The Illusion of Control, Superstitious Belief, and Optimism

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Illusion of Control, Superstitious Belief, and Optimism The Illusion of Control, Superstitious Belief, and Optimism JEFFREY RUDSKI Muhlenberg College Two hundred and seventy five participants each filled out three questionnaires exam- ining the illusion of control, optimism/pessimism, and paranormal belief. The illu- sion of control was related to overall paranormal belief, an effect primarily due to the superstition and precognition sub-scales on the PBS-R. Optimism was positively and pessimism negatively related to religiosity on the PBS-R, and pessimism was also positively related to the superstition sub-scale. No relationships between optimism or pessimism and the illusion of control were observed. Results are discussed in terms of superstition and the illusion of control both arising from circumstances that are inherently random or uncontrollable, and how multiple definitions of optimism or superstition could have effected the results. Two seemingly opposite sets of results have been reliably observed when people are faced with uncontrollable outcomes. On the one hand, there are reports of learned help- lessness, characterized by decreased motivation and cognitive capacity (Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978; Hiroto & Seligman, 1975). Conversely, Matute (1994, 1995) demonstrated that participants faced with an uncontrollable task tended to re- spond according to superstitious behavior and generate an illusion of control (i.e., the erroneous belief that one can influence outcomes in situations under which one has no control, see Langer, 1975). Similar superstitious behavior in the face of uncontrollable reinforcement has frequently been reported in the literature (e.g., Skinner, 1948; Ono, 1987; Wright, 1962; Rudski, Lischner, & Albert, 1999). These two sets of results appear contradictory. Matute (1994) suspected that the dis- crepant results (i.e., learned helplessness versus an illusion of control) arise from proce- dural differences. For instance, in Hiroto and Seligman's study, participants were given failure feedback in the form of turning on a light each time a response failed to produce the desired outcome. Conversely, failure feedback was not part of the procedure in studies where superstitious responding was observed. Matute suggested that the cogni- tive deficits observed in Hiroto and Seligman's study could be due to alternative expla- nations such as experimentally induced failure (e.g., Buchwald, 1978), egotism (Frankel & Snyder, 1978), or even extinction due to never having responses followed by desired outcomes. Finally, she suggested that learned helplessness and superstition (and the illusion of control) might represent opposite ends of the same continuum, with various factors and conditions determining which if either would be manifested following expo- sure to uncontrollable outcomes. Even under procedures conducive to its development, learned helplessness is usually observed in only two-thirds of human participants (Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, Current Psychology:Developmental, Learning, Personality,Social. Winter 2004, Vol. 22, No. 4, pp. 306- 315. Rudski 307 1978; Seligman, 1990; Thornton & Jacobs, 1978; and Hiroto, 1974). Moreover, for some participants helplessness is only transitory, whereas it can be quite persistent in others. To account for these individual differences, several personality factors have been hypothesized to play a role in mediating helplessness. First, the degree of a participants' belief in the paranormal may modify whether or not learned helplessness develops. Dudley (1999) had participants attempt to solve ana- grams following exposure to either solvable or unsolvable problems. Results showed that following unsolvable problems, the number of anagrams solved was positively related to level of paranormal beliefs. Moreover, paranormal beliefs increased follow- ing exposure to unsolvable, but not solvable problems. Dudley concluded that the adop- tion of paranormal beliefs can work to prevent the performance impairment often observed after instances of uncontrollability and failure. A second factor that might influence the development of helplessness is an individual's explanatory or attributional style (Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978). Attributional style can be thought of as the beliefs that people carry to explain why certain environmental events occur following their behaviors. Negative or pessimistic styles are linked to increased susceptibility to learned helplessness and depression, whereas positive or optimistic styles may provide "immunity" to helplessness or depression (Seligman, 1990). Several sets of studies suggest relationships between attributional style, optimism, and perceived controllability. Alloy and Abramson (1979) reported that depressed people (assumed to have a negative or pessimistic attributional style) were much less likely to show an illusion of control in a task where there was no contingency