Suspicious Receivers' Interaction Goals and Strategic Behaviors Within Dating Relationships

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Suspicious Receivers' Interaction Goals and Strategic Behaviors Within Dating Relationships SUSPICIOUS RECEIVERS' INTERACTION GOALS AND STRATEGIC BEHAVIORS WITHIN DATING RELATIONSHIPS A THESIS SUBMITTED TO THE GRADUATE DIVISION OF THE UNIVERSITY OF HAWAI'I IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIRMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF ARTS IN SPEECH AUGUST 2004 By In DukKim Thesis Committee: Min-Sun Kim, Chairperson Krystyna S. Aune William F. Sharkey iii © Copyright 2004 by In DukKim IV ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to thank my advisor, Dr. Min-Sun Kim for her mentorship and guidance. Throughout the program, she has been both a great advisor and a wonderful mentor who always understood me and encouraged me. Without her mother-like support and beliefin me, I would not be where I am today. I would also like to thank Dr. Bill Sharkey from whose class I gained the idea ofthis thesis. I very much appreciate his patient guidance throughout the process. His expertise in content analysis salvaged me from the big chaos ofdata analysis. Dr. Krystyna Aune is also someone who I deeply appreciate. Her constructive comments about my thesis were truly invaluable. I would also like to acknowledge my fellow graduate students, Kathleen Doss and Michelle Jemey-Davis, for their continuous help and support. This thesis project wouldn't have been completed without their help in coding. Lastly, I would like to thank my parents and friends for their endless support and love. v ABSTRACT The study ofsuspicion in previous research has primarily focused on the impact of suspicion on deception detection accuracy or the pragmatics ofsuspicious receivers. The premise ofthe present study is that cognitive aspects ofsuspicious receivers must be accounted for to further understand their active participation in deceptive communication. The present study attempts to identify suspicious receivers' interaction goals and strategic behaviors and to find links between the goals and behaviors. The influence ofsituational and affective factors on suspicious receivers' goals and behaviors are also investigated. Data were collected from one hundred and eighty two undergraduate students within dating relationships. The results revealed four interaction goals and nine behavior categories ofsuspicious receivers in dating relationship. Some qualitative differences between the goals were found in terms oftheir association with behavioral strategies. Also, outcome involvement, relational commitment, and emotional intensity were found to have important influences on suspicious receivers' behaviors. Implications for future suspicion research are discussed as well as the study's limitations. VI TABLE OF CONTENTS Acknowledgments iv Abstract v List ofTables vii Chapter I: Introduction 1 Suspicion in Deceptive Communication 3 Suspicious Receivers' Interaction Goals 8 Suspicious Receivers' Strategic Behaviors 16 Goal - Behavior Relationships 22 Factors Influencing the Dynamics ofSuspicion Display 24 Chapter II: Methods 30 Participants 30 Instrumentation 31 Coding 34 Chapter III. Results 37 Preliminary Analysis 37 Suspicious Receivers' Interaction Goals 40 Suspicious Receivers' Strategic Behaviors 42 Goal - Behavior Relationships 43 Influences ofPsychological and Affective Factors 44 Chapter IV. Discussions 48 Suspicious Receivers' Interaction Goals 48 Suspicious Receivers' Strategic Behaviors 50 Goal- Behavior Relationships 52 Influences ofPsychological and Affective Factors 53 Conclusion 55 Footnotes 58 References 59 Appendix A 73 Appendix B 74 Appendix C 75 vii LIST OF TABLES Table 1. Types ofSuspicion-Arousing Events 66 Table 2. Types ofSuspicion-Arousing Events and the Experience ofUncertainty 68 Table 3. Means and Standard Deviations ofthe Variables 69 Table 4. Correlation Coefficients ofthe Variables 70 Table 5. Conditional Proportions ofBehaviors in Relation to Goals 71 Table 6. ANOVA Results regarding the Associations between Psychological and Affective Factors and Suspicious Receivers' Strategic Behaviors 72 1 CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION Many deception researchers acknowledge that deception is one ofthe everyday communication strategies commonly adopted for various purposes (e.g., impression management and relational maintenance) (Buller & Burgoon, 