The Substance of Cultural Evolution: Expressed Collectively, As Is the Case for Multi- Culturally Framed Systems of Social Or- Level Selection

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Substance of Cultural Evolution: Expressed Collectively, As Is the Case for Multi- Culturally Framed Systems of Social Or- Level Selection The substance of cultural evolution: expressed collectively, as is the case for multi- Culturally framed systems of social or- level selection. While valid questions can be raised about ganization Smaldino’s characterization and differentiation of these three different levels, especially with Dwight W. Read regard to his thesis that group success in hu- Department of Anthropology and Department of man societies largely comes from “the organi- Statistics, University of California—Los Angeles, zation of a well-defined collection of differen- Los Angeles, CA 90094 tiated individuals all participating in a group- [email protected] level behavior” (p. 10-11), my focus here is on the phylogenetic trend going from solitary to Abstract: Models of cultural evolution need to structured groups and from individual to address not only the organizational aspects of hu- emergent to culturally framed behavior as we man societies, but also the complexity and struc- evolutionarily move towards our species, Ho- ture of cultural idea systems that frame their sys- mo sapiens, with its subdivision into highly tems of organization. These cultural idea systems determine a framework within which behaviors differentiated societies. The picture drawn by take place and provide mutually understood mean- Smaldino, using his wording for the limita- ings for behavior from the perspective of both tions of multilevel selection, “is not incorrect, agent and recipient that are critical for the coher- but it is incomplete” (p. 10). ence of human systems of social organization. The evolution of human social systems centers around the development of systems of Smaldino advances an argument similar to that organization that incorporate, rather than sup- of Lane et al. (2009) regarding the need to press, individual differentiation (Read 2012). make “a shift in perspective, from population Briefly, the phylogenetic trend towards in- thinking to organization thinking” (2009:12, creased individualization of behaviors that we emphasis in the original) by arguing that mod- see when we traverse the primates towards els of cultural evolution have not taken into Homo sapiens is paralleled by social com- account contextualization of human behavior plexity increasing exponentially with the num- through systems of organization that make ber of individualistic group members (Read human behavior more complex than just as 2012: Figure 4.3). This increase was accom- epiphenomena of individual level traits. This modated not only through neurological chang- leads him to consider three levels for modeling es (Dunbar 1998) but by changes in the struc- selection acting on traits: (1) individual traits, tural organization of social units that culminat- (2) multilevel traits (traits aggregated over be- ed, from a biological perspective, in reduction haviors engaged in collectively by interacting of the size of chimpanzee social units (Read group members), and (3) group traits ex- 2012) -- where chimpanzees social organiza- pressed through the institutionalized organiza- tion is often taken as a model for our ancestral tion of role-differentiated individuals (pp. 5-6). lineage when it diverged from the other pri- Group traits are, in his view, distinguishable mates (Chapais 2008) -- as a way to accom- by making use of the “specific organization of modate social complexity arising from highly [role] differentiated individuals” (p. 8), with individualized behavior (Read 2012: Figure selection acting on systems of organization 4.4). The social complexity introduced that maintain internal differentiation of indi- through increased individuality (what Smaldi- viduals, hence acting on emergent group be- no calls “individual differentiation”), was havior (p. 6) rather than on individual behavior eventually accommodated within the hominin ancestry of Homo sapiens by shifting from so- 1 Behavioral and Brain Sciences (2014) 37:3 cial systems based on face-to-face interaction organization of human societies (Read et al. that characterize the non-human primates 2009; Read 2012). While Smaldino correctly (which also leads to within group, aggregated places importance on systems of organization behavior upon which multi-level selection can that incorporate role differentiated individuals operate) to relational based systems of social in human societies, he does not discuss the fact organization (Smaldino’s institutionalized or- that these systems of organization need not be ganization of differentiated individuals) that emergent, but are often cultural constructions, are culturally framed (Read 2012). The fram- such as the culturally formed kinship systems ing through cultural idea systems is not in- that provide structure and organization in hu- cluded in Smaldino’s argument and is critical man societies, especially in the small scale so- to our understanding of human systems of so- cieties that were the evolutionary precursors of cial organization (cf. Leaf 2009). large scale human societies. Cultural kinship There is marked change in the ontological systems both define the societal boundaries level at which selection operates and fitness is and provide the structure and organization that measured concomitant with the sequence go- establishes the basis for the role differentiation ing from genetic traits expressed individually that Smaldino discusses (Leaf 2009; Leaf & and in isolation to traits expressed culturally Read 2012). The kinship terminologies that and collectively. The sequence begins with express the different systems