REFERENCES Abrams, M. (2001). the Biology Of… Perfect Pitch: Name That Tone

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

REFERENCES Abrams, M. (2001). the Biology Of… Perfect Pitch: Name That Tone REFERENCES Abrams, M. (2001). The biology of… perfect pitch: Name that tone. Can your child learn some of Mozart’s magic? Discover, December, 22 (12), <wysiwyg://9/http://ww w.discover.com/dec-01/ featbiology.html>. ABRSM. Scales, Arpeggios, and Broken Chord: Piano, Grade 3. London: The Associated Board of the Royal Schools of Music Publishing Ltd., 3. Agay, D. Denes Agay’s Learning to Play Piano, Book 1: Primer (New Revised Edition with Colour Guide). New York: Yorktown Music Press, Inc., 5 & 7. Alcott, M. (1997). An Introduction to Children with Special Education Needs. Scotland: Holder & Stoughton Educational. American Standards Association (1960). Acoustical Terminology SI, 1-1960. New York: American Standards Association. Andrews, F. M. and Deih, N. C. (1967). Development of a Technique for Identifying Elementary School Children’s Musical Concepts. US Office of Education Project 5-0233, Washington, D.C. Anastasi, A. and Levee, R. F. (1960). Intellectual deficit and musical talent: A case report. American Journal of Mental Deficiency. 64 (4), 695-703. Annett, J. (1989). Skills. In A. M. Colman and J. G. Beaumont (Eds.), Psychology Survey. London: Routledge. Atkinson, R. C. and Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Human Memory: A proposed system and its control process. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), Human Memory: Basic Process. New York: Academic Press. Attneave, F. and Olson, R. K. (1971) Pitch as a medium: A new approach to psycholophysical scaling. American Journal of Psychology, 84, 147-166. Bachem, A. (1937). Various types of absolute pitch. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 9, 146-151. Bachem, A. (1940). The genesis of absolute pitch. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 11, 434-439. Bachem, A. (1948a). Chroma fixation at the ends of the musical frequency scale. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 20 (5), 704-705. Bachem, A. (1948b). Note on Neu’s review of the literature on absolute pitch. Psychological Bulletin, 45 (2), 161-162. Bachem, A. (1954). Time factor in relative and absolute pitch determination. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 26 (5), 751-753. Bachem, A. (1955). Absolute pitch. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 27 (6), 1180-1185. Backus, J. (1969). The Acoustical Foundations of Music. New York: W. W. Norton & Co. 267 Baddeley, A. D. (1976). The Psychology of Memory. New York: Basic Books. Baggaley, J. (1974). Measurements of absolute pitch. Psychology of Music, 2 (2), 11-17. Baharloo, S.; Johnston, P.A.; Services, S.K.; Gitschier, J; and Freimer, N.B. (1998). Absolute pitch: An approach for identification of genetic and non-genetic components. American Journal of Human Genetics, 62, 224-231. Baharloo, S.; Services, S. K.; Risch, N.; Gitschier, J.; and Freimer, N. B. (2000). Familial aggregation of absolute pitch. America Journal of Human Genetics, 67 (3), 755-758. Baird, J. W. (1917). Memory of absolute pitch: Studies in Psychology, Titchener Commemorative Volume. Worcesler, MA: Wilson, 43-78. Balzano, G. J. (1984). Absolute pitch and pure tone identification. The Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 75 (2), 623-625. Balzano, G. J. (1986). What are musical pitch and timbre? Music Perception, 3 (3), 297-314. Barbour, J. M. (1972). Tuning and Temperament: A Historical Survey. New York: Da Capo Press. Barnea, A; Granot, R. and Pratt, H. (1994). Absolute pitch-Electrophysiology evidence. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 16 (1), 29-38. Bartholomew, W. T. (1942). Acoustics of Music. New York: Prentice-Hall. Bastien, J. Bastien Piano Basics: Piano, Primer Level. San Diego: Neil A. Kjos Music Company, 8-12, 14 & 43. Bekerian, D. A. and Baddeley, A. D. (1980). Saturation Advertising and the repetition effect. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 19, 17-25. Benguerel, A. P. and Westdal, C. (1991). Absolute pitch and the perception of sequential musical intervals. Music Perception, 9 (1), 105-120. Bergeson, T. R. and Trehub, S. E. (2002). Absolute pitch and tempo in mothers' songs to infants. Psychological Science, 13 (1), 72-75. Block, L. (1983). Comparative tone-colour responses of college music majors with absolute pitch and good relative pitch. Psychology of Music, 11 (2), 59-66. Board of Education. (1996). Report of the Sub-committee on Special Education. Hong Kong: The Government Printer. Boggs, L. P. (1907). Studies in absolute pitch. American Journal of Psychology, 18 (2), 194-205. Brady, P. T. (1970). Fixed-scale mechanism of absolute pitch. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 48, 883-887. 