Vicarious Shame and Guilt G P

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Vicarious Shame and Guilt G P 04 Lickel (bc/s) 29/3/05 1:09 pm Page 145 Group Processes & G Intergroup Relations P 2005 Vol 8(2) 145–157 I R Vicarious Shame and Guilt Brian Lickel University of Southern California Toni Schmader The University of Arizona Mathew Curtis University of Southern California Marchelle Scarnier The University of Arizona Daniel R. Ames Columbia University Participants recalled instances when they felt vicariously ashamed or guilty for another’s wrongdoing and rated their appraisals of the event and resulting motivations. The study tested aspects of social association that uniquely predict vicarious shame and guilt. Results suggest that the experience of vicarious shame and vicarious guilt are distinguishable. Vicarious guilt was predicted by one’s perceived interdependence with the wrongdoer (e.g. high interpersonal interaction), an appraisal of control over the event, and a motivation to repair the other person’s wrongdoing. Vicarious shame was predicted by the relevance of the event to a shared social identity with the wrongdoer, an appraisal of self-image threat, and a motivation to distance from the event. Implications for intergroup behavior and emotion are discussed. keywords guilt, interdependence, shame, social identity, stereotypes I envied other students back then [in postwar Stillwell, & Heatherton, 1995; Eisenberg, 2000; Germany] who had dissociated themselves from Keltner & Harker, 1998; Tangney & Fischer, their parents and thus from the entire generation 1995). Although we commonly think of shame of perpetrators, voyeurs, and the willfully blind, and guilt as feelings we experience as a result of accommodators and accepters, thereby overcoming perhaps not their shame, but at least their suffering our own actions, the above quotation suggests because of the shame (p. 171). (Bernhard Schlink, The Reader) Author’s note SHAME and guilt are powerful emotions of self- Address correspondence to Brian Lickel, condemnation that are thought to regulate Department of Psychology, University of social interactions in situations where one has Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90089, violated a moral or social code (Baumeister, USA [email: [email protected]] Copyright © 2005 SAGE Publications (London, Thousand Oaks, CA and New Delhi) 8:2; 145–157; DOI: 10.1177/1368430205051064 04 Lickel (bc/s) 29/3/05 1:09 pm Page 146 Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 8(2) that feelings of self-condemnation can some- The nature of shame and guilt times result from acts committed by others. Given that the emotional reactions that we have Traditional emotion research has often assumed to the wrongdoings of other group members that shame and guilt are more similar than are relevant to both intragroup and intergroup different (e.g. C. A. Smith & Ellsworth, 1985). relations, it is surprising that no theoretical However, more contemporary perspectives framework has been developed of the argue that these two emotions differ in terms of antecedents and consequences to distinguish the motivations they evoke and their associated vicarious shame and guilt. The importance of appraisals (e.g. Hong & Chiu, 1992; Niedenthal, drawing such distinctions is critical if one Tangney, & Gavanski, 1994; R. H. Smith, assumes that each emotion will have unique Webster, Parrott, & Eyre, 2002; Tangney, Miller, behavioral consequences: as we will describe, Flicker, & Barlow, 1996; Wicker, Payne, & guilt may elicit more approach related behav- Morgan, 1983). For example, guilt predicts iors designed to repair the situation, whereas approach-related responses meant to repair the shame may elicit more withdrawing behaviors damage that was caused by the guilt-eliciting designed to distance oneself from the situation. event, and in fact several studies indicate that Although the factors that distinguish vicarious guilt is particularly linked to a desire to confess, shame and guilt have not been empirically apologize, or atone for one’s own wrongdoings investigated, some theorists have suggested that (e.g. Tangney et al., 1996; Wicker et al., 1983). these emotions can be experienced for another’s Shame, on the other hand, appears to be more misdeeds (Morris, 1987; Ortony, Clore, & strongly linked to responses aimed at insulating Collins, 1988; Tangney & Fischer, 1995; Weiner, oneself from negative evaluation. Thus, rather 1986). Intergroup researchers, meanwhile, have than facilitating reparative actions, feelings of examined guilt reactions to ingroup bias (e.g. shame provoke a desire to hide, disappear, or Doosje, Branscombe, Spears, & Manstead, 1998; escape (Tangney, 1995; Wicker et al., 1983). Iyer, Leach, & Crosby, 2003; McGarty et al., in In addition to being distinguished by their press). Doosje et al. (1998), for example, showed correspondent motivations, shame and guilt that under certain conditions, Dutch students also seem to differ with respect to how an indi- feel guilty for their country’s past history of vidual interprets a transgression with respect to colonization in Indonesia, and perhaps as a con- the self (Tangney & Fischer, 