When the Left Brain Is Not Right the Right Brain May Be Left: Report of Personal Experience of Occipital Hemianopia

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

When the Left Brain Is Not Right the Right Brain May Be Left: Report of Personal Experience of Occipital Hemianopia J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.67.2.169 on 1 August 1999. Downloaded from J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1999;67:169–173 169 When the left brain is not right the right brain may be left: report of personal experience of occipital hemianopia Monroe Cole Abstract On the second day I became aware of visual Objectives—To make a personal report of hallucinations. These were especially promi- a hemianopia due to an occipital infarct, nent between 3 January and 10 January. (They sustained by a professor of neurology. are, however, not entirely gone after 2 years and Methods—Verbatim observation of neuro- appear to be fixed.) logical phenomena recorded during the My wife suggested, and continually encour- acute illness. aged me, not to miss the opportunity to record Results—Hemianopia, visual hallucina- my observations. tions, and non-occipital deficits without At first, virtually all of the hallucinations extraoccipital lesions on MRI, are de- were in bright colours, but some days they were scribed and discussed. in muted grey, brown, or yellow. These duller Conclusions—Hemianopia, due to an oc- colours would usually appear as swirling cipital infarct, without alexia, is not a dis- drapes, always in the right visual field, but ability which precludes a normal tending to interfere with vision in general. Only professional career. Neurorehabilitation twice did I see objects in black and white. has not been necessary. Sometimes the colour was not quite appropri- (J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1999;67:169–173) ate. Often there was a pony with his head cra- Keywords: occipital; hemianopia; visual hallucinations. dled in my right arm. I recognised the pony as the one owned by my granddaughter, but the colour was wrong. Personal reports of illness, disability, and the Most of the hallucinatory material was experience of being a patient, recorded by a recognisable but, as in the example above, the trained observer, are of use to the medical pro- colours did not always fit the object. I saw many fession. None of us plan to make such personal horses, dogs, and an even greater number of observations, but once the opportunity arises people, most of whom I could recognise. They advantage should be taken of it. moved, danced and swirled, but their purpose I am a right handed neurologist who under- was unclear. The hallucinations were neither went an aortic valve replacement with a St Jude horrendous nor frightening. Many hallucina- valve, as well as a double coronary artery tions were annoying even though I realised that bypass graft, on 2 January 1997 when aged 63. they were unreal. One hallucination often http://jnnp.bmj.com/ A self performed neurological examination in recurred. There would be a hand thrust out the recovery room disclosed a right hemiano- towards me; it would get larger, become pia. Brain MRI and neurological examination distorted, and then shake in a stereotyped (fig 1 and fig 2) demonstrated only a left manner either in the horizontal or sometimes occipital infarct on both banks of the calcarine in the vertical plane. Although the hallucina- fissure and the dense right hemianopia. tions interfered with vision, even at their worst Department of Neurology, Case on September 24, 2021 by guest. Protected copyright. Western Reserve University and University Hospitals of Cleveland, 11100 Euclid Avenue, Cleveland, OH 44106–5000, USA M Cole Correspondence to: Dr Monroe Cole, Department of Neurology, Case Western Reserve University and University Hospitals of Cleveland, 11100 Euclid Avenue, Cleveland, OH 44106–5000, USA. Telephone 001 216 844 3769; fax 001 216 844 5066. Received 16 October 1998 and in final form 5 March 1999 Accepted 5 March 1999 Figure 1 Brain MRI. (A) Sagittal view. Note infarct on both banks of the calcarine fissure. (B) Horizontal view. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.67.2.169 on 1 August 1999. Downloaded from 170 Cole I could continue to do some visual tasks such as the right visual field, noted mostly inferiorly. shaving. These are not annoying but occasionally Hallucinations did not occur during sleep distract me while I am reading. and could be diVerentiated from dreams. Dur- Generally, I am unaware of my hemianopia ing dreams, visual images were bilateral—that unless I attend to it or strike things on my right is, not confined to the right visual field—and side. I may start a line of writing on the wrong movements were not stereotyped. line above if the left side of the line is blank, In contradistinction to the hemianopic since I may be unaware of writing on the right hallucinations described by Kölmel,1 my hallu- side of the line. cinations were (1) not motionless; (2) not col- On a few occasions illusions were noted. At ourless; (3) not smaller than normal; (4) and the end of my hospital bed there was a clamp not extinguished by eye movement. Finally, (5) which, when I first began looking at it, seemed my hallucinations were virtually continuous to be a telescope on a military gunsight. After I during the first few weeks. saw what this actually was, I never had this On several occasions I saw what seemed to problem again. Much more unusual were be a fortification outline. Unlike my migrain- several episodes of false perception of colour. ous fortification scotomata, however, this did On one such occasion I painted an outdoor not move across the visual field but stayed in chair white. A few weeks later the house was one spot until it disappeared. Furthermore, the professionally stained white, and a deck was edges of the scotomata did not twirl or glisten, stained battleship grey. I looked at the chair, as my migrainous scotomata always do. which was some distance from the house, and Certain factors made the hallucinations wondered to myself why the painters had both- worse, such as tachycardia, increased heart ered to repaint my chair the same grey. I walked failure, or an arrhythmia requiring electro- over to inspect it carefully and found that it was as white as the day I painted it. conversion—which occurred five times post- I never read throughout my stay in operatively. During at least some of these hospital—the first time I ever remember episodes—for example, after cardioversion— I passing time without reading. In fact, I noted, was seen to be mildly confused. The hallucina- as did observant visitors, that I usually kept my tions were more prominent as the day went on eyes closed. I found vision bothersome al- and normally worse when I was taken to an though I never had diplopia. The exact quality unfamiliar environment. which bothered me is diYcult to analyse. I On discharge, while being driven home, I simply found it uncomfortable to see. After was constantly surprised by tall, hallucinatory discharge I was constantly encouraged to read objects along the right side of the road. Some- and was given a novel with large print. My times they would be on the right side of a bend enjoyment of reading was limited but rapidly in the road, causing me to wonder if the road improved, so that after the first book was com- were blocked. The further the car went, pleted I began to read for real pleasure. Medi- however, the more certain I became that our cal literature came rapidly thereafter. I had dif- progress would not be impeded. These halluci- ficulty reading in a moving car, mainly because nations were stereotyped: large tan or cream I would lose the line in which I was reading, but coloured buildings reminding me of old public after some weeks I could and would read con- http://jnnp.bmj.com/ buildings or churches one would see driving in stantly in a car. Italy. Such hallucinations persist, but only occasionally, and are very brief and evanescent. A further peculiarity of these roadside hallu- Discussion cinations are that they have only occurred while Hemianopic hallucinations occur in about an I am heading east, as they did when I first was eighth of hemianopic patients.1 driven home from the hospital. I have never In some patients the hemianopic hallucina- been aware of them while I am heading west— tions occur at the onset of the ictus—for exam- on September 24, 2021 by guest. Protected copyright. that is, away from home. Other hallucinatory ple, case 8 of Lance2—but usually there is a phenomena persist, especially diagonal lines in latent period before onset. Most hallucinations Figure 2 Visual fields by Goldmann perimetry. A. od 3 May 1997; B. os 3 May 1997. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.67.2.169 on 1 August 1999. Downloaded from When the left brain is not right the right brain may be left 171 consist of familiar people, animals, or objects; Consider, for example, some of the research on some have no obvious familiarity. Lance2 states macular sparing or splitting. Holmes11 14 that hallucinations consist of people and things showed that the central 10° of the visual field is rather than landscapes and scenes. projected to the most posterior part of the stri- ate cortex, including that part of the posterola- 7 WHERE IN THE BRAIN ARE HEMIANOPIC teral occipital tip. Cogan indicated that macu- HALLUCINATIONS ORGANISED? lar sparing is more common with parietal or They obviously do not arise from destroyed parieto-occipital lesions and less common with visual cortex. I would predict that they arise temporal or occipital lesions. He also states that from remaining visual cortex or, more likely, macular sparing is absent with “paracentral from extrastriate cortex.3 Manford and scotomas from lesions in the occipital pole.” Andermann4 have recently reviewed various But cases 14, 15, and 19 of Holmes and Lister10 putative mechanisms of visual hallucinations.