of cultural kin- fitness measured by the number of reproducing ship relations are not emergent, as research on progeny, then when behavioral interaction the structural logic of kinship systems has among progeny is part of the trait, as occurs demonstrated (e.g., Read 1984, 2001, 2007, with biologically based altruistic behaviors, 2010; Leaf & Read 2012; Read et al. 2013). inclusive fitness becomes the measure of se- Terminologies are not the epiphenomena of lection. With multilevel selection acting on already patterned behavior -- as was assumed traits expressed collectively through group in some of the early research on human kin- structure (what Smaldino refers to as “collec- ship systems and has been assumed in ac- tive behavior” or “aggregate traits”), group- counts of human evolution (e.g., Chapais derived fitness averaged over group members 2008) -- but are constructed idea systems (Leaf is assigned. Next are emergent traits, such as & Read 2012) that provide conceptual organi- the linear, stable (e.g., Isbell & Young 1993, zation for the small scale societies from which Range & Noë 2002), matrilineally inherited present day human societies have evolved. female dominance hierarchies (Kapsalis 2004) Kinship terminology systems have a genera- that emerge in many of the species making up tive logic to them that can be expressed the Cercopithecines from a female “placing” through a “grammar,” and differences among her biological daughter immediately below her kinship terminology systems are derived from in the dominance ranking (e.g., le Roux et al. systematic differences in the generative logic 2011; see Read 2012 and references therein). of kinship terminologies (Read 2013). Emergent traits, for Smaldino, provide transi- Models of cultural evolution need to ad- tion from the uniformity of group behavior dress not only the organizational aspects dis- assumed in multilevel selection to organized, cussed by Smaldino, but also the complexity role differentiated behavior through which and structure of cultural idea systems that group level traits are expressed. Here fitness is frame the systems of organization that are cen- measured directly through the group-level tral to human societies. These cultural idea trait. systems determine a framework within which Missing from this sequence, though, is the behaviors take place and provide mutually un- critical “next step” leading to the structure and derstood meanings for behavior from the per- 2 spective of both agent and recipient that are pological relativism (pp. 78-117). Urbana: critical for the coherence of human systems of University of Illinois Press. social organization. Read, D. (2007). Kinship theory: A paradigm shift. Ethnology, 46(4), 329-364. References Read, D. (2010). The algebraic logic of kin- Chapais, B. (2008). Primeval kinship: How ship terminology structure. Behavioral and pair bonding gave birth to human society. Brain Sciences, 33(5), 399-400. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Read, D. (2012). How culture makes us hu- Dunbar, R. I. M. (1998). The social brain hy- man: Primate evolution and the formation pothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology, 6, of human societies Walnut Creek: Left 178-190. Coast Press. Isbell, L., & Young, T. (1993). Social and eco- Read, D. (2013). A new approach to forming a logical influences on activity budgets of typology of kinship terminology systems: vervet monkeys, and their implications for From Morgan and Murdock to the present. group living. Behavioral Ecology and So- Structure and Dynamics, 6(1). ciobiology, 32, 377-385. http://www.escholarship.org/uc/item/0ss6j Kapsalis, E. (2004). Matrilineal kinship and 8sh primate behavior. In B. Chapais & C. M. Read, D., Lane, D., & van der Leeuw, S. Berman (Eds.), Kinship and behavior (2009). The innovation innovation. In D. inprimates (pp. 153-176). Oxford: Oxford Lane, D. Pumain, S. van der Leeuw & G. University Press. West (Eds.), Complexity perspectives in Lane, D., Maxfield, R. M., Read, D., & van innovation and social change
Recommended publications
  • 1. a Dangerous Idea
    About This Guide This guide is intended to assist in the use of the DVD Daniel Dennett, Darwin’s Dangerous Idea. The following pages provide an organizational schema for the DVD along with general notes for each section, key quotes from the DVD,and suggested discussion questions relevant to the section. The program is divided into seven parts, each clearly distinguished by a section title during the program. Contents Seven-Part DVD A Dangerous Idea. 3 Darwin’s Inversion . 4 Cranes: Getting Here from There . 8 Fruits of the Tree of Life . 11 Humans without Skyhooks . 13 Gradualism . 17 Memetic Revolution . 20 Articles by Daniel Dennett Could There Be a Darwinian Account of Human Creativity?. 25 From Typo to Thinko: When Evolution Graduated to Semantic Norms. 33 In Darwin’s Wake, Where Am I?. 41 2 Darwin's Dangerous Idea 1. A Dangerous Idea Dennett considers Darwin’s theory of evolution by natural selection the best single idea that anyone ever had.But it has also turned out to be a dangerous one. Science has accepted the theory as the most accurate explanation of the intricate design of living beings,but when it was first proposed,and again in recent times,the theory has met with a backlash from many people.What makes evolution so threatening,when theories in physics and chemistry seem so harmless? One problem with the introduction of Darwin’s great idea is that almost no one was prepared for such a revolutionary view of creation. Dennett gives an analogy between this inversion and Sweden’s change in driving direction: I’m going to imagine, would it be dangerous if tomorrow the people in Great Britain started driving on the right? It would be a really dangerous place to be because they’ve been driving on the left all these years….