1995). Feeling sequence, feel motivated to provide reparations guilty for one’s own wrongdoing is often associ- to the people of Indonesia. Furthermore, ated with a focus on the specific controllable research on the black sheep effect (e.g. behaviors that led up to the wrongful event, Eidelman & Biernat, 2003; Marques, Yzerbyt, & whereas feeling ashamed involves a more global Leyens, 1988) suggests that individuals emphasis on what that event seems to imply derogate group members who fail to comply about the dispositional qualities one possesses with the norms of the group. This past research (Tangney & Fischer, 1995; Wicker, et al., 1983). demonstrates that people have emotional and For example, individuals who recall a guilt behavioral reactions to the negative behaviors experience are more likely to have the counter- of ingroup members. However, because none factual thought, ‘If only I had acted differently’, of this research has examined both shame and whereas those who recall a shame experience guilt, the conclusions yielded about the general are more likely to think, ‘If only I were a nature of vicarious emotion are limited. The different type of person’ (Niedenthal et al., goal of the present paper was to test a model 1994). Similarly, individuals perceive that they that distinguishes between vicarious shame and have more control over situations that they guilt felt for another’s wrongdoing. Rather than recall as guilt experiences as compared to focus on one particular type of group in one shame experiences (Wicker et al., 1983). Shame, type of context, our model is intended to however, in comparison to guilt, involves a generalize to a variety of groups and situations. greater feeling of self-consciousness and a fear 146 04 Lickel (bc/s) 29/3/05 1:09 pm Page 147 Lickel et al. vicarious shame and guilt that one will be rejected by others (R. H. Smith presumes that the impact of the group on the et al., 2002; Tangney et al., 1996; Wicker et al., self will be greater to the extent that one’s 1983). association to the group is strong. In fact, there is some evidence that vicarious shame and guilt Vicarious shame and guilt reactions are stronger in China, a collectivist culture, than in the United States, an indi- Given the dominant conceptualization of shame vidualist culture (Stipek, 1998). and guilt as self-conscious emotions, it is not However, a consideration of association- surprising that existing research has focused strength alone does not lead to predictions almost exclusively on people’s experience of about the degree to which shame versus guilt is these emotions after they have committed a likely to occur. Given the appraisals that are blameworthy action themselves. Indeed, proposed to distinguish shame from guilt previous research suggests that shame and guilt experiences, we considered whether there were are unique among negative emotions in that distinct aspects of social association that might both typically involve high appraisals of be linked to appraisals of behavioral control personal responsibility for a negative event over the wrongdoer (thus evoking vicarious (Manstead & Tetlock, 1989; C. A. Smith & guilt) versus appraisals that the event is a threat Ellsworth, 1985). Our work is a departure from to one’s image (thus evoking vicarious shame). this past research in that we suggest that actual Drawing upon group theory and research (e.g. personal causality for a negative event is not Brewer, 2000; Hamilton, Sherman, & Lickel, necessary to experience shame or guilt. We use 1998; Krech & Crutchfield, 1948; Lewin, 1948; the terms vicarious shame and guilt to refer to Prentice, Miller, & Lightdale, 1994; Rabbie & instances in which a person who is not the Horwitz, 1988; Tönnies, 1998; Wilder & Simon, proximal agent of a wrongdoing experiences 1998), we highlight two dimensions of social these emotions. associations that might play a role in activating Our basic premise, that the emotional lives of these appraisals in response to another’s wrong- individuals are shaped by their association with doing. One dimension is the extent to which others, is a premise shared with many prior individuals are associated by a sense of shared researchers (e.g. Mackie, Devos, & Smith, 1999; identity. The second dimension is the extent to E. R. Smith, 1993). For example, social psy- which individuals are associated by being inter- chologists have examined how people ‘pick up’ personally interdependent with one another. Past and experience the same emotion of those with research, such as Prentice et al., 1994, assumed whom they are in close physical proximity (e.g. that these two dimensions might refer to distinct Hatfield, Cacioppo, & Rapson, 1994; Miller, types of groups (which they contrasted as 1987), as well as how people may experience common identity versus common bond groups). positive and negative feelings as a result of their In our analysis, we assume that some degree of social identity or association with successful or interpersonal interdependence and some unsuccessful others (e.g. Cialdini & de Nicholas, degree of shared identity are present in any 1989; Tajfel & Turner, 1986; Tesser, 1988) or social association.