between their behavior and desired outcomes than were non-depressed participants. When combined with reports linking helplessness to depression (e.g., Seligman, 1990), these findings support the notion that learned helplessness and the illusion of control are in some way opposites. In studies examining relationships between optimism and controllability, optimistic bias is typically examined by having people make judgments regarding the comparative likelihood of future events. For example, participants might be asked about their chances of getting into an automobile accident relative to the average person. Sample means typically reveal above average estimates of positive outcomes (e.g., owning a large house), and below average estimates of negative outcomes (e.g., having a heart attack), particularly in instances where there is a high degree of perceived controllability (for reviews see Harris, 1996; McKenna, 1993). Similar patterns of results have been inter- preted as suggesting that people tend to be optimistically biased towards events believed to be controllable (e.g., Harris, 1996; Hoorens, 1994). In terms of dispositional tenden- cies, it might follow that there is a relationship between an illusion of control and optimism. The current study examined the interrelationships between perceived controllability, optimism, pessimism, and superstition. It is hypothesized that the illusion of control, superstition, and optimism will be positively related. 308 Current Psychology / Winter 2004 METHODS Participants Two hundred and seventy five undergraduate students (169 female and 106 male, age range 18-25 years old) at an American liberal arts college participated in the study. Participants were treated in accordance with the "Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct" (American Psychological Association, 1992) and the institution's Internal Review policy. Six participants did not fill out the Illusion of Control survey (see below). Surveys were disseminated at the end of various classes. Approximately half of the participants received course participation credits, while the other half filled out the surveys on a voluntary basis (dependent upon the professor). A written debrief- ing statement was disseminated to participants. Materials and Procedure Participants completed a three-part survey in a group setting. The first part allowed for the inference of the illusion of control. Participants were told to imagine that they had three lottery tickets: Ticket "A" was selected with their "lucky numbers." Ticket "B" was found blowing down the street. The numbers on Ticket "C" were selected by a computer. They were then asked (1) which ticket they would be most likely to give away, (2) which ticket they would be most likely to keep, and (3) whether or not they would exchange Ticket "A" ("lucky numbers") for two Ticket "C's" (computer gener- ated). For the first two questions, participants were given the option of responding "no preference." This procedure is based on one of Langer's (1975) studies in her seminal paper on the illusion of control. In her study, participants either chose their own lottery ticket or were handed one by the experimenter. Langer's dependent measure was whether or not a participant would be willing to exchange their ticket for another. She found that participants who selected their own tickets attached higher value to them than did par- ticipants who were handed their tickets by the experimenter despite the odds of winning being identical in each condition, suggesting an illusion of control. Optimism was assessed with the O/P instrument (Dember & Brooks, 1989; Dember, Martin, Hummer, Howe, & Melton, 1989). This measure was selected because it has been found to be both reliable and valid, and it provides separate scores for dispositional optimism and pessimism, which have been shown to be partially independent (Hummer, Dember, Melton, & Schefft, 1992). Thus, in addition to examining relationships be- tween optimism, the illusion of control and superstition, examination of pessimism in relation to these constructs was possible. Superstitious belief was assessed with Tobacyk's Revised Paranormal Belief Scale (1988). Participants rate their level of agreement with 26 statements (anchors 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree) relating to various paranormal beliefs. The Revised Paranormal Belief Scale (PBS-R) has seven sub-scales that include religiosity, psi-belief (e.g., telekinesis), witchcraft, superstition (e.g., bad luck stemming from black cats or broken mirrors), spiritualism (e.g., reincarnation), extraordinary life (e.g., the Loch Rudski 309 Ness Monster), and
Recommended publications
  • Power and Illusory Control