1994; DePaulo, Kashy, Kirkendo1, Wyer, & Epstein, 1996; McComack, 1992). In DePaulo et aI.'s (1996) diary studies, participants reported telling a lie in every three to five social interactions, meaning that communicators engage in deceptive behavior in at least 20 percent ofthe daily interaction. Despite the ubiquity ofdeception, it is repeatedly reported that receivers ofthe deceptive message often fail to detect the deception (Levine, Park, & McComack, 1999; McComack & Parks, 1986; Stiff, Kim, & Ramesh, 1992). Between strangers, the lack of knowledge about the sender's idiosyncratic behavioral features is deemed to hamper the accurate detection ofthe sender's deceptive demeanor (Miller, Mongeau, & Sleight, 1986). On the other hand, it was found that interactants in a developed relationship (i.e., friends and intimates) are not significantly better than strangers in assessing the dishonesty oftheir partner, although they have baseline knowledge oftheir partners' idiosyncratic behaviors. It was argued that people hold a judgmental heuristic (i.e., truth­ bias) that predisposes them to believe others' truthfulness rather than to evaluate or to cognitively scrutinize the complex stimuli (McComack & Parks, 1986; Stiffet aI., 1992). This generalized beliefofthe partner's honesty was demonstrated to have substantial power over people's deception detection accuracy, which lowered the accuracy rate significantly below chance levels (Levine et aI., 1999). However, not always does the truth-bias interfere with an individual's deception detection. There are instances in which receivers' predisposition toward truth-bias is 2 offset and the finer grained cognitive processing ofthe senders' behavior is initiated. A growing number ofdeception studies have pointed out that suspicion reduces the receivers' reliance on the truth-bias and motivates the receivers to assess the veracity of the messages from the senders (Buller, Strzyzewski, & Comstock, 1991; Burgoon, Buller, Dillman, & Walther, 1995; Burgoon, Buller, Ebesu, White, & Rockwell, 1996; McCornack & Levine, 1990b; Stiff et aI., 1992). The previous studies in suspicion literature are oriented in two main directions: the impact ofsuspicion on (a) the receiver's deception detection accuracy and (b) the interactional patterns ofdeceptive communication. The studies on a suspicious receiver's deception detection accuracy examined how one or the combination ofthe following factors affect the receiver's deception detection accuracy: the presence ofsuspicion (Tons & DePaulo, 1985; Stiffet aI., 1992), the level ofsuspicion (McCornack & Levine, 1990b), the receiver's probing (Buller et aI., 1991), the relationship between interactants (McCornack & Parks, 1986). Overall, suspicion has been found to be a significant factor to decrease the receiver's truth-bias and, when in its moderate level, increase the deception detection accuracy. In terms ofpragmatics, it was found that a suspicious receiver strategically controls the expression ofhis or her suspicion, while nonstrategically leaking the uneasiness ofsuspicion (Buller & Burgoon, 1996; Buller et aI., 1991; Burgoon et aI., 1995, 1996). Although the previous suspicion studies enhanced our knowledge ofthe impact ofsuspicion on a receiver's deception detection accuracy and shed better insights into the interplay between the receiver's suspicion and the pragmatics ofdeceptive communication, our understanding ofsuspicion is still incomplete. One ofthe missing pieces in the puzzle ofsuspicion is the suspicious receiver's cognitions. Little is known 3 about the cognitive mechanisms underlying the receiver's strategic suspicion displays. Also, previous suspicion studies failed to fully explain what it takes for the receivers to be better able to detect deception. It is a receiver's cognition that links his or her aroused suspicion to the behavior. Thus, there is a significant need for communication researchers to examine the role ofa suspicious receiver's cognition in deceptive communication. The present study aims to explore the suspicious receiver's cognition and its relation to his or her behavior within dating relationships. The principal question is what the receiver's interaction goals and behaviors are and how the goals are associated with strategic behaviors. In addition, the impact ofthree antecedents (i.e., outcome involvement, relationship commitment, and emotional intensity) on a suspicious receiver's goals and behaviors is also examined. In the following