Recommended publications
  • Social Comparison As the Thief of Joy: Emotional Consequences of Viewing Strangers' Instagram Posts De Vries, D.A.; Möller, A.M.; Wieringa, M.S.; Eigenraam, A.W
    UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Social comparison as the thief of joy: Emotional consequences of viewing strangers' Instagram posts de Vries, D.A.; Möller, A.M.; Wieringa, M.S.; Eigenraam, A.W. ; Hamelink, K. DOI 10.1080/15213269.2016.1267647 Publication date 2018 Document Version Final published version Published in Media Psychology License CC BY-NC-ND Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): de Vries, D. A., Möller, A. M., Wieringa, M. S., Eigenraam, A. W., & Hamelink, K. (2018). Social comparison as the thief of joy: Emotional consequences of viewing strangers' Instagram posts. Media Psychology, 21(2), 222-245. https://doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2016.1267647 General rights It is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). Disclaimer/Complaints regulations If you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, stating your reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Ask the Library: https://uba.uva.nl/en/contact, or a letter to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam, The Netherlands. You will be contacted as soon as possible. UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (https://dare.uva.nl) Download date:01 Oct 2021 MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 2018, VOL.
    [Show full text]
  • Making Sense of Emotional Contagion
    Making Sense of Emotional Contagion Carme Isern-Mas † [email protected] Antoni Gomila † [email protected] ABSTRACT Emotional contagion is a phenomenon that has attracted much interest in recent times. However, the main theory, mimicry theory, fails to properly address its many facets. In particular, we will focus on two shortcomings: the elicitation of emotional contagion is not context-independent, and there can be cases of emotional contagion without motor mimicry. We contend that a general theory of emotion elicitation (such as Scherer’s Component Process Model of Emotion) is better suited to account for these features, because of its multi-level appraisal component. From this standpoint, emotional contagion is viewed as a particular kind of emotional response that involves the same components and processes of emotional responses in general. 1. Introduction Emotions appear to be contagious (Dezecache, Eskenazi, & Grèzes, 2016; Doherty, 1997). Examples abound: there is the phenomenon of laugher contagion (Provine, 2012); mothers share the distress of their children (Manini et al., 2013); friends tend to converge on their feelings (McIntosh, 2006); 2- or 4-day-old newborns cry when they hear the cry of another newborn, but not when they hear another noise (Simner, 1971). Clinical psychologists are likely to catch their patients’ feelings, especially in cases of depression where expressions of sadness might be especially salient (Hatfield, Cacioppo, & Rapson, 1993). Different terms have been used to describe this phenomenon in which one person catches up the emotion of another, such as “emotional propagation”, “emotional replication” (Dezecache, Jacob, & Grèzes, 2015), or † Human Evolution and Cognition Group (EvoCog), UIB, IFISC, Associated Unit to CSIC, 07122 Palma, Spain.
    [Show full text]
  • Browsing Facebook: Envy, Happiness, and Tie Strength?