Recommended publications
  • Vision Research Xxx (2015) Xxx–Xxx
    Vision Research xxx (2015) xxx–xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Altered white matter in early visual pathways of humans with amblyopia ⇑ Brian Allen a, Daniel P. Spiegel c,d, Benjamin Thompson c,e, Franco Pestilli b,1, Bas Rokers a, ,1 a Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI, United States b Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, United States c Optometry and Vision Science, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand d McGill Vision Research, Department of Ophthalmology, McGill University, Montreal, Canada e Optometry and Vision Science, University of Waterloo, Canada article info abstract Article history: Amblyopia is a visual disorder caused by poorly coordinated binocular input during development. Little is Received 8 September 2014 known about the impact of amblyopia on the white matter within the visual system. We studied the Received in revised form 16 December 2014 properties of six major visual white-matter pathways in a group of adults with amblyopia (n = 10) and Available online xxxx matched controls (n = 10) using diffusion weighted imaging (DWI) and fiber tractography. While we did not find significant differences in diffusion properties in cortico-cortical pathways, patients with Keywords: amblyopia exhibited increased mean diffusivity in thalamo-cortical visual pathways. These findings sug- Amblyopia gest that amblyopia may systematically alter the white matter properties of early visual pathways. White matter 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Tractography Ó Diffusion-MRI 1. Introduction absent sensitivity to binocular disparity (Holmes & Clarke, 2006; Li et al., 2011). Amblyopia is a developmental disorder that occurs when the The neuro-anatomical consequences of amblyopia in humans visual input from the two eyes is poorly correlated during early are less established than the functional deficits.
    [Show full text]
  • (Homonymous Hemianopsia): Tapping Into Blindsight
    CORE Metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk Provided by Elsevier - Publisher Connector Journal of Medical Hypotheses and Ideas (2015) 9,S8–S13 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Journal of Medical Hypotheses and Ideas journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jmhi REGULAR ARTICLE Enhancing visual performance in individuals with cortical visual impairment (homonymous hemianopsia): Tapping into blindsight Faith A. Birnbaum a, Steven A. Hackley b, Lenworth N. Johnson a,* a Neuro-Ophthalmology Unit, Department of Ophthalmology, The Warren Alpert Medical School of Brown University/Lifespan/Rhode Island Hospital, Providence, RI, United States b Department of Psychological Sciences of the University of Missouri Columbia, Columbia, MO, United States Received 2 October 2015; revised 29 November 2015; accepted 15 December 2015 Available online 22 January 2016 KEYWORDS Abstract Homonymous hemianopsia is a type of cortical blindness in which vision is lost Blindsight; completely or partially in the left half or the right half of the field of vision. It is prevalent in Cortical blindness; approximately 12% of traumatic brain injury and 35% of strokes. Patients often experience Homonymous hemianopsia; difficulty with activities such as ambulating, eating, reading, and driving. Due to the high prevalence Augmented virtual reality; of homonymous hemianopsia and its associated difficulties, it is imperative to find methods for Vision restoration therapy visual rehabilitation in this condition. Traditional methods such as prism glasses can cause visual confusion and result in patient noncompliance. There is a large unmet medical need for improving this condition. In this article, we propose that modifying visual stimuli to activate non-cortical areas of visual processing, such as lateral geniculate nucleus and superior colliculus, may result in increased visual awareness.