sections, the previous suspicion literature is reviewed and the findings from relevant research areas are applied to the context ofsuspicion. Through this exploratory analysis, suspicious receivers' primary interaction goals and the various types ofstrategic behaviors enacted to achieve the goals are identified, and the relationship between the goals and the behaviors are examined. Finally, how outcome involvement, relational commitment, and emotional intensity relate to the goals and the behaviors are investigated. Suspicion in Deceptive Communication Individuals hold strong expectations about communication with others (Burgoon & Walther, 1990; Grice, 1989). The expectations function as mental guidelines, which direct people to make decisions with less cognitive effort (Hubbell, Mitchell, & Gee, 2001; Stiff et aI., 1992). One such expectation is the beliefthat persons will be truthful in
Recommended publications
  • Inter-American Court of Human Rights
    INTER-AMERICAN COURT OF HUMAN RIGHTS CASE OF FERNÁNDEZ PRIETO AND TUMBEIRO V. ARGENTINA JUDGMENT OF SEPTEMBER 1, 2020 (Merits and reparations) In the case of Fernández Prieto and Tumbeiro v. Argentina, the Inter-American Court of Human Rights (hereinafter “the Inter-American Court” or “the Court”), composed of the following judges:* Elizabeth Odio Benito, President L. Patricio Pazmiño Freire, Vice President Eduardo Vio Grossi, Judge Humberto Antonio Sierra Porto, Judge Eduardo Ferrer Mac-Gregor Poisot, Judge, and Ricardo Pérez Manrique, Judge, also present, Pablo Saavedra Alessandri, Secretary,** pursuant to Articles 62(3) and 63(1) of the American Convention on Human Rights (hereinafter “the American Convention” or “the Convention”) and Articles 31, 32, 42, 65 and 67 of the Rules of Procedure of the Court (hereinafter “the Rules of Procedure” or “ the Court’s Rules of Procedure”), delivers this judgment which is structured as follows: * Judge Eugenio Raúl Zaffaroni, an Argentine national, did not take part in the deliberation or signature of this judgment, in accordance with the provisions of Article 19(1) and (2) of the Court’s Rules of Procedure. ** The Deputy Secretary, Romina I. Sijniensky, did not participate in the processing of this case, or in the deliberation and signature of this judgment. TABLE OF CONTENTS I INTRODUCTION OF THE CASE AND PURPOSE OF THE DISPUTE 3 II PROCEEDINGS BEFORE THE COURT 4 III JURISDICTION 6 IV ACKNOWLEDGEMENT OF RESPONSIBILITY 6 A. Observations of the parties and of the Commission 6 B. Considerations of the Court 7 V EVIDENCE 9 A. Admissibility of the documentary evidence 9 B.
    [Show full text]
  • The Role of Exporters' Emotional Intelligence in Building Foreign
    This is a repository copy of The Role of Exporters’ Emotional Intelligence in Building Foreign Customer Relationships. White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: http://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/150142/ Version: Accepted Version Article: Leonidou, LC, Aykol, B, Fotiadis, TA et al. (2 more authors) (2019) The Role of Exporters’ Emotional Intelligence in Building Foreign Customer Relationships. Journal of International Marketing, 27 (4). pp. 58-80. ISSN 1069-031X https://doi.org/10.1177/1069031X19876642 © American Marketing Association, 2019. This is an author produced version of an article published in the Journal of International Marketing. Uploaded in accordance with the publisher's self-archiving policy. Reuse Items deposited in White Rose Research Online are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved unless indicated otherwise. They may be downloaded and/or printed for private study, or other acts as permitted by national copyright laws. The publisher or other rights holders may allow further reproduction and re-use of the full text version. This is indicated by the licence information on the White Rose Research Online record for the item. Takedown If you consider content in White Rose Research Online to be in breach of UK law, please notify us by emailing [email protected] including the URL of the record and the reason for the withdrawal request. [email protected] https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/ The Role of Exporters’ Emotional Intelligence in Building Foreign Customer Relationships Abstract Despite the critical importance of emotional intelligence in effectively interacting with other people, its role has been overlooked in scholarly research on cross-border interorganizational relationships.