    [Show full text]
  • Social Science Studies and Experiments with Web Applications
    Social Science Studies and Experiments with Web Applications Author Dawit Bezu Mengistu Supervisor Aris Alissandrakis Exam date 30 August 2018 Subject Social Media and Web Technologies Level Master Course code 5ME11E-VT18 Abstract This thesis explores a web-based method to do studies in cultural evolution. Cu- mulative cultural evolution (CCE) is defined as social learning that allows for the accumulation of changes over time where successful modifications are maintained un- til additional change is introduced. In the past few decades, many interdisciplinary studies were conducted on cultural evolution. However, until recently most of those studies were limited to lab experiments. This thesis aims to address the limitations of the experimental methods by replicating a lab-based experiment online. A web-based application was developed and used for replicating an experiment on conformity by Solomon Asch[1951]. The developed application engages participants in an optical illusion test within different groups of social influence. The major finding of the study reveals that conformity increases on trials with higher social influence. In addition, it was also found that when the task becomes more difficult, the subject's conformity increases. These findings were also reported in the original experiment. The results of the study showed that lab-based experiments in cultural evolution studies can be replicated over the web with quantitatively similar results. Keywords| Cumulative Cultural Evolution, web-based experiment, optical illusion, real-time communication 1 Dedication To Simon & Yohana 2 Acknowledgements I want to thank the Swedish Institute (SI) for granting me a scholarship. I would like to express my great appreciation to my supervisor Dr.
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolution of Cultural Evolution
    Evolutionary Anthropology 123 ARTICLES The Evolution of Cultural Evolution JOSEPH HENRICH AND RICHARD McELREATH Humans are unique in their range of environments and in the nature and diversity of attempted to glean as much as they their behavioral adaptations. While a variety of local genetic adaptations exist within could from the aboriginals about nar- our species, it seems certain that the same basic genetic endowment produces arctic doo, an aquatic fern bearing spores foraging, tropical horticulture, and desert pastoralism, a constellation that represents they had observed the aboriginals us- a greater range of subsistence behavior than the rest of the Primate Order combined. ing to make bread. Despite traveling The behavioral adaptations that explain the immense success of our species are along a creek and receiving frequent cultural in the sense that they are transmitted among individuals by social learning and gifts of fish from the locals, they were have accumulated over generations. Understanding how and when such culturally unable to figure out how to catch evolved adaptations arise requires understanding of both the evolution of the psycho- them. Two months after departing logical mechanisms that underlie human social learning and the evolutionary (popu- from their base camp, the threesome lation) dynamics of cultural systems. had become entirely dependent on nardoo bread and occasional gifts of fish from the locals. Despite consum- In 1860, aiming to be the first Euro- three men (King, Wills and Gray) ing what seemed to be sufficient calo- peans to travel south to north across from their base camp in Cooper’s ries, all three became increasingly fa- Australia, Robert Burke led an ex- Creek in central Australia with five tigued and suffered from painful tremely well-equipped expedition of fully loaded camels (specially im- bowel movements.