    [Show full text]
  • Abnormal Retinotopic Representations in Human Visual Cortex Revealed by Fmri
    Acta Psychologica 107 -2001) 229±247 www.elsevier.com/locate/actpsy Abnormal retinotopic representations in human visual cortex revealed by fMRI Antony B. Morland a,*, Heidi A. Baseler c, Michael B. Homann a, Lindsay T. Sharpe b, Brian A. Wandell c a Psychology Department, University of London, Royal Holloway, Egham, Surrey TW20 0EX, UK b Psychology Department, Newcastle University, Newcastle, UK c Psychology Department, Stanford University, CA 93155, USA Received 1 September 2000; received in revised form 8 December 2000; accepted 11 December 2000 Abstract The representation of the visual ®eld in early visual areas is retinotopic. The point-to-point relationship on the retina is therefore maintained on the convoluted cortical surface. Func- tional magnetic resonance imaging -fMRI) has been able to demonstrate the retinotopic representation of the visual ®eld in occipital cortex of normal subjects. Furthermore, visual areas that are retinotopic can be identi®ed on computationally ¯attened cortical maps on the basis of positions of the vertical and horizontal meridians. Here, we investigate abnormal retinotopic representations in human visual cortex with fMRI. We present three case studies in which patients with visual disorders are investigated. We have tested a subject who only possesses operating rod photoreceptors. We ®nd in this case that the cortex undergoes a re- mapping whereby regions that would normally represent central ®eld locations now map more peripheral positions in the visual ®eld. In a human albino we also ®nd abnormal visual cortical activity. Monocular stimulation of each hemi®eld resulted in activations in the hemisphere contralateral to the stimulated eye. This is consistent with abnormal decussation at the optic chiasm in albinism.
    [Show full text]
  • Neuropsychologia 128 (2019) 150–165
    Neuropsychologia 128 (2019) 150–165 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Neuropsychologia journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia Psychophysical and neuroimaging responses to moving stimuli in a patient with the Riddoch phenomenon due to bilateral visual cortex lesions T Michael J. Arcaroa,b,c, Lore Thalerd, Derek J. Quinlane, Simona Monacof, Sarah Khang, Kenneth F. Valyearh, Rainer Goebeli, Gordon N. Duttonj, Melvyn A. Goodalee,g, Sabine Kastnerb,c, ⁎ Jody C. Culhame,g, a Department of Neurobiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA b Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, USA c Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton, NJ, USA d Department of Psychology, Durham University, Durham, UK e Brain and Mind Institute, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada f Center for Mind and Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Trento, Italy g Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada h School of Psychology, University of Bangor, Bangor, Wales i Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands j Department of Visual Science, Glasgow Caledonian University, Glasgow, UK ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Keywords: Patients with injury to early visual cortex or its inputs can display the Riddoch phenomenon: preserved Riddoch phenomenon awareness for moving but not stationary stimuli. We provide a detailed case report of a patient with the Riddoch Blindsight phenomenon, MC. MC has extensive bilateral lesions to occipitotemporal cortex that include most early visual Vision cortex and complete blindness in visual field perimetry testing with static targets. Nevertheless, she shows a Motion perception remarkably robust preserved ability to perceive motion, enabling her to navigate through cluttered environ- FMRI ments and perform actions like catching moving balls.