    [Show full text]
  • Bereavement Resource Manual 2018 Purpose
    Richmond’s Bereavement Resource Manual 2018 Purpose This manual is designed to serve as an educational resource guide to grieving families and bereavement professionals in the Central Virginia area and to provide a practical list of available national and local support services. It is meant to be a useful reference and is not intended as an exhaustive listing. Grief is not neat and tidy. At Full Circle Grief Center, we realize that each person’s grief journey is unique and personal, based on many factors. Keep in mind that there is no “right” or “wrong” way to cope with grief. After losing a loved one, family members have varying ways of coping and may require different levels of support over time. We hope that some aspect of this manual will be helpful to those grieving in our community and the professionals, friends, and family who support them. Manual created by: Graphic Design by: Copyright © 2010 Allyson England Drake, M.Ed., CT Kali Newlen-Burden Full Circle Grief Center. Founder and Executive Director www.kalinewlen.com Revised January 2018. Full Circle Grief Center All rights reserved. Cover Art Design by: Logan H. Macklin, aged 13 2 Table of Contents Purpose Page 2 Full Circle Grief Center Page 4 Grief and Loss Pages 5 - 9 Children, Teens and Grief Pages 10 - 20 Perinatal Loss and Death of an Infant Pages 21 - 23 Suicide Loss Pages 24 -26 When Additional Support is Needed Pages 27-31 Self-Care Page 32 Rituals and Remembrance Page 33 How to Help and Support Grieving Families Page 34 Community Bereavement Support Services Pages 35-47 Online Grief and Bereavement Services Pages 48-49 Book List for Grief and Loss Pages 50-61 Thoughts from a Grieving Mother Pages 61-63 Affirmations and Aspirations Pages 64-65 3 Full Circle’s mission is to provide comprehensive, professional grief support to children, adults, families, and communities.
    [Show full text]
  • Reasonable Suspicion and Mere Hunches
    Vanderbilt Law Review Volume 59 Issue 2 Article 3 3-2006 Reasonable Suspicion and Mere Hunches Craig S. Lerner Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarship.law.vanderbilt.edu/vlr Part of the Criminal Law Commons Recommended Citation Craig S. Lerner, Reasonable Suspicion and Mere Hunches, 59 Vanderbilt Law Review 407 (2019) Available at: https://scholarship.law.vanderbilt.edu/vlr/vol59/iss2/3 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by Scholarship@Vanderbilt Law. It has been accepted for inclusion in Vanderbilt Law Review by an authorized editor of Scholarship@Vanderbilt Law. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Reasonable Suspicion and Mere Hunches Craig S. Lerner 59 Vand. L. Rev. 407 (2006) In Terry v. Ohio, Earl Warren held that police officers could temporarily detain a suspect, provided that they relied upon "specific, reasonable inferences," and not simply upon an "inchoate and unparticularized suspicion or 'hunch."' Since Terry, courts have strained to distinguish "reasonablesuspicion," which is said to arise from the cool analysis of objective and particularized facts, from "mere hunches," which are said to be subjective, generalized, unreasoned and therefore unreliable. Yet this dichotomy between facts and intuitions is built on sand. Emotions and intuitions are not obstacles to reason, but indispensable heuristic devices that allow people to process diffuse, complex information about their environment and make sense of the world. The legal rules governing police conduct are thus premised on a mistaken assumption about human cognition. This Article argues that the legal system can defer, to some extent, to police officers' intuitions without undermining meaningful protections against law enforcement overreaching.