268 Brammer, L. M. (1951). Sensory cues in pitch judgement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41 (5), 336-340. Braun, M. (2001). Speech mirrors norm-tones: Absolute pitch as a normal but precognitive trait. Acoustics Research Online, April, 85-90. Braun, M. (2002). Absolute pitch in emphasized speech. Acoustics Research Letters Online, March, 77-82. Brown, J. A. (1958). Some test of the decay theory of immediate memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 10, 12-21. Brown, A. and Dowling, P. (1998). Doing Research / Reading Research: A Mode of Interrogation for Education. London: The Falmer Press. Brown, K. (1999). Striking the right note. New Scientists, 164 (2215), 38. Brown, S.; Merker, B. and Wallin, N. L. (2001). An introduction to evolustionary Musicology. In N.L. Wallin, B. Merker, and S. Brown (Eds.), The Origins of Music (2nd ed.). Massachusett: The MIT Press, 3-24. Brown, W. A.; Cammuso, K.; Sachs, H.; Winklosky, B.; Mullane, J.; Bernier, R.; Svenson, S.; Arin, D.; Rosen-Sheidley, B. and Folstein, S. E. (2003). Autism-related language, personality and cognition in people with absolute pitch: result of a preliminary study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 163-167. Burnam, E. M. A Dozen A Day, Book 1 (Chinese Version). Taipei: Tien Yin Publishing, 27. Burge, D.L. (1986). The Perfect Pitch Ear-Training Course. America Educational Music Publications, Inc. Burlingham, D. (1976). The Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, Volume XXIII. New York: International University Press, Inc. Burns, M. (1999). Intervals, Scales, and Tuning. In Deutsch (Ed.). The Psychology of Music (2nd ed.). San Diego: Academic Press, 215-264. Burns, E. M. and Campbell, S. L. (1994). Frequency and Frequency-ratio resolution by possessors of relative and absolute pitch: Examples of categorical perception? The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 96 (5), Pt. 1, 2704-2719. Burns, E. M. and Feth, L. L. (1983). Pitch of sinusoids and complex tones above 10kHz. In R. Klinke and R. Hartmann (Eds.), Hearing-Physiological Bases and Psychophysics. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. Burns, E. M. and Ward, W. D. (1978). Categorical-phenomenon or epiphenomenon? Evidence from experiments in the perception of melodic musical intervals. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 63, 456-488. Butler, D. (1992). The Musician's Guide to Perception and Cognition. New York: Schirmer Books. 269 Campbell, M. and Greated, C. (1987). The Musician's Guide to Acoustics. New York: Schirmer Books. Carpenter, A. (1951). A case of absolute pitch. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3, Pt. 2, 92-93. Carroll, J. B. (1975). Speed and accuracy of absolute pitch judgements: Some latter-day results. Educational Testing Service Research Bulletin (RB-75-35). Princetion, NJ: Educational Testing Service. Carroll, J. B. and Greenberg, J. H. (1961). Two cases of synesthesis for colour and musical tonality associated with absolute pitch ability. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 13, 48. Carterette, E. C. and Kendall, R. A. (1999). Comparative music perception and cognition. In D. Deutsch (Ed.). The Psychology of Music (2nd ed.). San Diego: Academic Press, 725-791. Chang, C. C. (2003). Fundamentals of Piano Practice (2nd ed.). <http://members.aol. com/chang8828>. Charness, N.; Clifton, J. and MacDonald L. (1988). Case study of a musical “mono-savant”: A cognitive-psychological focus. In L. K. Obler and D. Fein (Eds.), The Exceptional Brain: Neuropsychology of Talent and Special Abilities. New York: The Guilford Press, 277-293. Cherulnik, P. D. (2001). Methods for Behavioural Research: A Systematic Approach. California: Sage Publications, Inc. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power and Analysis for the Behaviour Sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Cohen, A. J. and Baird, K. (1990). The acquisition of absolute pitch: The question of critical periods. Psychomusicology, 9 (1), 31-37. Comer, D. (1985, May 16). Musical talent brightens life of blind severely retarded man. The Toronto Star, 6. Copp, E. F. (1916). Musical ability. Journal of Heredity, 7, 297-305. Corliss, E. L. (1973). Remark on “fixed-scale mechanism of absolute pitch”. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 53, 1737-1739. Collier, J. L. (1983). Louis Armstrong: An American Genius. New York: Oxford University Press. Corso, J. F. (1957). Absolute judgements of musical tonality. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 29 (1), 138-144. Crozier, J. (1980). Absolute pitch. Paper presented at the International Society for Empirical Aesthetics, Montreal. 270 Crozier, J. B. (1997). Absolute Pitch: Practice makes perfect, the earlier the better. Psychology of Music, 25, 110-119. Crummer, G. C.; Walton, J. P.; Wayman, J. W.; Hantz, E. C. and Frisina, R. D. (1994). Neural processing of musical timbre by musicians, non-musicians and musicians possessing absolute pitch. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 95 (5), Pt. 1, 2720-2727. Crutchfield, W. (1990). There may be more to music than meets a typical ear. The New York Times, December 23, 31 & 42. Costall, A. (1985). The relativity of absolute pitch. In P. Howard, I. Cross, and R. West (Eds.), Musical Structure and Cognition. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, 189-208. Costall, A. (1987). The magical number seven plus and minus two: the problem of perfect pitch. Psychology of Music, 15, 109. Cuddy, L. L. (1968). Practice effects in the absolute judgement of pitch. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 43, 1069-1076.