    [Show full text]
  • A Dictionary of Neurological Signs
    FM.qxd 9/28/05 11:10 PM Page i A DICTIONARY OF NEUROLOGICAL SIGNS SECOND EDITION FM.qxd 9/28/05 11:10 PM Page iii A DICTIONARY OF NEUROLOGICAL SIGNS SECOND EDITION A.J. LARNER MA, MD, MRCP(UK), DHMSA Consultant Neurologist Walton Centre for Neurology and Neurosurgery, Liverpool Honorary Lecturer in Neuroscience, University of Liverpool Society of Apothecaries’ Honorary Lecturer in the History of Medicine, University of Liverpool Liverpool, U.K. FM.qxd 9/28/05 11:10 PM Page iv A.J. Larner, MA, MD, MRCP(UK), DHMSA Walton Centre for Neurology and Neurosurgery Liverpool, UK Library of Congress Control Number: 2005927413 ISBN-10: 0-387-26214-8 ISBN-13: 978-0387-26214-7 Printed on acid-free paper. © 2006, 2001 Springer Science+Business Media, Inc. All rights reserved. This work may not be translated or copied in whole or in part without the written permission of the publisher (Springer Science+Business Media, Inc., 233 Spring Street, New York, NY 10013, USA), except for brief excerpts in connection with reviews or scholarly analysis. Use in connection with any form of information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software, or by similar or dis- similar methodology now known or hereafter developed is forbidden. The use in this publication of trade names, trademarks, service marks, and similar terms, even if they are not identified as such, is not to be taken as an expression of opinion as to whether or not they are subject to propri- etary rights. While the advice and information in this book are believed to be true and accurate at the date of going to press, neither the authors nor the editors nor the publisher can accept any legal responsibility for any errors or omis- sions that may be made.
    [Show full text]
  • Visual Command Hallucinations in a Patient with Pure Alexia D H Ffytche, J M Lappin, M Philpot
    80 PAPER J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as on 5 January 2004. Downloaded from Visual command hallucinations in a patient with pure alexia D H ffytche, J M Lappin, M Philpot ............................................................................................................................... J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2004;75:80–86 Around 25% of patients with visual hallucinations secondary to eye disease report hallucinations of text. The hallucinated text conveys little if any meaning, typically consisting of individual letters, words, or nonsense letter strings (orthographic hallucinations). A patient is described with textual visual hallucinations of a very different linguistic content following bilateral occipito-temporal infarcts. The hallucinations consisted of grammatically correct, meaningful written sentences or phrases, often in the See end of article for second person and with a threatening and command-like nature (syntacto-semantic visual hallucinations). authors’ affiliations A detailed phenomenological interview and visual psychophysical testing were undertaken. The patient ....................... showed a classical ventral occipito-temporal syndrome with achromatopsia, prosopagnosia, and Correspondence to: associative visual agnosia. Of particular significance was the presence of pure alexia. Illusions of colour Dr D H ffytche, Institute of induced by monochromatic gratings and a novel motion–direction illusion were also observed, both Psychiatry, De Crespigny consistent with the residual capacities of the patient’s spared visual cortex. The content of orthographic Park, Denmark Hill, London SE5 8AF, UK; visual hallucinations matches the known specialisations of an area in the left posterior fusiform gyrus—the [email protected] visual word form area (VWFA)—suggesting the two are related. The VWFA is unlikely to be responsible for the syntacto-semantic hallucinations described here as the patient had a pure alexic syndrome, a Received 4 April 2003 In revised form 9 June 2003 known consequence of VWFA lesions.