    Research Paper No. 2009 Illusory Control: A Generative Force Behind Power’s Far-Reaching Effects Nathanael J. Fast Deborah H. Gruenfeld Niro Sivanathan Adam D. Galinsky December 2008 R ESEARCH P APER S ERIES Electronic copy available at: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1314952 Running Head: POWER AND ILLUSORY CONTROL Illusory Control: A Generative Force Behind Power’s Far-Reaching Effects Nathanael J. Fast and Deborah H Gruenfeld Stanford University Niro Sivanathan London Business School Adam D. Galinsky Northwestern University *In press at Psychological Science Please address correspondence concerning this article to Nathanael J. Fast, Department of Organizational Behavior, Stanford University, 518 Memorial Way, Stanford, CA 94305; email: [email protected]. Electronic copy available at: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1314952 Power and Illusory Control 2 Abstract Three experiments demonstrated that the experience of power leads to an illusion of personal control. Regardless of whether power was experientially primed (Experiments 1 and 3) or manipulated through manager-subordinate roles (Experiment 2), it led to perceived control over outcomes that were beyond the reach of the powerholder. Furthermore, this illusory control mediated the influence of power on several self-enhancement and approach-related effects found in the power literature, including optimism (Experiment 2), self-esteem (Experiment 3), and action-orientation (Experiment 3), demonstrating its theoretical importance as a generative cause and driving force behind many of power’s far-reaching effects. A fourth experiment ruled out an alternative explanation: that positive mood, rather than illusory control, is at the root of power’s effects. The discussion considers implications for existing and future research on the psychology of power, perceived control, and positive illusions.
    [Show full text]
  • Impact of Illusion of Control on Perceived Efficiency in Pakistani Financial Markets
    Impact of Illusion…. Abasyn Journal of Social Sciences Vol. 5 No. 2 Impact of Illusion of Control on Perceived Efficiency in Pakistani Financial Markets Sidra Ajmal1 Maria Mufti2 Dr. Zulfiqar Ali Shah3 Abstract The purpose of this study is to test impact of illusion of control on perceived market efficiency. On the basis of literature review, questionnaire was developed. The study is carried out on sample of convenience of investors, financial analyst and finance scholars of Islamabad/ Rawalpindi. Questionnaire comprises of 15 items related to three forms of market efficiency and particular bias (illusion of control). The findings of the regression show significant results. This means that illusion of control has impact on perceived efficiency of Pakistani financial market. This study focuses on only individual located in Islamabad/Rawalpindi, and the sample size is also small due to time constraints. Further research can be done on a bigger scale to get the more general conclusions. This study will help investors to identify their biases and then formulate different strategies to reduce their irrational behavior. Key words: Illusion of control, market efficiency 1Sidra Ajmal, MS (Scholar), SZABIST, Islamabad. 2Maria Mufti, MS (Scholar), SZABIST, Islamabad. 3Dr. Zulfiqar Ali Shah, Associate Professor, SZABIST, Islamabad. Illusion of control is a psychological term referring to the tendency of people to overestimate their abilities to control any event, where as market efficiency is the phenomena primarily explained by Fama (1970). He had created a hypothesis that Sidra Ajmal, Maria Mufti, & Dr. Zulfiqar Ali Shah 100 Impact of Illusion…. Abasyn Journal of Social Sciences Vol. 5 No.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Psychology Glossary
    Social Psychology Glossary This glossary defines many of the key terms used in class lectures and assigned readings. A Altruism—A motive to increase another's welfare without conscious regard for one's own self-interest. Availability Heuristic—A cognitive rule, or mental shortcut, in which we judge how likely something is by how easy it is to think of cases. Attractiveness—Having qualities that appeal to an audience. An appealing communicator (often someone similar to the audience) is most persuasive on matters of subjective preference. Attribution Theory—A theory about how people explain the causes of behavior—for example, by attributing it either to "internal" dispositions (e.g., enduring traits, motives, values, and attitudes) or to "external" situations. Automatic Processing—"Implicit" thinking that tends to be effortless, habitual, and done without awareness. B Behavioral Confirmation—A type of self-fulfilling prophecy in which people's social expectations lead them to behave in ways that cause others to confirm their expectations. Belief Perseverance—Persistence of a belief even when the original basis for it has been discredited. Bystander Effect—The tendency for people to be less likely to help someone in need when other people are present than when they are the only person there. Also known as bystander inhibition. C Catharsis—Emotional release. The catharsis theory of aggression is that people's aggressive drive is reduced when they "release" aggressive energy, either by acting aggressively or by fantasizing about aggression. Central Route to Persuasion—Occurs when people are convinced on the basis of facts, statistics, logic, and other types of evidence that support a particular position.
