Social Bonding and Nurture Kinship: Compatibility Between Cultural and Biological Approaches / Maximilian P

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Social Bonding and Nurture Kinship: Compatibility Between Cultural and Biological Approaches / Maximilian P London School of Economics and Political Science SOCIAL BONDING & NURTURE KINSHIP compatibility between cultural and biological approaches (UNABRIDGED DOCTORAL THESIS EDITION) Maximilian Holland Copyright © 2004 Maximilian P. Holland Copyright © 2012 Maximilian P. Holland All rights reserved. First Edition published 2004 in the UK: Senate House Library, University of London, Malet Street, London WC1E 7HU This edition is essentially an unabridged version of the originally published doctoral thesis, reformatted, with typographic corrections and a new preface. An online version of this text available under a Creative Commons Noncommercial Sharealike license; it can be accessed through various online holdings, including the British Library. 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 British Library Cataloguing Data A catalogue record for this title is available from the British Library. British Library Persistent ID: uk.bl.ethos.411642 Publication Type: Thesis (Ph.D.) - University of London, 2004. Awarding Body: London School of Economics and Political Science LSE Persistent ID: etheses.lse.ac.uk/465/ Holland, Maximilian P. Social bonding and nurture kinship: compatibility between cultural and biological approaches / Maximilian P. Holland Library of Congress subject classification: H Social Sciences > HM Sociology 332 pages. Includes table of contents and bibliographic references. ISBN-13: 9781480182004 ISBN-10: 1480182001 ii SOCIAL BONDING & NURTURE KINSHIP compatibility between cultural and biological approaches [This page blank] iv PREFACE TO THE 2012 EDITION This book contains the first widely available print edition of the doctoral thesis Social bonding and nurture kinship, completed at the London School of Economics and Political Science (LSE) in 2004. The thesis has previously existed solely in manuscript format in the LSE library archives, and has more recently become available in digital format from that same institution and also from the British library. Why is this work only now available in book format? At the time, the thesis examiners (Elliott Sober and Christina Toren) were quick to suggest that it should be of considerable interest to scholars and academic publishers alike, with likely just a re-hash of the introduction and conclusion to broaden its appeal. But having successfully completed the research, and about to commence a teaching post, the last thing I felt like doing at the time was to immediately plunge back into rewriting two or more chapters. I wanted to let my hair down and have a change of mental scenery, and to enjoy the more tangible satisfactions of engaging students with ideas and critical enquiry, the potential for which had been one of my prime motivations in undertaking the PhD. Another factor in putting the work to one side in 2004 was my disappointment with some of the levels of scholarship in the fields I had spent four years investigating. In retrospect, part of that disappointment was in fact with myself, for having fallen into an attraction to and engagement with certain areas of biological anthropology that appeared to promise a rigorous foundation upon which to understand the human condition. I had been exposed to the British educational system, with its training in the positivist tradition, trust in hard sciences, tendency towards reductionism, and neglect of the role of culture, relativism, and even modest aspects of postmodernism. Coming from this background, although there was the potential to engage with the latter ideas during undergraduate studies at the LSE, I was ignorant of their contribution and did not seek them out. Thus my initial attraction to and faith in the tools and frames of reference of areas of biological anthropology. For a young scholar there is of course also a great emotional appeal in a perspective that promises a rigorous and formal handle on a complex world, that makes it feel more v predictable, and less threatening. On a more conciliatory note I would maintain, both then and now, that a perspective on humans grounded in our ontogeny as part of the biological world is both well supported beyond the frailty of any single paradigm, perhaps logically unavoidable (it is hard to define ‗human‘ for purposes of study without repeal to at least some biology) and increasingly urgent as we necessarily collectively re-examine our relationship with natural environments and our long term trajectory. There are thus a priori reasons why rigorous insights from biology (if they can be arrived at) may enable a more nuanced understanding of the human condition. In practice however academic disciplines often suffer from training their members in a narrow tradition, suffer from cultural divisions, and compartmentalisation. These problems sometimes amplify into territorial disputes with other disciplines, mutual misunderstanding and sometimes mistrust, defensiveness and parochialism. Interdisciplinary scholarship, though much lauded, is seldom encouraged. The narrow disciplinary path, if unchecked, can lead towards dogmatism around methods, core assumptions (beliefs) and even data. Having carefully worked through the theories and evidence around social behaviour and kinship during the research it has been hard to avoid the conclusion, then and now, that at least a small number of areas of biological anthropology (some parts of sociobiology and evolutionary psychology) may have suffered from these difficulties. That‘s not to say that some colleagues across the metaphorical corridor in cultural anthropology and other social sciences could not have more constructively engaged with the apparent gaps, though it is hard to do this when your entire body of data (ethnography) is disregarded and your approach is supposedly threatened with colonisation and reductionism! The current synthesis outlines a more constructive resolution. What lessons might be drawn from this impasse and its eventual resolution? I would first look to narrow disciplinarity as the root of the problem. Then towards the general lack of awareness of the importance of culture (despite over a hundred years since Boas); both in how disciplinary culture affects vi our own openness as scientists (making potentially complimentary frameworks seem foreign and unwelcome); and especially in the human sciences, in how our inherited societal culture often constrains those of us tinkering with ideas about human nature to unwittingly derive our models from a culturally particular perspective which we then (and necessarily unsuccessfully) attempt to generalise to all humans. It is tempting to attribute these shortcomings to biologists, but note that cultural anthropologists themselves had only begun to deconstruct their own narrowly derived models of human kinship in the period between the late 1960s through the early 1980s (chapters 1, 3 and 8). In a historical sense then, it was this unfortunate timing that created the initial clash and subsequent impasse. Cultural anthropologists were changing their theoretical position on kinship around the time that biologists were starting to draw upon the kinship data that anthropologists had largely produced under the increasingly discredited ‗old model‘. The work in this book inevitably had to wade through such debates, but precisely by engaging seriously with the theories and evidence of several approaches, it has produced a robust general framework for their resolution in this particular area of study (social bonding, social behaviour and kinship). Readers will justifiably question how much of the original thesis remains valid and relevant in 2012, whilst research in the areas under study has not stood still. Two points should be made: Firstly, the thesis builds its synthesis largely around the identification of a logical error common to sociobiological accounts, and supports this critique (and proposes a more parsimonious position) on the basis of both argument and wide ranging evidence. Here the fundamentals are unchanged. On the one hand, the same common error remains dominant in evolutionary psychology accounts; research continues to be regularly published which attempts to describe the extent to which the expression of human social behaviour is biased towards identifying and favouring genetic relatives, based on the mistaken assumption that this is a prediction of the fundamental biological theory. On the other hand, the broad pattern of the surrounding evidence drawn upon in the thesis has not been reversed by recent findings, just more detail added. vii Secondly, debates around the robustness of inclusive fitness theory have arisen in recent years, specifically its relative parsimony compared to a natural selection approach as an accounting method for analysing multi-level selection. The language and framework of inclusive fitness theory forms the central thread of the biological account in the thesis (see mainly chapter 2), because this has long been the dominant theory and remains the lingua franca in biology. The thesis employs a broad definition of inclusive fitness, identifying the later developments of Price (1970), Hamilton (1975), Grafen (1985), Queller (1992) and Frank (1997) as being more accurate than Hamilton‘s commonly referenced 1960s papers. The thesis also deliberately distinguishes inclusive fitness theory from the narrower kin selection theory (cf. Hamilton 1975). In doing so, and in specifying the generality of inclusive fitness theory regarding levels of selection analysis, the thesis is largely neutral in respect of these recent debates. Further, an important aspect of the
Recommended publications
  • Evolution, Politics and Law
    Valparaiso University Law Review Volume 38 Number 4 Summer 2004 pp.1129-1248 Summer 2004 Evolution, Politics and Law Bailey Kuklin Follow this and additional works at: https://scholar.valpo.edu/vulr Part of the Law Commons Recommended Citation Bailey Kuklin, Evolution, Politics and Law, 38 Val. U. L. Rev. 1129 (2004). Available at: https://scholar.valpo.edu/vulr/vol38/iss4/1 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Valparaiso University Law School at ValpoScholar. It has been accepted for inclusion in Valparaiso University Law Review by an authorized administrator of ValpoScholar. For more information, please contact a ValpoScholar staff member at [email protected]. Kuklin: Evolution, Politics and Law VALPARAISO UNIVERSITY LAW REVIEW VOLUME 38 SUMMER 2004 NUMBER 4 Article EVOLUTION, POLITICS AND LAW Bailey Kuklin* I. Introduction ............................................... 1129 II. Evolutionary Theory ................................. 1134 III. The Normative Implications of Biological Dispositions ......................... 1140 A . Fact and Value .................................... 1141 B. Biological Determinism ..................... 1163 C. Future Fitness ..................................... 1183 D. Cultural N orm s .................................. 1188 IV. The Politics of Sociobiology ..................... 1196 A. Political Orientations ......................... 1205 B. Political Tactics ................................... 1232 V . C onclusion ................................................. 1248 I. INTRODUCTION