    [Show full text]
  • Neuropsychiatry Review Series: Disorders of Visual Perception. Dominic Ffytche, Jan Dirk Blom, Marco Catani
    Neuropsychiatry Review series: Disorders of Visual perception. Dominic Ffytche, Jan Dirk Blom, Marco Catani To cite this version: Dominic Ffytche, Jan Dirk Blom, Marco Catani. Neuropsychiatry Review series: Disorders of Visual perception.. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, BMJ Publishing Group, 2010, 81 (11), pp.1280. 10.1136/jnnp.2008.171348. hal-00587980 HAL Id: hal-00587980 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00587980 Submitted on 22 Apr 2011 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Disorders of visual perception Dr Dominic H ffytche1,4* Dr JD Blom2,3 4 Dr M Catani 1 Department of Old Age Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, UK 2 Parnassia Bavo Group, The Hague, the Netherlands 3 Department of Psychiatry, University of Groningen, Groningen, the Netherlands 4 Natbrainlab, Department of Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, UK *Address for Correspondence Dr D H ffytche Department of Old Age Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry PO70, King’s College
    [Show full text]
  • The Primary Visual Cortex, and Feedback to It, Are Not Necessary for Conscious Vision
    doi:10.1093/brain/awq305 Brain 2011: 134; 247–257 | 247 BRAIN A JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGY The primary visual cortex, and feedback to it, are not necessary for conscious vision 1 2 Dominic H. ffytche and Semir Zeki Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/brain/article/134/1/247/292667 by guest on 30 September 2021 1 Department of Old Age Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, UK 2 Wellcome Laboratory of Neurobiology, University College London, UK Correspondence to: Prof. S. Zeki, Wellcome Laboratory of Neurobiology, University College London, London WC1E 6BT, UK E-mail: [email protected] A compelling single case report of visual awareness (visual qualia) without primary visual cortex would be sufficient to refute the hypothesis that the primary visual cortex and the back-projections to it are necessary for conscious visual experience. In a previous study, we emphasized the presence of crude visual awareness in Patient G.Y., with a lesion of the primary visual cortex, who is aware of, and able to discriminate, fast-moving visual stimuli presented to his blind field. The visual nature of Patient G.Y.’s blind field experience has since been questioned and it has been suggested that the special circumstances of repeated testing over decades may have altered Patient G.Y.’s visual pathways. We therefore sought new evidence of visual awareness without primary visual cortex in patients for whom such considerations do not apply. Three patients with hemianopic field defects (Patient G.N. and Patient F.B. with MRI confirmed primary visual cortex lesions, Patient C.G.
    [Show full text]
  • A Dictionary of Neurological Signs.Pdf
    A DICTIONARY OF NEUROLOGICAL SIGNS THIRD EDITION A DICTIONARY OF NEUROLOGICAL SIGNS THIRD EDITION A.J. LARNER MA, MD, MRCP (UK), DHMSA Consultant Neurologist Walton Centre for Neurology and Neurosurgery, Liverpool Honorary Lecturer in Neuroscience, University of Liverpool Society of Apothecaries’ Honorary Lecturer in the History of Medicine, University of Liverpool Liverpool, U.K. 123 Andrew J. Larner MA MD MRCP (UK) DHMSA Walton Centre for Neurology & Neurosurgery Lower Lane L9 7LJ Liverpool, UK ISBN 978-1-4419-7094-7 e-ISBN 978-1-4419-7095-4 DOI 10.1007/978-1-4419-7095-4 Springer New York Dordrecht Heidelberg London Library of Congress Control Number: 2010937226 © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2001, 2006, 2011 All rights reserved. This work may not be translated or copied in whole or in part without the written permission of the publisher (Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, 233 Spring Street, New York, NY 10013, USA), except for brief excerpts in connection with reviews or scholarly analysis. Use in connection with any form of information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed is forbidden. The use in this publication of trade names, trademarks, service marks, and similar terms, even if they are not identified as such, is not to be taken as an expression of opinion as to whether or not they are subject to proprietary rights. While the advice and information in this book are believed to be true and accurate at the date of going to press, neither the authors nor the editors nor the publisher can accept any legal responsibility for any errors or omissions that may be made.
    [Show full text]
  • Subhi 2017.Pdf
    ANGLIA RUSKIN UNIVERSITY FACULTY OF SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY TOWARDS A FUNCTIONAL VISUAL FIELD ASSESSMENT FOR LOW VISION HIKMAT SUBHI A thesis in partial fulfilment of the requirements of Anglia Ruskin University for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy (PhD) Submitted: June 2017 Acknowledgments I would like to express my special appreciation and thanks to my first supervisor Dr Keziah Latham for her unwavering support and guidance during my PhD. Thanks also to my second and third supervisors, Dr Joy Myint and Dr Michael Crossland for their invaluable guidance and advice. Thank you to CamSight, the International Glaucoma Association, Huntingdonshire Society for the Blind, and RP Fighting Blindness for their help in the recruitment of participants. I am grateful also to Anglia Ruskin University Eye Clinic for providing facilities for my data collection. I would like to acknowledge the financial support from the College of Optometrists who funded this research over the last 3 years, and the Visual Function and Physiology Research Group at Anglia Ruskin University who provided funding to attend numerous national and international conferences. Thanks to my family and friends for their patience and encouragement, in particular my parents, Aisha Elmuntasser and Ahmed Subhi, and siblings, Emnani, Kalid, and Mohammed who offered much support and humour. Finally, I would like to express my deep and sincere gratitude to the individuals who gave up their time and travelled from far and wide to participate in this research. The completion of this research and thesis would not have been possible without you. i Abstract Visual field assessment is not only important to monitor disease progression, but also to reflect and predict functional difficulty in the real world.