    Browsing Facebook: Envy, Happiness, and Tie Strength? Ruoyun Lin Sonja Utz Leibniz Institut für Wissensmedien Twitters: @ReDefTie @RuoyunLin Email: [email protected] PSYCHOLOGICAL EFFECTS OF SOCIAL MEDIA USAGE Active Passive usage usage ? Feeling of Joyful connectedness Envy, depression EMOTIONAL RESPONSES: ON A MESSAGE LEVEL? Happiness vs. Envy? Tie strength? (Granovetter, 1973) - Strong tie (best friend) - Weak tie (acquaintance) RESEARCH QUESTIONS RQ1: What are the most prevalent momentary emotional outcomes after reading a post on Facebook? Emotional outcomes Content of post (Happiness & Envy) RESEARCH QUESTIONS RQ1: What are the most prevalent momentary emotional outcomes after reading a post on Facebook? RQ2: Does tie strength between the poster and reader moderate the emotions (happiness and envy)? Tie strength Emotional outcomes Content of post (Happiness & Envy) Individual-related factors THEORETICAL BACKGROUND RQ2: Does tie strength between the poster and reader moderate the emotions (happiness and envy)? Happiness is contagious! Emotional Contagion (Hatfield et al. 1993; Kramer, 2012) Empathy is more pronounced when relationship is closer Perception-Action Model (Norscia & Palagi, 2011) Stronger tie intensified emotions THEORETICAL BACKGROUND RQ2: Does tie strength between the poster and reader moderate the emotions (happiness and envy)? • Upward Social comparison (Festinger, 1954) • Preconditions (Smith,2004; Tesser, 1984) – Self-relevance – Perceived similarity • Benign envy: moving up motivation • Malicious envy: pulling
    [Show full text]
  • Digital Emotion Contagion
    Digital Emotion Contagion Amit Goldenberg James J. Gross Authors Affiliation: Amit Goldenberg* – Harvard University, Harvard Business School James J. Gross – Stanford University, Department of Psychology *Correspondence: [email protected] Key Words: emotion, emotion contagion, digital media, social media 1 People spend considerable time on digital media, and during this time they are often exposed to others’ emotion expressions. This exposure can lead their own emotion expressions to become more like others’ emotion expressions, a process we refer to as digital emotion contagion. This paper reviews the growing literature on digital emotion contagion. After defining emotion contagion, we suggest that one unique feature of digital emotion contagion is that it is mediated by digital media platforms that are motivated to upregulate users’ emotions. We then turn to measurement, and consider the challenges of demonstrating that digital emotion contagion has occurred, and how these challenges have been addressed. Finally, we call for a greater focus on understanding when emotion contagion effects will be strong versus weak or non-existent. 2 The Ubiquity of Digital Emotion Contagion In 2014, PNAS published a study that sought to demonstrate emotion contagion on social media using an experimental design [1]. In this study, the content that Facebook users saw was manipulated without their knowledge to be less negative or less positive. Users’ emotions were evaluated with a dictionary-based program that counts the number of positive and negative words in each text [2]. Results indicated that those who were exposed to less negative or less positive emotions produced less of these emotions themselves. This is the only published study that has manipulated users’ emotions without their knowledge on a digital media platform.