    [Show full text]
  • Anthropological Evolutionary Ecology: a Critique
    Journal of Ecological Anthropology Volume 4 Issue 1 Volume 4, Issue 1 (2000) Article 1 2000 Anthropological Evolutionary Ecology: A Critique Suzanne Joseph University of Georgia, Department of Anthropology Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarcommons.usf.edu/jea Recommended Citation Joseph, Suzanne. "Anthropological Evolutionary Ecology: A Critique." Journal of Ecological Anthropology 4, no. 1 (2000): 6-30. Available at: https://scholarcommons.usf.edu/jea/vol4/iss1/1 This Research Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Anthropology at Scholar Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Journal of Ecological Anthropology by an authorized editor of Scholar Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. 6 Journal of Ecological Anthropology Vol. 4 2000 ARTICLES Anthropological Evolutionary Ecology: A Critique SUZANNE JOSEPH1 Abstract The goal of this paper is to critically evaluate Anthropological Evolutionary Ecology (AEE) as a paradigm by utilizing the method for theory framework developed by Pickett et al. (1994). While AEE can contribute in some ways to our understanding of human behavior through methods and techniques derived from neo- Darwinian theory (as well as current approaches in animal behavior and decision theory), AEE as a para- digm remains theoretically ill-equipped for the study of human ecology. This critique will focus on Anthropo- logical Evolutionary Ecology, however, references will be made to Biological Evolutionary Ecology (BEE) since AEE relies heavily on theoretical components derived from BEE. Introduction A critique of Anthropological Evolution- increasing completeness of theory. Box 1 de- ary Ecology (AEE) as a theoretical paradigm scribes the major components of theory that are should begin with a definition of paradigm.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolutionary Psychology: a Primer
    Evolutionary Psychology: A Primer Leda Cosmides & John Tooby Center for Evolutionary Psychology UC Santa Barbara http://www.psych.ucsb.edu/research/cep/ Introduction The goal of research in evolutionary psychology is to discover and understand the design of the human mind. Evolutionary psychology is an approach to psychology, in which knowledge and principles from evolutionary biology are put to use in research on the structure of the human mind. It is not an area of study, like vision, reasoning, or social behavior. It is a way of thinking about psychology that can be applied to any topic within it. In this view, the mind is a set of information-processing machines that were designed by natural selection to solve adaptive problems faced by our hunter-gatherer ancestors. This way of thinking about the brain, mind, and behavior is changing how scientists approach old topics, and opening up new ones. This chapter is a primer on the concepts and arguments that animate it. Debauching the mind: Evolutionary psychology's past and present In the final pages of the Origin of Species, after he had presented the theory of evolution by natural selection, Darwin made a bold prediction: "In the distant future I see open fields for far more important researches. Psychology will be based on a new foundation, that of the necessary acquirement of each mental power and capacity by gradation." Thirty years later, William James tried to do just that in his seminal book, Principles of Psychology, one of the founding works of experimental psychology (James, 1890). In Principles, James talked a lot of "instincts".
    [Show full text]
  • Sense and Nonsense: Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behaviour
    Sense and Nonsense: Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behaviour Kevin N. Laland Gillian R. Brown OXFORD UNIVERSITY PRESS SN-Prelims (i-xii) 3/4/02 12:22 PM Page i Sense and Nonsense SN-Prelims (i-xii) 3/4/02 12:22 PM Page ii This page intentionally left blank SN-Prelims (i-xii) 3/4/02 12:22 PM Page iii Sense and Nonsense Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behaviour Kevin N. Laland Royal Society University Research Fellow Sub-Department of Animal Behaviour University of Cambridge and Gillian R. Brown Research Scientist Sub-Department of Animal Behaviour University of Cambridge 1 SN-Prelims (i-xii) 3/4/02 12:22 PM Page iv 1 Great Clarendon Street, Oxford OX2 6DP Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers the University's objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide in Oxford New York Auckland Bangkok Buenos Aires Cape Town Chennai Dar es Salaam Delhi Hong Kong Istanbul Karachi Kolkata Kuala Lumpur Madrid Melbourne Mexico City Mumbai Nairobi São Paulo Shanghai Taipei Tokyo Toronto and an associated company in Berlin Oxford is a registered trade mark of Oxford University Press in the UK and in certain other countries Published in the United States By Oxford University Press Inc., New York © Kevin N. Laland and Gillian R. Brown, 2002 The moral rights of the authors have been asserted Database right Oxford University Press (maker) First published 2002 All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, without the prior permission in writing of Oxford University Press, or as expressly permitted by law, or under terms agreed with the appropriate reprographics rights organization.