    [Show full text]
  • Is Cultural Evolution Analogous to Biological Evolution? a Critical Review of Memetics 1
    Intellectica , 2007/2-3, 46-47 , pp . 49-68 Is Cultural Evolution Analogous to Biological Evolution? 1 A Critical Review of Memetics Dominique Guillo Résumé : L'évolution de la culture est-elle analogue à l'évolution biologique ? Une revue critique de la mémétique . Les défenseurs de la mémétique proposent de bâtir une théorie de la culture à partir d’une analogie avec l’évolution biologique. Cette théorie néo-darwinienne de la culture doit être soigneusement distinguée de la psychologie évolutionniste et de l’anthropologie cognitive, car elle n’est pas réductionniste. Plus largement, elle doit être distinguée de toutes les théories de la culture appuyées sur l’hypothèse selon laquelle l’esprit individuel est actif et non passif lorsqu’il adopte un trait culturel. Elle se rapproche par certains aspects de paradigmes traditionnels des sciences sociales, mais constitue un modèle bien spécifique. En dépit de l’intérêt des arguments qu’elle propose et de certaines recherches qu’elle a suscitées, elle s’appuie sur des propositions et des concepts – en particulier le concept de mème – qui soulèvent des difficultés. La nature des mèmes est incertaine et problématique. À supposer qu’ils existent, leur empire ne peut couvrir qu’une partie des phénomènes culturels. Enfin, la recherche des preuves de leur existence rencontre de sérieux obstacles. Mots-clé : culture; évolution; psychologie évolutionniste; gène; imitation; mème; sélection naturelle; néo-darwinisme; choix rationnel; réplicateur ; apprentissage social ABSTRACT : The advocates of memetics seek to construct a theory of culture on the basis of an analogy with biological evolution. Such a neo-Darwinian theory of culture should be carefully distinguished from evolutionary psychology and cognitive anthropology, as it is not reductionist.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolution of Social Organization
    Chapter 13. EVOLUTION OF SOCIAL ORGANIZATION “Honor, duty, country” West Point Motto “To us at the time, a suicide air force was a very natu- ral thing, nothing more than a means of self-defense toward the end of the war. True, the war ended and saved me 28 years ago, but if I had to be a Kamikaze pilot again, I would.” Sei Watanabe Lt. Gen. Japanese Defense Forces, ret. I. The Problem of Cooperation A. Three Unusual Features of Human Societies Human societies exhibit cooperation, coordination,anddivision of labor, three fea- tures that place them at striking variance with most animals. Cooperation involves individ- uals doing something for the common benefit of everyone in a social group, as when soldiers defend a whole nation against its enemies. Coordination involves everyone doing things one way instead of another so that social activity can proceed efficiently (Susden, 1986). For example, we all agree to drive on the same side of the road and to pronounce words in the same way to avoid the chaos that would result if everyone ‘did their own thing’ (as we children of the 60s once imagined possible). The division of labor results when dif- ferent individuals undertake specialized tasks, and then exchange the products of their la- bor. The sexual division of labor is the most ancient example in human societies. Historically, men’s and women’s activities have differed fairly radically, but within the household each sex’s products are contributed to a common pot that family members draw upon. Highly social animals are rare, and basic Darwinian analysis shows why (Alex- ander, 1974).
    [Show full text]
  • CULTURAL TRANSMISSION a View from Chimpanzees and Human Infants
    JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY Tomasello / CULTURAL TRANSMISSION Human beings are biologically adapted for culture in ways that other primates are not, as evidenced most clearly by the fact that only human cultural traditions accumulate modifications over historical time (the ratchet effect). The key adaptation is one that enables individuals to understand other individuals as inten- tional agents like the self. This species-unique form of social cognition emerges in human ontogeny at around 1 year of age as infants begin to engage with other persons in various kinds of joint attentional activi- ties involving gaze following, social referencing, and gestural communication. Young children’s joint attentional skills then engender some uniquely powerful forms of cultural learning, enabling the acquisition of language, discourse skills, tool use practices, and many other conventional activities. These novel forms of cultural learning allow human beings to pool their cognitive resources both contemporaneously and over historical time in ways that are unique in the animal kingdom. CULTURAL TRANSMISSION A View From Chimpanzees and Human Infants MICHAEL TOMASELLO Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Germany Primates are highly social beings. They begin their lives clinging to their mothers and nurs- ing, and they spend their next few months, or even years, still in close proximity to her. Adult primates live in close-knit social groups, for the most part, in which members individually recognize one another and form various types of long-term social relationships (see Tomasello & Call, 1994, 1997, for reviews). As primates, human beings follow this same pattern, of course, but they also have some unique forms of sociality that may be character- ized as “ultrasocial” or, in more common parlance, cultural (Tomasello, Kruger, & Ratner, 1993).