    [Show full text]
  • The Impact of Emotional Contagion and Its Relationship to Mood
    The Impact of Emotional Contagion and its Relationship to Mood Lauren Englert Abstract Emotional contagion or the tendency to mimic and feel emotional displays and experiences of others in social interactions is more than likely to be influenced by affect or the existing mood of individuals engaging in those interactions (Hatfield, Cacioppo, & Rapson, 1992). This current study analyzed the relationship between susceptibility to emotional contagion and existing mood using two well-established scales: the PANAS (Positive and Negative Affect Schedule) scale and the EC (Emotional Contagion) scale (Watson, Clark, & Tellegan, 1988; Doherty, 1997). Two hypotheses were assessed with the first one being that individuals with a high self-report of mood (positive or negative) will be more susceptible to emotional contagion and the second one stating that women will have higher self-reports of mood and will be more susceptible to emotional contagion than men. Results, limitations, and implications are discussed further throughout the study. Keywords: emotional contagion, affect In 1992, Hatfield, Cacioppo, and Rapson proposed that by observing another’s face, voice, posture, and instrumental behavior during conversation and social interaction, people unconsciously and automatically begin mimicking the emotions expressed by the person they are conversing with, and ultimately, may come to feel as the other person feels. They termed this phenomenon as emotional contagion that can occur in various ways through multiple situations. More specifically, Hatfield et al. (1992) discussed primitive emotional contagion that is relatively automatic, unintentional, uncontrollable, and largely unconscious and also the mechanisms that might account for it. There are a variety of differences that are assumed in an individual’s ability to mirror others’ emotions, and these include genetic heritage, gender, personality, and early experiences (Hatfield et al., 1992).
    [Show full text]
  • Emotional Contagion Within Social Media
    College of Saint Benedict and Saint John's University DigitalCommons@CSB/SJU Celebrating Scholarship and Creativity Day Undergraduate Research 4-25-2019 Emotional Contagion Within Social Media Anna Dols College of Saint Benedict/Saint John's University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.csbsju.edu/ur_cscday Recommended Citation Dols, Anna, "Emotional Contagion Within Social Media" (2019). Celebrating Scholarship and Creativity Day. 62. https://digitalcommons.csbsju.edu/ur_cscday/62 This Paper is brought to you for free and open access by DigitalCommons@CSB/SJU. It has been accepted for inclusion in Celebrating Scholarship and Creativity Day by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@CSB/SJU. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Running Head: EMOTIONAL CONTAGION WITHIN SOCIAL MEDIA 1 Emotional Contagion Within Social Media Anna M. Dols College of Saint Benedict/Saint John’s University Author Note Anna M. Dols, Psychology Department, College of Saint Benedict/Saint John’s University. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to: 37 South College Avenue, Box #0155, St. Joseph, MN, 56374; E-mail: [email protected]. EMOTIONAL CONTAGION WITHIN SOCIAL MEDIA 2 Abstract Social networking sites have been growing in popularity over the last decade or so, and there have been many previous studies that have analyzed the possible consequences of these communication and interaction sites. Emotional contagion is the phenomenon of having one person’s emotions and behaviors directly trigger similar emotions or behaviors in other people. On social media, emotional contagion would mean that the emotions in the posts and content the viewer is exposed to are transferred to themselves without their knowledge.
    [Show full text]
  • The Emotional Responses of Browsing Facebook: Happiness, Envy, and the Role of Tie Strength ⇑ Ruoyun Lin , Sonja Utz
    Computers in Human Behavior 52 (2015) 29–38 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Computers in Human Behavior journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh The emotional responses of browsing Facebook: Happiness, envy, and the role of tie strength ⇑ Ruoyun Lin , Sonja Utz Leibniz-Institut für Wissensmedien (Knowledge Media Research Center), Tübingen, Germany article info abstract Article history: On Facebook, users are exposed to posts from both strong and weak ties. Even though several studies Available online 10 June 2015 have examined the emotional consequences of using Facebook, less attention has been paid to the role of tie strength. This paper aims to explore the emotional outcomes of reading a post on Facebook and Keywords: examine the role of tie strength in predicting happiness and envy. Two studies – one correlational, based Facebook on a sample of 207 American participants and the other experimental, based on a sample of 194 German Emotional contagion participants – were conducted in 2014. In Study 2, envy was further distinguished into benign and Happiness malicious envy. Based on a multi-method approach, the results showed that positive emotions are more Social comparison prevalent than negative emotions while browsing Facebook. Moreover, tie strength is positively Envy Tie strength associated with the feeling of happiness and benign envy, whereas malicious envy is independent of tie strength after reading a (positive) post on Facebook. Ó 2015 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/). 1. Introduction and theoretical background 1.1.