    [Show full text]
  • Infographic I.10
    The Digital Health Revolution: Leaving No One Behind The global AI in healthcare market is growing fast, with an expected increase from $4.9 billion in 2020 to $45.2 billion by 2026. There are new solutions introduced every day that address all areas: from clinical care and diagnosis, to remote patient monitoring to EHR support, and beyond. But, AI is still relatively new to the industry, and it can be difficult to determine which solutions can actually make a difference in care delivery and business operations. 59 Jan 2021 % of Americans believe returning Jan-June 2019 to pre-coronavirus life poses a risk to health and well being. 11 41 % % ...expect it will take at least 6 The pandemic has greatly increased the 65 months before things get number of US adults reporting depression % back to normal (updated April and/or anxiety.5 2021).4 Up to of consumers now interested in telehealth going forward. $250B 76 57% of providers view telehealth more of current US healthcare spend % favorably than they did before COVID-19.7 could potentially be virtualized.6 The dramatic increase in of Medicare primary care visits the conducted through 90% $3.5T telehealth has shown longevity, with rates in annual U.S. health expenditures are for people with chronic and mental health conditions. since April 2020 0.1 43.5 leveling off % % Most of these can be prevented by simple around 30%.8 lifestyle changes and regular health screenings9 Feb. 2020 Apr. 2020 OCCAM’S RAZOR • CONJUNCTION FALLACY • DELMORE EFFECT • LAW OF TRIVIALITY • COGNITIVE FLUENCY • BELIEF BIAS • INFORMATION BIAS Digital health ecosystems are transforming• AMBIGUITY BIAS • STATUS medicineQUO BIAS • SOCIAL COMPARISONfrom BIASa rea• DECOYctive EFFECT • REACTANCEdiscipline, • REVERSE PSYCHOLOGY • SYSTEM JUSTIFICATION • BACKFIRE EFFECT • ENDOWMENT EFFECT • PROCESSING DIFFICULTY EFFECT • PSEUDOCERTAINTY EFFECT • DISPOSITION becoming precise, preventive,EFFECT • ZERO-RISK personalized, BIAS • UNIT BIAS • IKEA EFFECT and • LOSS AVERSION participatory.
    [Show full text]
  • Effects of Mindset on Positive Illusions
    EffectEffectss ooff MindseMindsett oonn PositivPositivee IllusionIllusionss ShelleSheJleyy EE.. TayloTaylorr PetePeterr MM.. GollwitzeGoUwitzerr UniversitUniversityy oofCalifomia,f California, LoLoss AngeleAngeless UniversitUniversityy ooff KonstanKonstanzz SS.. EE.. TayloTayklrandr and JJ.. DD.. Brown'Brown'ss (1988(1988)) positioposilionn thalhalt mentallmentallyy healthhealthyy peoplpoopIee exhibiexhibitt positivpositivee illusioni11lol5ionss raiseraisess aI dilemmadikmma:: HowHow dodo pewitpeople funC1Wnfunction effectivelyeffectively ifthc:irif their perceptionsperceptions arcare positivelypositively biased?biased? UsingUsing Gollwitzer'Go/lwiucr'ss deliberative-implementadeliberative-implementall mindseffiindsett distinctiondistinction,, wv.ee assesseassessedd whethewhetherr peoplpeoplee iinn a deliber­deliber- ativativee mindsemioosett shoshoww leslesss evidenceevidence ofof positivpositivee illusionillusionss thathann peoplpc~e inin anan implementalimplemental mindsetmind$Cl... ParticPartk·- ipantipanlSs completecompletedd aa mindsemindsett tastaskk anandd assessmentassessmentss ooff moodmood,, self-perceptionsself-perceptions,, anandd perceiveperceivedd (invul(in)vul­- nerabilitnerabilityy t10o riskrisk.. DeliberatioDeliberationn leledd tloworsenedo worsened moodmood,, greatevealerr perceiveperceivedd riskrisk,, anandd poorepoorerr self-percepself.percep­- tionstiool,, relativrelativee ttoo implementationimplementation;; controcontroll (n(noo mindsetmindset)) participantparticipantss typicalltypicallyy scorescoredd iinn betweenbet~n.