    [Show full text]
  • Suspicion of Motives Predicts Minorities' Responses to Positive Feedback in Interracial Interactions
    UCSF UC San Francisco Previously Published Works Title Suspicion of Motives Predicts Minorities' Responses to Positive Feedback in Interracial Interactions. Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/2j8345b6 Journal Journal of experimental social psychology, 62 ISSN 0022-1031 Authors Major, Brenda Kunstman, Jonathan W Malta, Brenna D et al. Publication Date 2016 DOI 10.1016/j.jesp.2015.10.007 Peer reviewed eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 62 (2016) 75–88 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Experimental Social Psychology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jesp Suspicion of motives predicts minorities' responses to positive feedback in interracial interactions☆,☆☆,☆☆☆ Brenda Major a,⁎, Jonathan W. Kunstman b,BrennaD.Maltac,PamelaJ.Sawyera, Sarah S.M. Townsend d, Wendy Berry Mendes e a University of California, Santa Barbara, United States b Miami University, United States c New York University, United States d University of Southern California, United States e University of California, San Francisco, United States HIGHLIGHTS • Anti-bias norms increase attributional ambiguity of feedback to minorities. • Some minorities suspect Whites’ positivity toward them is insincere. • Suspicion of motives predicts uncertainty, threat and decreased self-esteem. • Attributionally ambiguous positive feedback is threatening for minorities. • Suspicion that positive evaluations are insincere can have negative consequences. article info abstract
    [Show full text]
  • “Spidey Sense”: Emotional Intelligence and Police Decision-Making at Domestic Incidents
    Applying “spidey sense”: Emotional Intelligence and Police Decision-Making at Domestic Incidents Anne Eason This thesis is submitted in partial fulfilment of the Requirement’s for the award of the degree of Professional Doctorate of Criminal Justice Studies of the University of Portsmouth. September 2019 Declaration Whilst registered as a candidate for the above degree, I have not been registered for any other research award. The results and conclusions embodied in this thesis are the work of the named candidate and have not been submitted for any other academic award. Word count: 49,726 2 Abstract Chaos theory suggests a small act can have a powerful impact on future events. This research takes this suggestion and explores the decision-making of police officers at domestic violence incidents as just such an act that can have a long-term impact on victim(s) lives. Applying the concept of emotional intelligence as an assistive means for problem-solving and decision-making, this research aimed to find out how, or if, police officers draw upon emotional intelligence in their decision- making at domestic incidents and what value they place on its development within the force. Taking a qualitative approach, 27 police officers across four police areas in England were interviewed and the data, coded and categorised, in a thematic analysis that revealed four central themes forming a new ‘emotional intelligence competence’. Environmental competence is symbolic of policing practice that is adaptive and autonomous and as recommended, supporting officers develop this will frustrate the diminishment of emotional intelligence that reportedly occurs through longer-term policing experience.
    [Show full text]
  • The Distancing-Embracing Model of the Enjoyment of Negative Emotions in Art Reception
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2017), Page 1 of 63 doi:10.1017/S0140525X17000309, e347 The Distancing-Embracing model of the enjoyment of negative emotions in art reception Winfried Menninghaus1 Department of Language and Literature, Max Planck Institute for Empirical Aesthetics, 60322 Frankfurt am Main, Germany [email protected] Valentin Wagner Department of Language and Literature, Max Planck Institute for Empirical Aesthetics, 60322 Frankfurt am Main, Germany [email protected] Julian Hanich Department of Arts, Culture and Media, University of Groningen, 9700 AB Groningen, The Netherlands [email protected] Eugen Wassiliwizky Department of Language and Literature, Max Planck Institute for Empirical Aesthetics, 60322 Frankfurt am Main, Germany [email protected] Thomas Jacobsen Experimental Psychology Unit, Helmut Schmidt University/University of the Federal Armed Forces Hamburg, 22043 Hamburg, Germany [email protected] Stefan Koelsch University of Bergen, 5020 Bergen, Norway [email protected] Abstract: Why are negative emotions so central in art reception far beyond tragedy? Revisiting classical aesthetics in the light of recent psychological research, we present a novel model to explain this much discussed (apparent) paradox. We argue that negative emotions are an important resource for the arts in general, rather than a special license for exceptional art forms only. The underlying rationale is that negative emotions have been shown to be particularly powerful in securing attention, intense emotional involvement, and high memorability, and hence is precisely what artworks strive for. Two groups of processing mechanisms are identified that conjointly adopt the particular powers of negative emotions for art’s purposes.