    [Show full text]
  • Effects of Social Anxiety on Emotional Mimicry and Contagion: Feeling Negative, but Smiling Politely
    UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Effects of Social Anxiety on Emotional Mimicry and Contagion: Feeling Negative, but Smiling Politely Dijk, C.; Fischer, A.H.; Morina, N.; Van Eeuwijk, C.; van Kleef, G.A. DOI 10.1007/s10919-017-0266-z Publication date 2018 Document Version Final published version Published in Journal of Nonverbal Behavior License CC BY Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Dijk, C., Fischer, A. H., Morina, N., Van Eeuwijk, C., & van Kleef, G. A. (2018). Effects of Social Anxiety on Emotional Mimicry and Contagion: Feeling Negative, but Smiling Politely. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 42(1), 81-99. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-017-0266-z General rights It is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). Disclaimer/Complaints regulations If you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, stating your reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Ask the Library: https://uba.uva.nl/en/contact, or a letter to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam, The Netherlands. You will be contacted as soon as possible. UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (https://dare.uva.nl) Download date:27 Sep 2021 J Nonverbal Behav (2018) 42:81–99 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-017-0266-z ORIGINAL PAPER Effects of Social Anxiety on Emotional Mimicry and Contagion: Feeling Negative, but Smiling Politely 1 2 3 Corine Dijk • Agneta H.
    [Show full text]
  • Emotional Contagion and Its Influence on Group Behavior Author(S): Sigal G
    The Ripple Effect: Emotional Contagion and Its Influence on Group Behavior Author(s): Sigal G. Barsade Source: Administrative Science Quarterly, Vol. 47, No. 4 (Dec., 2002), pp. 644-675 Published by: Sage Publications, Inc. on behalf of the Johnson Graduate School of Management, Cornell University Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/3094912 . Accessed: 12/02/2015 02:13 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. Sage Publications, Inc. and Johnson Graduate School of Management, Cornell University are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Administrative Science Quarterly. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded from 128.103.149.52 on Thu, 12 Feb 2015 02:13:10 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions The Ripple Effect: Group emotional contagion, the transfer of moods Emotional among people in a group, and its influence on work Contagion was examined and Its Influence on group dynamics in a laboratory study of managerial decision making using multiple, convergent Group Behavior measures of mood, individual attitudes, behavior, and group-level dynamics. Using a 2 x 2 experimental design, Sigal G. Barsade with a trained confederate enacting mood conditions, the effect of emotional was found YaleUniversity predicted contagion among group members, using both outside coders' rat- ings of participants' mood and participants' self-reported mood.
    [Show full text]
  • Understanding Emotional Transitions: the Interpersonal Consequences of Changing Emotions in Negotiations
    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology © 2011 American Psychological Association 2011, Vol. 101, No. 3, 541–556 0022-3514/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0023545 Understanding Emotional Transitions: The Interpersonal Consequences of Changing Emotions in Negotiations Allan Filipowicz Sigal Barsade and Shimul Melwani INSEAD, Singapore The Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania Research on the interpersonal functions of emotions has focused primarily on steady-state emotion rather than on emotional transitions, the movement between emotion states. The authors examined the influence of emotional transitions on social interactions and found that emotional transitions led to consistently different outcomes than their corresponding steady-state emotions. Across 2 computer-mediated nego- tiations and a face-to-face negotiation, participants negotiating with partners who displayed a “becoming angry” (happy to angry) emotional transition accepted worse negotiation outcomes yet formed better relational impressions of their partners than participants negotiating with partners who displayed steady-state anger. This relationship was mediated through 2 mechanisms: attributional and emotional contagion processes. The “becoming happy” (angry to happy) emotional transition as compared with steady-state happiness was not significantly related to differences in negotiation outcomes but was significantly related to differences in relational impressions, where perceivers of the “becoming happy” emotional transition gave their partners lower relational impression ratings than perceivers of steady-state happiness. Keywords: emotional transitions, emotions, interpersonal functions of emotions, negotiations, social interactions A rich and growing body of research shows that emotions have here on the largely unexplored phenomenon of emotional transi- interpersonal functions; that is, our own emotions not only influ- tions and how they influence social interactions.