    [Show full text]
  • Illusion and Well-Being: a Social Psychological Perspective on Mental Health
    Psyehologlcal Bulletin Copyright 1988 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1988, Vol. 103, No. 2, 193-210 0033-2909/88/$00.75 Illusion and Well-Being: A Social Psychological Perspective on Mental Health Shelley E. Taylor Jonathon D. Brown University of California, Los Angeles Southern Methodist University Many prominenttheorists have argued that accurate perceptions of the self, the world, and the future are essential for mental health. Yet considerable research evidence suggests that overly positive self- evaluations, exaggerated perceptions of control or mastery, and unrealistic optimism are characteris- tic of normal human thought. Moreover, these illusions appear to promote other criteria of mental health, including the ability to care about others, the ability to be happy or contented, and the ability to engage in productive and creative work. These strategies may succeed, in large part, because both the social world and cognitive-processingmechanisms impose filters on incoming information that distort it in a positive direction; negativeinformation may be isolated and represented in as unthreat- ening a manner as possible. These positive illusions may be especially useful when an individual receives negative feedback or is otherwise threatened and may be especially adaptive under these circumstances. Decades of psychological wisdom have established contact dox: How can positive misperceptions of one's self and the envi- with reality as a hallmark of mental health. In this view, the ronment be adaptive when accurate information processing wcU-adjusted person is thought to engage in accurate reality seems to be essential for learning and successful functioning in testing,whereas the individual whose vision is clouded by illu- the world? Our primary goal is to weave a theoretical context sion is regarded as vulnerable to, ifnot already a victim of, men- for thinking about mental health.
    [Show full text]
  • John Collins, President, Forensic Foundations Group
    On Bias in Forensic Science National Commission on Forensic Science – May 12, 2014 56-year-old Vatsala Thakkar was a doctor in India but took a job as a convenience store cashier to help pay family expenses. She was stabbed to death outside her store trying to thwart a theft in November 2008. Bloody Footwear Impression Bloody Tire Impression What was the threat? 1. We failed to ask ourselves if this was a footwear impression. 2. The appearance of the impression combined with the investigator’s interpretation created prejudice. The accuracy of our analysis became threatened by our prejudice. Types of Cognitive Bias Available at: http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/List_of_cognitive_biases | Accessed on April 14, 2014 Anchoring or focalism Hindsight bias Pseudocertainty effect Illusory superiority Levels-of-processing effect Attentional bias Hostile media effect Reactance Ingroup bias List-length effect Availability heuristic Hot-hand fallacy Reactive devaluation Just-world phenomenon Misinformation effect Availability cascade Hyperbolic discounting Recency illusion Moral luck Modality effect Backfire effect Identifiable victim effect Restraint bias Naive cynicism Mood-congruent memory bias Bandwagon effect Illusion of control Rhyme as reason effect Naïve realism Next-in-line effect Base rate fallacy or base rate neglect Illusion of validity Risk compensation / Peltzman effect Outgroup homogeneity bias Part-list cueing effect Belief bias Illusory correlation Selective perception Projection bias Peak-end rule Bias blind spot Impact bias Semmelweis
    [Show full text]
  • The Self-Attribution Bias and Paranormal Beliefs ⇑ Michiel Van Elk
