCRIPTION DRUG ADVERTISEMENTS By Larry Ty Abernathy Approved: __________________________________ ___________________________________ Carolyn E. Adams-Price Deborah K. Eakin Associate Professor of Psychology
    [Show full text]
  • On the Relationship Between Recognition Familiarity and Perceptual Fluency: Evidence for Distinct Mnemonic Processes
    Acta Psychologica 98 (1998) 211±230 On the relationship between recognition familiarity and perceptual ¯uency: Evidence for distinct mnemonic processes Anthony D. Wagner *, John D.E. Gabrieli Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Jordan Hall, Bldg. 420, Stanford, CA 94305-2130, USA Abstract Fluent reprocessing of perceptual aspects of recently experienced stimuli is thought to sup- port repetition priming eects on implicit perceptual memory tests. Although behavioral and neuropsychological dissociations demonstrate that separable mnemonic processes and neural substrates mediate implicit and explicit test performance, dual-process theories of memory posit that explicit recognition memory judgments may be based on familiarity derived from the same perceptual ¯uency that yields perceptual priming. Here we consider the relationship between familiarity-based recognition memory and implicit perceptual memory. A select re- view of the literature demonstrates that the ¯uency supporting implicit perceptual memory is functionally and anatomically distinct from that supporting recognition memory. In con- trast to perceptual ¯uency, recognition familiarity is more sensitive to conceptual than to per- ceptual processing, and does not depend on modality-speci®c sensory cortices. Alternative possible relationships between familiarity in explicit memory and ¯uency in implicit memory are discussed. Ó 1998 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved. PsycINFO classi®cation: 2343 Keywords: Explicit memory; Declarative memory; Implicit memory; Nondeclarative memory; Recognition memory; Priming; Process dissociation; Dual-process theories * Corresponding author. Present address: Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Room 854, William James Hall, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA. Tel.: +1 617-495-3856; fax: +1 617-496-3122; e-mail: [email protected]. 0001-6918/98/$19.00 Ó 1998 Elsevier Science B.V.
    [Show full text]