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolutionary Subject : Science Fiction from the Perspective of Darwinian Literary Studies
    Title: The Evolutionary Subject : Science Fiction from the Perspective of Darwinian Literary Studies Author: Bartłomiej Kuchciński Citation style: Kuchciński Bartłomiej. (2019). The Evolutionary Subject : Science Fiction from the Perspective of Darwinian Literary Studies. Praca doktorska. Katowice : Uniwersytet Śląski UNIVERSITY OF SILESIA IN KATOWICE FACULTY OF PHILOLOGY BARTŁOMIEJ KUCHCIŃSKI 5118 THE EVOLUTIONARY SUBJECT: SCIENCE FICTION FROM THE PERSPECTIVE OF DARWINIAN LITERARY STUDIES PHD THESIS SUPERVISOR: Prof. zw. dr hab. Wojciech Kalaga SOSNOWIEC, 2019 UNIWERSYTET ŚLĄSKI W KATOWICACH WYDZIAŁ FILOLOGICZNY BARTŁOMIEJ KUCHCIŃSKI 5118 PODMIOT EWOLUCYJNY: FANTASTYKA NAUKOWA Z PERSPEKTYWY LITERATUROZNAWSTWA DARWINISTYCZNEGO ROZPRAWA DOKTORSKA Praca w języku angielskim PROMOTOR: Prof. zw. dr hab. Wojciech Kalaga SOSNOWIEC, 2019 Table of Contents INTRODUCTION ....................................................................................................... 1 CHAPTER 1: INTO THE FRAY: THE SOCIOBIOLOGICAL PARADIGM AND SCIENTIFIC CONSILIENCE ..................................................................................... 7 The Scope of Darwinist Thought ............................................................................. 8 Evolutionary Explanations of Human Behaviour................................................... 12 Scientific Consilience ............................................................................................. 34 Conclusion .............................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Culture and the Evolution Learning of Social
    ELSEVIER Culture and the Evolution of Social Learning Mark V. Flinn Department of Anthropology, University of Missouri Applications of modern evolutionary theory to human culture have generated several different theoretical approaches that challenge traditional anthropological perspectives. “Cultural selection” and “mind parasite” theories model culture as an independent evo- lutionary system because transmission of cultural traits via social learning is distinct from transmission of genes vla DNA replication. “Dual-inheritance” and “co-evolution” theories model culture as an intermediary evolutionary process that involves informa- tion from two inheritance systems: genetics and social learning. “Evolutionary psychol- ogy” theories emphasize that the evolutionary history of natural selection on mental pro- cesses links culture and biological adaptation; hence, cultural information is viewed as part of the organic phenotype and not an independent evolutionary system. Cross-cul- tural universals and scenarios of the “environment of evolutionary adaptedness” are used to identify characteristics of the “evolved mind” (human nature). “Behavioral ecol- ogy” theories examine relations between behavior and environmental context. Behav- ioral/cultural variations are viewed as products of flexible decision-making processes (evolved mind) that may respond adaptively to micro-environmental differences. It is difficult to devise empirical tests that distinguish among these theories, because they share many basic premises and make similar predictions
    [Show full text]
  • Institutional Evolution in the Holocene: the Rise of Complex Societies
    INSTITUTIONAL EVOLUTION IN THE HOLOCENE: THE RISE OF COMPLEX SOCIETIES Peter J. Richerson Department of Environmental Science and Policy University of California Davis [email protected] Robert Boyd Department of Anthropology University of California Los Angeles [email protected] Keywords: Cultural evolution, complex societies, origins of agriculture, evolution of institutions Summary: The evolution of complex societies began when agricultural subsistence systems raised human population densities to levels that would support large scale cooperation, and division of labor. All agricultural origins sequences postdate 11,500 years ago probably because late Pleistocene climates we extremely variable, dry, and the atmosphere was low in carbon dioxide. Under such conditions, agriculture was likely impossible. However, the tribal scale societies of the Pleistocene did acquire, by gene- culture coevolution, tribal social instincts that simultaneously enable and constrain the evolution of complex societies. Once agriculture became possible, a competitive ratchet drove further improvements in subsistence and in scale of social organization . Those societies that grew and became better organized were advantaged in individual wealth and economic and military power, and tended to conquer, absorb, or be imitated by smaller and less well organized societies. Internal competitors for power espousing useful social innovations could deliver improved returns when their quest was successful. Notwithstanding the ratchet, social complexity increased only slowly in the first half of the Holocene and even afterwards few periods except the past two centuries saw changes that were dramatic on the scale of individual lifetimes. We attempt a taxonomy of the processes that regulate rates of institutional evolution, cause reversals of complexity against the ratchet, and impose historical contingency on institutional evolution.i April 2000.