    [Show full text]
  • Kindred Ambivalence
    KINDRED AMBIVALENCE: ART AND THE ADULT-CHILD DYNAMIC IN AMERICA’S COLD WAR by MICHAEL MARBERRY A THESIS Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts in the Department of English in the Graduate School of The University of Alabama TUSCALOOSA, ALABAMA 2010 Copyright Michael Marberry 2010 ALL RIGHTS RESERVED ABSTRACT The pervasive ideological dimension of the Cold War resulted in an extremely ambivalent period in U.S. history, marked by complex and conflicting feelings. Nowhere is this ambivalence more clearly seen than in the American home and in the relationship between adults and children. Though the adult-child dynamic has frequently harbored ambivalent feelings, the American Cold War era—with its increased emphasis on the family in the face of ideological struggle—served to highlight this ambivalence. Believing that art reveals historical and cultural concerns, this project explores the extent to which adult-child ambivalence is prominent within American art from the period—particularly, the coming-of-age story, as it is a genre intrinsically concerned with the interactions between adults and children. Chapter one features an analysis of Katherine Anne Porter’s “Old Order” coming-of-age sequence, specifically “The Source” and “The Circus.” Establishing Porter’s relevance to the Cold War period, this chapter illustrates how her young heroine (Miranda Gay) experiences ambivalence within her familial relationships—which, in turn, comes to foreshadow and represent the adult-child ambivalence within the Cold War period. Chapter two expands its scope to include a larger historical context and a different artistic mode. With the rise of cinema during the Cold War period, the horror film became a genre extremely interested in adult-child ambivalence, frequently depicting the child as a destructive force and the adult as a victim of parenthood.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Rethinking Jealousy Experience and Expression
    Rethinking Jealousy Experience and Expression: An Examination of Specialness Meaning Framework Threat and Identification of Retroactive Jealousy Responses Dissertation Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University By Jessica Renee Frampton Graduate Program in Communication The Ohio State University 2019 Dissertation Committee Dr. Jesse Fox, Advisor Dr. Kelly Garrett Dr. Shelly Hovick Dr. Roselyn Lee-Won 1 Copyrighted by Jessica Renee Frampton 2019 2 Abstract Extant jealousy models predict jealousy is a response to perceiving a partner’s current or anticipated involvement with a rival as threatening to a relationship’s existence, relational benefits, or self-esteem (e.g., Guerrero & Andersen, 1998; White & Mullen, 1989). Those three threats may explain cases of reactive jealousy, which occurs in response to a partner’s unambiguous involvement with a current rival (Barelds & Barelds-Dijkstra, 2007; Bringle, 1991), but they likely cannot explain cases of retroactive jealousy. Retroactive jealousy entails a negative response to information about a partner’s prior romantic or sexual experiences that occurred before the primary relationship began (Frampton & Fox, 2018b). This type of jealousy is evoked even though the partner is not perceived to be currently romantically or sexually involved with ex-partners. This difference in the nature of retroactive jealousy makes it difficult for current jealousy models to predict retroactive jealousy experience and expression. Two studies were conducted to further explore retroactive jealousy experience and expression. Study 1 experimentally tested predictions about threat to a specialness meaning framework derived from the meaning maintenance model (MMM; Heine, Proulx, & Vohs, 2006; Proulx & Inzlicht, 2012) alongside of predictions about threat to the relationship’s existence, relational benefits, and self-esteem.
    [Show full text]
  • Compassion and Sympathy As Moral Motivation Moral Philosophy Has Long Taken an Interest in the Emotions
    Compassion and Sympathy as Moral Motivation Moral philosophy has long taken an interest in the emotions. Ever since Plato’s defense of the primacy of reason as a source of motiva- tion, moral philosophers have debated the proper role of emotion in the character of a good person and in the choice of individual actions. There are striking contrasts that can be drawn among the main tradi- tions in moral philosophy as to the role they assign to the emotions, and to the particular emotions that they evaluate positively and nega- tively. Here are some examples. Utilitarianism is often presented as a the- ory which simply articulates an ideal of sympathy, where the morally right action is the one that would be favored by someone who is equally sympathetic to the pleasure and pains of all sentient beings. And, on another level, utilitarianism tends to evaluate highly actions motivated by sympathy and compassion, and to evaluate negatively actions motivated by malice and spite. Kantianism (or deontology, as it is often called) has a completely different structure and, conse- quently, a different attitude towards the emotions. It conceives of morality as the self-imposed laws of rational agents, and no emotion is thought to be involved in the generation of these laws. It is true that Kant himself does find a special role for the emotion—if that is the right word—of respect for rational agents and for the laws they impose on themselves. But Kant seems to regard respect as a sort of effect within us of our own inscrutable moral freedom, and not as the source of moral legislation.