    [Show full text]
  • Thresholds and Transitions in Hominin Cultural Evolution
    philosophies Article The Encultured Primate: Thresholds and Transitions in Hominin Cultural Evolution Chris Buskes Department of Philosophy, Radboud University, 6500 HD Nijmegen, The Netherlands; [email protected] Received: 30 November 2018; Accepted: 28 January 2019; Published: 1 February 2019 Abstract: This article tries to shed light on the mystery of human culture. Human beings are the only extant species with cumulative, evolving cultures. Many animal species do have cultural traditions in the form of socially transmitted practices but they typically lack cumulative culture. Why is that? This discrepancy between humans and animals is even more puzzling if one realizes that culture seems highly advantageous. Thanks to their accumulated knowledge and techniques our early ancestors were able to leave their cradle in Africa and swarm out across the planet, thereby adjusting themselves to a whole range of new environments. Without culture this would have been impossible. So we may ask once again: if cumulative culture is so useful, why don’t other animals have it? In order to explain this mystery I won’t appeal to the major transitions in human evolution—like walking upright, crafting stone tools and controlling fire, etc.—because that would be question begging. Instead I try to unearth the mechanisms that caused those evolutionary turning points to occur in the first place. It seems that unlike other animals, humans are predisposed to efficiently acquire, store and transmit cultural information in such ways that our cultures can genuinely evolve. Keywords: cultural evolution; cumulative culture; gene–culture coevolution; dual inheritance; universal Darwinism; memetics 1. Introduction Why is Homo sapiens the only species on our planet with cumulative culture? This issue is a profound mystery because it is obvious that culture has many merits.
    [Show full text]
  • Culture and the Evolution Learning of Social
    ELSEVIER Culture and the Evolution of Social Learning Mark V. Flinn Department of Anthropology, University of Missouri Applications of modern evolutionary theory to human culture have generated several different theoretical approaches that challenge traditional anthropological perspectives. “Cultural selection” and “mind parasite” theories model culture as an independent evo- lutionary system because transmission of cultural traits via social learning is distinct from transmission of genes vla DNA replication. “Dual-inheritance” and “co-evolution” theories model culture as an intermediary evolutionary process that involves informa- tion from two inheritance systems: genetics and social learning. “Evolutionary psychol- ogy” theories emphasize that the evolutionary history of natural selection on mental pro- cesses links culture and biological adaptation; hence, cultural information is viewed as part of the organic phenotype and not an independent evolutionary system. Cross-cul- tural universals and scenarios of the “environment of evolutionary adaptedness” are used to identify characteristics of the “evolved mind” (human nature). “Behavioral ecol- ogy” theories examine relations between behavior and environmental context. Behav- ioral/cultural variations are viewed as products of flexible decision-making processes (evolved mind) that may respond adaptively to micro-environmental differences. It is difficult to devise empirical tests that distinguish among these theories, because they share many basic premises and make similar predictions
    [Show full text]
  • Culture Is Part of Human Biology Why the Superorganic Concept Serves the Human Sciences Badly
    Culture is Part of Human Biology Why the Superorganic Concept Serves the Human Sciences Badly Peter J. Richerson Department of Environmental Science and Policy University of California - Davis Davis, California 95616 [email protected] & Robert Boyd Department of Anthropology University of California - Los Angeles Los Angeles, California 90095 [email protected] Draft 3 7/17/01. Published in Science Studies: Probing the Dynamics of Scientific Knowledge, edited by S. Maasen and M. Winterhager, Bielefeld: transcript Verlag, 2001. © by the authors. Cultural Evolution Is Important Rates of violence in the American South have long been much greater than in the North. Accounts of duels, feuds, bushwhackings, and lynchings occur prominently in visitors’ accounts, newspaper articles, and autobiography from the 18th Century onward. According to crime statistics these differences persist today. In their book, Culture of Honor, Richard Nisbett and Dov Cohen (1996) argue that the South is more violent than the North because Southerners have different, culturally acquired beliefs about personal honor than Northerners. The South was disproportionately settled by Protestant Scotch-Irish, people with an animal herding background, whereas Northern settlers were English, German and Dutch peasant farmers. Most herders live in thinly settled, lawless regions. Since livestock are easy to steal, herders seek reputations for willingness to engage in violent behavior as a deterrent to rustling and other predatory behavior. Of course, bad men come to subscribe to the same code, the better to intimidate their victims. As this arms race proceeds, arguments over trivial acts can rapidly escalate if a man—less often a woman—thinks his honor is at stake, and the resulting “culture of honor” leads to high rates of violence.