    [Show full text]
  • Synthesis in the Human Evolutionary Behavioural Sciences
    Rebecca Sear Synthesis in the human evolutionary behavioural sciences Article (Accepted version) (Refereed) Original citation: Sear, Rebecca and Lawson, David W. and Dickins, Thomas E. (2007) Synthesis in the human evolutionary behavioural sciences. Journal of evolutionary psychology, 5 (1-4). pp. 3-28. DOI: 10.1556/JEP.2007.1019 © 2007 Akadémiai Kiadó This version available at: http://eprints.lse.ac.uk/21227/ Available in LSE Research Online: October 2008 LSE has developed LSE Research Online so that users may access research output of the School. Copyright © and Moral Rights for the papers on this site are retained by the individual authors and/or other copyright owners. Users may download and/or print one copy of any article(s) in LSE Research Online to facilitate their private study or for non-commercial research. You may not engage in further distribution of the material or use it for any profit-making activities or any commercial gain. You may freely distribute the URL (http://eprints.lse.ac.uk) of the LSE Research Online website. This document is the author’s final manuscript accepted version of the journal article, incorporating any revisions agreed during the peer review process. Some differences between this version and the published version may remain. You are advised to consult the publisher’s version if you wish to cite from it. Synthesis in the Human Evolutionary Behavioural Sciences Running title: the human evolutionary behavioural sciences Rebecca Sear12 Department of Social Policy London School of Economics David W. Lawson Human Evolutionary Ecology Group Department of Anthropology University College London Thomas E.
    [Show full text]
  • Synthesis in the Human Evolutionary Behavioural Sciences
    Synthesis in the Human Evolutionary Behavioural Sciences Running title: the human evolutionary behavioural sciences Rebecca Sear12 Department of Social Policy London School of Economics David W. Lawson Human Evolutionary Ecology Group Department of Anthropology University College London Thomas E. Dickins School of Psychology University of East London & Centre for Philosophy of Natural and Social Science London School of Economics 1 Corresponding author. Address: Department of Social Policy, London School of Economics, Houghton St, London WC2A 2AE, UK. Tel: 020 7955 7348. Fax: 020 7955 7415. Email: [email protected] 2 All authors contributed equally to this paper. We have ordered authorship in reverse alphabetical order 1 Abstract Over the last three decades, the application of evolutionary theory to the human sciences has shown remarkable growth. This growth has also been characterised by a ‘splitting’ process, with the emergence of distinct sub-disciplines, most notably: Human Behavioural Ecology (HBE), Evolutionary Psychology (EP) and studies of Cultural Evolution (CE). Multiple applications of evolutionary ideas to the human sciences are undoubtedly a good thing, demonstrating the usefulness of this approach to human affairs. Nevertheless, this fracture has been associated with considerable tension, a lack of integration, and sometimes outright conflict between researchers. In recent years however, there have been clear signs of hope that a synthesis of the human evolutionary behavioural sciences is underway. Here, we briefly review the history of the debate, both its theoretical and practical causes; then provide evidence that the field is currently becoming more integrated, as the traditional boundaries between sub-disciplines become blurred. This article constitutes the first paper under the new editorship of the Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, which aims to further this integration by explicitly providing a forum for integrated work.