    Consciousness and Cognition 49 (2017) 313–321 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Consciousness and Cognition journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/concog The self-attribution bias and paranormal beliefs ⇑ Michiel van Elk Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands Amsterdam Brain and Cognition Center, University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands article info abstract Article history: The present study investigated the relation between paranormal beliefs, illusory control Received 8 December 2016 and the self-attribution bias, i.e., the motivated tendency to attribute positive outcomes Revised 2 February 2017 to oneself while negative outcomes are externalized. Visitors of a psychic fair played a card Accepted 2 February 2017 guessing game and indicated their perceived control over randomly selected cards as a function of the congruency and valence of the card. A stronger self-attribution bias was observed for paranormal believers compared to skeptics and this bias was specifically Keywords: related to traditional religious beliefs and belief in superstition. No relation between para- Paranormal beliefs normal beliefs and illusory control was found. Self-report measures indicated that paranor- Self-attribution bias Cognitive biases mal beliefs were associated to being raised in a spiritual family and to anomalous Individual differences experiences during childhood. Thereby this study suggests that paranormal beliefs are Superstition related to specific cognitive biases that in turn are shaped by socio-cultural factors. Illusion of control Ó 2017 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction Paranormal beliefs refer to an eclectic range of New Age beliefs and practices, involving belief in Psi, precognition, witch- craft, superstition, as well as telekinesis and channeling (Lindeman & Svedholm, 2012).
    [Show full text]
  • Optimal Illusion of Control and Related Perception Biases
    OPTIMAL ILLUSION OF CONTROL AND RELATED PERCEPTION BIASES Olivier Gossner Jakub Steiner CERGE Charles University Center for Economic Research and Graduate Education Academy of Sciences of the Czech Republic Economics Institute EI WORKING PAPER SERIES (ISSN 1211-3298) Electronic Version 571 Working Paper Series 571 (ISSN 1211-3298) Optimal Illusion of Control and Related Perception Biases Olivier Gossner Jakub Steiner CERGE-EI Prague, September 2016 ISBN 978-80-7343-378-9 (Univerzita Karlova v Praze, Centrum pro ekonomický výzkum a doktorské studium) ISBN 978-80-7344-394-8 (Národohospodářský ústav AV ČR, v. v. i.) Optimal Illusion of Control and Related Perception Biases∗ Olivier Gossnery CREST, CNRS { Ecole´ Polytechnique, Universit´eParis Saclay and London School of Economics Jakub Steinerz University of Edinburgh and Cerge-Ei September 2016 Abstract We study perception biases arising under second-best perception strategies. An agent correctly observes a parameter that is payoff-relevant in many decision problems that she encounters in her environment but is unable to retain all the information un- til her decision. A designer of the decision process chooses a perception strategy that determines the distribution of the perception errors. If some information loss is un- avoidable due to cognition constraints, then (under additional conditions) the optimal perception strategy exhibits the illusion of control, overconfidence, and optimism. ∗We thank Andrew Clausen, Bard Harstad, Yuval Heller, Ed Hopkins, Drazen Prelec, J´ozsefS´akovics, Colin Stewart, Andy Zapechelnyuk, and the audiences at YEM 2016, BRIC 2016, GAMES 2016 and the Limited Cognition workshop in Barcelona for comments. Ludmila Matyskov´ahas provided excellent research assistance and Judith Levi has carefully edited the text.