    [Show full text]
  • Facial Mimicry and Emotional Contagion to Dynamic Emotional Facial Expressions and Their Inffuence on Decoding Accuracy
    International Journal of Psychophysiology 40Ž. 2001 129᎐141 Facial mimicry and emotional contagion to dynamic emotional facial expressions and their in¯uence on decoding accuracy Ursula Hessa,U, Sylvie Blairy b aDepartment of Psychology, Uni¨ersity of Quebec at Montreal, P.O. Box 8888, Station Centre-Ville, Montreal, QC, Canada H3C 3P8 bFree Uni¨ersity of Brussels, ERASME Hospital, Brussels, Belgium Received 27 June 2000; received in revised form 26 July 2000; accepted 1 August 2000 Abstract The present study had the goal to assess whether individuals mimic and show emotional contagion in response to relatively weak and idiosyncratic dynamic facial expressions of emotions similar to those encountered in everyday life. Furthermore, the question of whether mimicry leads to emotional contagion and in turn facilitates emotion recognition was addressed. Forty-one female participants rated a series of short video clips of stimulus persons expressing anger, sadness, disgust, and happiness regarding the emotions expressed. An unobtrusive measure of emotional contagion was taken. Evidence for mimicry was found for all types of expressions. Furthermore, evidence for emotional contagion of happiness and sadness was found. Mediational analyses could not con®rm any relation between mimicry and emotional contagion nor between mimicry and emotion recognition. ᮊ 2001 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Facial mimicry; Emotional contagion; Emotion recognition 1. Introduction munication of affective statesw e.g. Freud, 1921, based on a theory by Lipps, 1907; Bavelas et al., It has been suggested that mimicry ᎏ the 1986x . For example, RogersŽ. 1957 saw the imita- imitation of others' non-verbal displays by an tion of a client's non-verbal behavior as a means observer ᎏ plays an important role in the com- to communicate empathy and some schools of therapyŽ.
    [Show full text]
  • Emotional Contagion, Viral Performativity, and the Death of David Bowie
    Selected Papers of AoIR 2016: The 17th Annual Conference of the Association of Internet Researchers Berlin, Germany / 5-8 October 2016 THE (NET)WORK OF MOURNING: EMOTIONAL CONTAGION, VIRAL PERFORMATIVITY, AND THE DEATH OF DAVID BOWIE Peta Mitchell Queensland University of Technology Axel Bruns Queensland University of Technology Felix Victor Münch Queensland University of Technology David BoWie’s death in January 2016 led to a vast outpouring on social media of grief and public mourning. According to TWitter UK (2016), over 4 million BoWie-related tWeets Were sent in the 24-hour period folloWing neWs of his death becoming public, with the stream of tributes peaking at 20 thousand tWeets per minute just after 7 am GMT (TWitter UK, 2016). BoWie’s death takes its place among a number of other high- profile ‘celebrity’ deaths in the social media era, including those of Michael Jackson in 2009, Which famously tested TWitter’s capacity, and, more recently, Robin Williams in 2014 and Leonard Nimoy in 2015. What makes BoWie’s death particularly notable in regard to social media is the Way in Which it has opened up a reneWed dialogue about public responses to celebrity deaths, and Whether this marks a neW moment in the sociology of mourning. Within days of BoWie’s death, journalists such as Caroline Framke (2016) Were attempting to explain “why We grieve artists We’ve never met, in one tWeet.” While Framke argued that these public expressions of grief Were individual and authentic, other commentators expressed skepticism. Spiked editor Brendan O’Neill (2016), for instance, contrasted the “shalloW” sadness expressed by “hacks” and “fans” on social media With the “deep and raW and real” grief of BoWie’s family, Which in his vieW Was signalled by their relative distance from social media, their need to grieve in private: “BoWie’s son, Duncan Jones, left TWitter immediately after announcing his father’s death.
    [Show full text]