    [Show full text]
  • Viral Buzz: Rumor and Its Disruptions in Pandemic Uncertainty
    Special Feature “Against ‘Disaster’: Sociologica. V.15 N.1 (2021) Critical Reflections on the Concept” – peer-reviewed ISSN 1971-8853 https://doi.org/10.6092/issn.1971-8853/11620 https://sociologica.unibo.it/ Viral Buzz: Rumor and Its Disruptions in Pandemic Uncertainty Gary Alan Fine* Department of Sociology, Northwestern University (United States) Submitted: October 18, 2020 – Revised version: February 19, 2021 Accepted: April 25, 2021 – Published: May 26, 2021 Abstract Building on theory and research on rumor dynamics, I examine how the search for and ac- quisition of information during a time of medical crisis relies on the politics of plausibility and the politics of credibility. In this, I examine how the content and the source of informa- tion affects the spread of uncertain knowledge during periods of disaster, recognizing the social dynamics of ignorance, a key issue for domains of knowledge of which the public has little ability to judge. The assertion of multiple truth claims about the current pandemic leads to challenges to previously taken-for-granted realities, but also potentially provides solutions. The dynamic may be different in conditions that require an immediate response and those that evolve over a longer period (fast and slow rumors). Using rumors about the COVID-19 pandemic, I address how epistemic disruption undercuts established norms (disruption-of) but also creates the possibility of desirable change through new negotia- tions, strengthening community (disruption-for). Keywords: Rumor; Knowledge; Disruption; Pandemic; COVID-19. Acknowledgements I thank Iddo Tavory for his comments on a draft of this paper. * [email protected] Copyright © 2021 Gary Alan Fine 43 The text in this work is licensed under the Creative Commons BY License.
    [Show full text]
  • Stoicism (As Emotional Compression) Is Emotional Labor
    Feminist Philosophy Quarterly Volume 6 | Issue 2 Article 4 2020 Stoicism (as Emotional Compression) Is Emotional Labor Olúfẹ́mi O. Táíwò Georgetown University [email protected] Recommended Citation Táíwò, Olúfẹ́mi O. “Stoicism (as Emotional Compression) Is Emotional Labor.” Feminist Philosophy Quarterly 6 (2). Article 4. Táíwò – Stoicism (as Emotional Compression) Is Emotional Labor Stoicism (as Emotional Compression) Is Emotional Labor1 Olúfẹmí O. Táíwò Abstract The criticism of “traditional,” “toxic,” or “patriarchal” masculinity in both academic and popular venues recognizes that there is some sense in which the character traits and tendencies that are associated with masculinity are structurally connected to oppressive, gendered social practices and patriarchal social structures. One important theme of criticism centers on the gender distribution of emotional labor, generally speaking, but this criticism is also particularly meaningful in the context of heterosexual romantic relationships. I begin with the premise that there is a gendered and asymmetrical distribution in how much emotional labor is performed, but I also consider that there might be meaningful and informative distinctions in what kind of emotional labor is characteristically performed by different genders. Specifically, I argue that the social norms around stoicism and restricted emotional expression are masculine-coded forms of emotional labor, and that they are potentially prosocial. Responding to structural and interpersonal asymmetries of emotional labor could well involve supplementing or better cultivating this aspect of male socialization rather than discarding it. Keywords: Stoicism, emotional labor, work, masculinity, feminism Socrates: We would be right, then, to remove the lamentations of famous men. We would leave them to women (provided they are not excellent women) and cowardly men.
    [Show full text]