    [Show full text]
  • I Am Fine but You Are Not: Optimistic Bias and Illusion of Control on Information Security Hyeun-Suk Rhee University of Texas at Dallas
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by AIS Electronic Library (AISeL) Association for Information Systems AIS Electronic Library (AISeL) International Conference on Information Systems ICIS 2005 Proceedings (ICIS) December 2005 I Am Fine but You Are Not: Optimistic Bias and Illusion of Control on Information Security Hyeun-Suk Rhee University of Texas at Dallas Young Ryu University of Texas at Dallas Cheong-Tag Kim Seoul National University Follow this and additional works at: http://aisel.aisnet.org/icis2005 Recommended Citation Rhee, Hyeun-Suk; Ryu, Young; and Kim, Cheong-Tag, "I Am Fine but You Are Not: Optimistic Bias and Illusion of Control on Information Security" (2005). ICIS 2005 Proceedings. 32. http://aisel.aisnet.org/icis2005/32 This material is brought to you by the International Conference on Information Systems (ICIS) at AIS Electronic Library (AISeL). It has been accepted for inclusion in ICIS 2005 Proceedings by an authorized administrator of AIS Electronic Library (AISeL). For more information, please contact [email protected]. I AM FINE BUT YOU ARE NOT: OPTIMISTIC BIAS AND ILLUSION OF CONTROL ON INFORMATION SECURITY Hyeun-Suk Rhee Young U. Ryu School of Management School of Management University of Texas at Dallas University of Texas at Dallas Dallas, TX U.S.A. Dallas, TX U.S.A. [email protected] [email protected] Cheong-Tag Kim Department of Psychology Seoul National University Seoul, Korea [email protected] Abstract Information security is a critically important issue in current networked business and work environments. While there is extensive publicity on the increasing incidents of numerous information security breaches and their serious consequences, recent surveys and research on information security repeatedly identify the low levels of user and managerial awareness as a key obstacle to achieving a good information security posture.
    [Show full text]
  • 50 Cognitive and Affective Biases in Medicine (Alphabetically)
    50 Cognitive and Affective Biases in Medicine (alphabetically) Pat Croskerry MD, PhD, FRCP(Edin), Critical Thinking Program, Dalhousie University Aggregate bias: when physicians believe that aggregated data, such as those used to develop clinical practice guidelines, do not apply to individual patients (especially their own), they are exhibiting the aggregate fallacy. The belief that their patients are atypical or somehow exceptional, may lead to errors of commission, e.g. ordering x-rays or other tests when guidelines indicate none are required. Ambiguity effect: there is often an irreducible uncertainty in medicine and ambiguity is associated with uncertainty. The ambiguity effect is due to decision makers avoiding options when the probability is unknown. In considering options on a differential diagnosis, for example, this would be illustrated by a tendency to select options for which the probability of a particular outcome is known over an option for which the probability is unknown. The probability may be unknown because of lack of knowledge or because the means to obtain the probability (a specific test, or imaging) is unavailable. The cognitive miser function (choosing an option that requires less cognitive effort) may also be at play here. Anchoring: the tendency to perceptually lock on to salient features in the patient’s initial presentation too early in the diagnostic process, and failure to adjust this initial impression in the light of later information. This bias may be severely compounded by the confirmation bias. Ascertainment bias: when a physician’s thinking is shaped by prior expectation; stereotyping and gender bias are both good examples. Attentional bias: the tendency to believe there is a relationship between two variables when instances are found of both being present.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Embrace Your Cognitive Bias
    1 Embrace Your Cognitive Bias http://blog.beaufortes.com/2007/06/embrace-your-co.html Cognitive Biases are distortions in the way humans see things in comparison to the purely logical way that mathematics, economics, and yes even project management would have us look at things. The problem is not that we have them… most of them are wired deep into our brains following millions of years of evolution. The problem is that we don’t know about them, and consequently don’t take them into account when we have to make important decisions. (This area is so important that Daniel Kahneman won a Nobel Prize in 2002 for work tying non-rational decision making, and cognitive bias, to mainstream economics) People don’t behave rationally, they have emotions, they can be inspired, they have cognitive bias! Tying that into how we run projects (project leadership as a compliment to project management) can produce results you wouldn’t believe. You have to know about them to guard against them, or use them (but that’s another article)... So let’s get more specific. After the jump, let me show you a great list of cognitive biases. I’ll bet that there are at least a few that you haven’t heard of before! Decision making and behavioral biases Bandwagon effect — the tendency to do (or believe) things because many other people do (or believe) the same. Related to groupthink, herd behaviour, and manias. Bias blind spot — the tendency not to compensate for one’s own cognitive biases. Choice-supportive bias — the tendency to remember one’s choices as better than they actually were.
    [Show full text]