    [Show full text]
  • Lecture 4: Lesions and Neurological Examination of Visual Function in Extrastriate Visual Cortex
    HMS230: Visual Object Recognition Gabriel Kreiman LECTURE NOTES BEWARE: These are only highly preliminary notes. In the future, they will become part of a textbook on “Visual Object Recognition”. In the meantime, please interpret with caution. Feedback is welcome at [email protected] Lecture 4: Lesions and neurological examination of visual function in extrastriate visual cortex To find out how something works, it is often useful to try to take it apart, examine its function upon removing individual components and then putting it back together. This has been an important approach in Neuroscience as well. Of course, this is easier said than done. First, different components of the system may interact with one another, such that removing one of them could lead to indirect functional consequences. Second, there could be a significant amount of redundancy, such that another component could take over, thereby shadowing the actual use of the removed piece. Third and equally important, it is not always easy to remove parts of the system (the brain) in a clean way, without affecting multiple other parts in the process. In spite of these and other challenges to be presented below, much has been learnt about the function of the visual object recognition circuitry through the study of lesions in animal models as well as in humans. 4.1 Some tools to study the functional role of brain areas in animal models Several tools are in use to examine the effect of removing or silencing a given brain area including lesion studies, cooling experiments, pharmacological intervention, imaging combined with specific cell ablation and molecular tools such as gene knock-outs.
    [Show full text]
  • Residual Perception of Biological Motion In
    Neuropsychologia 93 (2016) 301–311 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Neuropsychologia journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia Residual perception of biological motion in cortical blindness ⁎ crossmark Nicolas Ruffieuxa, ,1, Meike Ramona,1, Junpeng Laoa,1, Françoise Colombob, Lisa Stacchia, ⁎ François-Xavier Borruatc, Ettore Accollab,d, Jean-Marie Annonib,d, Roberto Caldaraa, a Eye and Brain Mapping Laboratory (iBMLab), Department of Psychology, University of Fribourg, Faucigny 2, 1700 Fribourg, Switzerland b Unit of Neuropsychology and Aphasiology, Fribourg Hospital, CP, 1708 Fribourg, Switzerland c Jules-Gonin Eye Hospital, University of Lausanne, Avenue de France 15, 1004 Lausanne, Switzerland d Laboratory for Cognitive and Neurological Sciences, Department of Medicine, University of Fribourg, Ch. du Musée 5, 1700 Fribourg, Switzerland ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Keywords: From birth, the human visual system shows a remarkable sensitivity for perceiving biological motion. This Cortical blindness visual ability relies on a distributed network of brain regions and can be preserved even after damage of high- Biological motion level ventral visual areas. However, it remains unknown whether this critical biological skill can withstand the Residual visual ability loss of vision following bilateral striate damage. To address this question, we tested the categorization of human Striate damage and animal biological motion in BC, a rare case of cortical blindness after anoxia-induced bilateral striate Task dependency damage. The severity of his impairment, encompassing various aspects of vision (i.e., color, shape, face, and Eye tracking object recognition) and causing blind-like behavior, contrasts with a residual ability to process motion. We presented BC with static or dynamic point-light displays (PLDs) of human or animal walkers.
    [Show full text]