    [Show full text]
  • The Creative Life of Bill Hamilton
    Human Ethology Bulletin – Proc. of the XXII. ISHE Conference (2015): 7-21 Theoretical Review SCIENCE AS ADVENTURE: THE CREATIVE LIFE OF BILL HAMILTON Ullica Segerstrale Department of Social Sciences, Illinois Institute of Technology, 3301 S. Dearborn St., Siegel Hall 116, Chicago, IL 60616, USA. [email protected] ABSTRACT Half a century ago the paper by graduate student William Donald “Bill” Hamilton, "The Genetical Evolution of Social Behaviour" (1964), started a paradigm shift in science. That paper showed how basic social behaviors – selfishness, altruism, cooperation, and spite – could be expressed in the language of population genetics, thus opening the door to mathematical model building and testing. He showed especially that altruism can evolve as long as the benefit of an altruistic act falls on a genetically related individual rather than on a random member of a population. Later Hamilton, ever the pioneer, was to open up many other new research fields. But his ideas were often too novel and he had a hard time convincing journal referees. What they did not know was the range of methods by which he privately arrived at his conclusions: from “external” naturalistic exploration and mathematical modeling to “internal” anthropomorphic understanding of the study object, to a knowledge state that involved a veritable merger between observer and observed. Colorful computer simulation became the natural mediator between his naturalistic, esthetic and mathematical talents. Brazil played a huge liberating and stimulating role in Hamilton’s life and it became his home abroad, away from his serious Oxford professor persona. He was especially intrigued by the evolutionary puzzles of the flooded forest and helped develop ecological research in the area.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 21 Ethology, Sociobiology, and Evolutionary Psychology Paul E
    Chapter 21 Ethology, Sociobiology, and Evolutionary Psychology paul e. griffiths “It is only a comparative and evolutionary psychology that can provide the needed basis; and this could not be created before the work of Darwin.” William McDougall, Introduction to Social Psychology, 1908 1. A Century of Evolutionary Psychology The evolution of mind and behavior was of intense interest to Charles Darwin through- out his life. His views were made public a decade before his death in The Descent of Man (e.g., 1981 [1871]) and The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals (1965 [1872]). Evolutionary psychology has been an active fi eld of research and a topic of public controversy from that time to the present. At least four distinct phases can be distinguished in the development of evolutionary psychology since Darwin and his immediate successor George Romanes. These are: instinct theory, classical ethology, sociobiology, and Evolutionary Psychology, the last of which I capitalize to distinguish it from evolutionary psychology in general. The instinct theories of Conwy Lloyd Morgan, James Mark Baldwin, William James, William McDougall, and others were an important part of early-twentieth-century psychology (Richards, 1987) but will not be discussed here because no trace of these theories can be discerned in evolutionary psychology today. It was not until the years leading up to World War II that the ethologists Konrad Lorenz and Nikolaas Tinbergen created the tradition of rigorous, Darwinian research on animal behavior that devel- oped into modern behavioral ecology (Burkhardt, 2005). At fi rst glance, research on specifi cally human behavior seems to exhibit greater discontinuity than research on animal behavior in general.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolution of Social Behaviour Patterns in Primates and Man
    Reprinted from PROCEEDINGS OF THE BRITISH ACADEMY 88 EVOLUTION OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOUR PATTERNS IN PRIMATES AND MAN W.G. Runciman, J. Maynard Smith, and R.I.M. Dunbar (editors) Oxford University Press Proceedings of the British Academy, 88, 119-143 Friendship and the Banker's Paradox: Other pathways to the Evolution of Adaptations for Altruism JOHN TOOBY & LEDA COSMIDES Center for Evolutionary Psychology, University of California, Santa Barbara, CA 93106, USA Keywords: reciprocity; altruism; co-operation; social exchange; reciprocal altruism; evolutionary psychology. Summary. The classical definition of altruism in evolutionary biology requires that an organism incur a fitness cost in the course of providing others with a fitness benefit. New insights are gained, however, by exploring the implications of an adaptationist version of the 'problem of altruism', as the existence of machinery designed to deliver benefits to others. Alternative pathways for the evolution of altruism are discussed, which avoid barriers thought to limit the emergence of reciprocation across species. We define the Banker's Paradox, and show how its solution can select for cognitive machinery designed to deliver benefits to others, even in the absence of traditional reciprocation. These models allow one to understand aspects of the design and social dynamics of human friendship that are otherwise mysterious. FROM A SELECTIONIST TO AN ADAPTATIONIST ANALYSIS OF ALTRUISM THEANALYSIS OF THE EVOLUTION OF ALTRUISM has been a central focus of modern evolutionary biology for almost four decades, ever since Williams, Hamilton, and Maynard Smith caused researchers to appreciate its significance (Williams & Williams 1957; Hamilton 1963, 1964; and Maynard 0 The British Academy 1996.
    [Show full text]