    [Show full text]
  • Richerson, Boyd and Henrich
    Unpublished Manuscript Prepared for Dahlem Conference on Genetic and Cultural Evolution of Cooperation Berlin 23-28 June, 2002 CULTURAL EVOLUTION OF HUMAN COOPERATION Peter Richerson Department of Environmental Science and Policy University of California—Davis Davis, CA 95616 Robert Boyd Anthropology Department University of California—Los Angeles Los Angeles, CA 90095 Joseph Henrich Anthropology Department Emory University Atlanta, GA 30322 Version 3.0 9/17/02 1 Abstract. We here review the evolutionary theory relevant to the question of human cooperation and compare the results to other theoretical perspectives. Then, we will summarize some of our own work, distilling a compound explanation that we believe gives a plausible account of human cooperation and selfishness. This account leans heavily on group selection1 on cultural2 variation but also includes lower level forces driven by both micro-scale cooperation and purely selfish motives. We propose that innate aspects of our social psychology coevolved with group selected cultural institutions to produce just the kinds of social and moral faculties originally proposed by Darwin. We call this the “tribal social instincts” hypothesis. The account is systemic in the sense that human social systems are functionally differentiated, conflicted, and diverse. A successful explanation of human cooperation has to account for these complexities. For example, a tribal scale cultural group selection process alone cannot account for human patterns of cooperation because, on the one hand, much conflict exists within tribes and, on the other, people have proven able to organize cooperation on a much larger scale than tribes. We include multi-level selection and gene-culture coevolution effects to account for some of these complexities.
    [Show full text]
  • Tracing Cultural Evolution Through Memetics
    Tracing Cultural Evolution Through Memetics Tiktik Dewi Sartika1 [email protected] Abstract Viewing human being, as a part of evolution process is still a controversial issue for some people, in fact the evolution runs. As a sociocultural entity, human being has distinctive characters in its evolution process. A Theory inherited from Darwin may have only been able to answer how a simple unit such genes evolve to such complex animal like human. Yet, how among those complex animals interact, communicate, and replicate idea in so forth formed a such self-organized sociocultural complexity, may only be, at the moment, answered by what Dawkins says as memetic evolution with meme as the replicator which, in near future, hoped to be a very potential tool for analyzing social phenomena. Keywords: human, evolution, meme, interaction, communication, replication, self-organization, complexity, social system, socioculture, Darwin, Dawkins. “We are built as gene machines and cultured as meme machines, but we have the power to turn against our creators. We alone on earth, can rebel against the tyranny of the selfish replicators” -Richard Dawkins, The Selfish Gene- 1 Background: Evolution, are we exceptional? Darwin’s theory of evolution is, though admitted insufficient to answer human complexity today, fundamental to the study of life in schools, labs and many academic institutions. As he wrote in the Origin of Species, simple animal through natural selection and variation, gradually and cumulatively evolved to such complex animal without the need of designer. During the evolution, one character or genetic information by chances is inherited then spread through successive generation in different circumstances causing variability.
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolution of Altruism and the Serial Rediscovery of the Role of Relatedness Tomas Kaya , Laurent Kellera,1 , and Laurent Lehmanna
    The evolution of altruism and the serial rediscovery of the role of relatedness Tomas Kaya , Laurent Kellera,1 , and Laurent Lehmanna aDepartment of Ecology and Evolution, University of Lausanne, 1015 Lausanne, Switzerland Edited by Joan E. Strassmann, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, and approved October 2, 2020 (received for review July 6, 2020) The genetic evolution of altruism (i.e., a behavior resulting in a understand the genetic evolution of altruism, and more gener- net reduction of the survival and/or reproduction of an actor to ally of any trait, it is crucial to consider the average fitness of benefit a recipient) once perplexed biologists because it seemed all individuals bearing a given allele responsible for producing paradoxical in a Darwinian world. More than half a century ago, a change in that trait. In particular, in a population of homo- W. D. Hamilton explained that when interacting individuals are geneous individuals, an altruism-inducing allele will increase in genetically related, alleles for altruism can be favored by selec- frequency when rb − c > 0, where −c is the average effect of tion because they are carried by individuals more likely to interact the altruism-inducing allele on the fitness of its bearer, b is the with other individuals carrying the alleles for altruism than ran- average effect on the fitness of recipients, and r is the genetic dom individuals in the population (“kin selection”). In recent relatedness between the actor and the recipients (7). decades, a substantial number of supposedly alternative path- Relatedness r is a regression coefficient measuring how the ways to altruism have been published, leading to controversies alleles in a particular individual covary in frequency with those surrounding explanations for the evolution of altruism.
    [Show full text]