Recommended publications
  • MUSIC in the (Deaf) BRAIN Musical Ear Training with Cochlear Implants
    MUSIC IN THE (deaf) BRAIN Musical ear training with cochlear implants by Bjørn Petersen Musical training and testing ( Figures 3 & 4). Rhythm and pitch discrimination also showed a difference in favor of the music group, though smaller. “What would you do if I sang out of tune, Sixteen newly operated adult CI users (21-73 years) matched We observed a progress but no difference in the ability to would you stand up and walk out on me?” in two groups, took part in this longitudinal study. Shortly discriminate melodies and vocal emotions. Ringo Starr in “With a little help from my friends” after switch-on of the CI the eight subjects in the music (Lennon & McCartney 1967). group began weekly one-to-one musical ear training lessons, that contained a variety of musical activities and listening Electronic ears exercises. For home practice, we provided specially adapted audio-visual training material. The remaining eight subjects A Cochlear Implant (CI) is a neural prosthesis that helps acted as controls, and did not receive any musical training. deaf people to hear. A surgically inserted electrode in the cochlea stimulates the neurons, whereby the auditory nerve To detect the progress in discrimination of pitch, rhythm Figure 2 Figure 5 is activated. This way sound signals reach the brain’s auditory and timbre we created a battery of music tests (Figure 1). Speech perception scores Two single subject cases of neurological and behavioral plasticity as system, in many cases allowing recipients to converse on the documented in PET scans and behavioral tests done at 0, 3 and 6 months Perception of speech and prosody was measured with the after switch-on of the CI sound processor.
    [Show full text]
  • Pedagogical Practices Related to the Ability to Discern and Correct
    Florida State University Libraries Electronic Theses, Treatises and Dissertations The Graduate School 2014 Pedagogical Practices Related to the Ability to Discern and Correct Intonation Errors: An Evaluation of Current Practices, Expectations, and a Model for Instruction Ryan Vincent Scherber Follow this and additional works at the FSU Digital Library. For more information, please contact [email protected] FLORIDA STATE UNIVERSITY COLLEGE OF MUSIC PEDAGOGICAL PRACTICES RELATED TO THE ABILITY TO DISCERN AND CORRECT INTONATION ERRORS: AN EVALUATION OF CURRENT PRACTICES, EXPECTATIONS, AND A MODEL FOR INSTRUCTION By RYAN VINCENT SCHERBER A Dissertation submitted to the College of Music in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Degree Awarded: Summer Semester, 2014 Ryan V. Scherber defended this dissertation on June 18, 2014. The members of the supervisory committee were: William Fredrickson Professor Directing Dissertation Alexander Jimenez University Representative John Geringer Committee Member Patrick Dunnigan Committee Member Clifford Madsen Committee Member The Graduate School has verified and approved the above-named committee members, and certifies that the dissertation has been approved in accordance with university requirements. ii For Mary Scherber, a selfless individual to whom I owe much. iii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The completion of this journey would not have been possible without the care and support of my family, mentors, colleagues, and friends. Your support and encouragement have proven invaluable throughout this process and I feel privileged to have earned your kindness and assistance. To Dr. William Fredrickson, I extend my deepest and most sincere gratitude. You have been a remarkable inspiration during my time at FSU and I will be eternally thankful for the opportunity to have worked closely with you.
    [Show full text]
  • Hammered Dulcimer Owners Guide.Indd
    H D O’ G Dusty Strings Company Founded in 1978, Dusty Strings is a company of dedicated instrument build- ers whose love of wood, fi ne craftsmanship, and music results in some of the fi nest hammered dulcimers and harps available. We invite you to write or call us any time with questions you may have, or simply to let us know about yourself and your dulcimer. We hope your Dusty Strings hammered dulcimer will provide you with many years of musical enjoyment. © 1998 Dusty Strings 3450 16th Ave. W. • Seattle, WA 98119 • (866) 634-1656 www.dustystrings.com Contents Introduction and History .......................................................................................1 Anatomy of the Hammered Dulcimer ....................................................................2 Maintenance and Care of Your Dulcimer ............................................................... 2 String Maintenance .........................................................................................2 String Types .................................................................................................... 3 Replacing Broken Strings ................................................................................ 3 Restringing the Whole Dulcimer..................................................................... 4 String Buzzes ...................................................................................................4 Finish ..............................................................................................................5 General
    [Show full text]
  • Absolute Pitch
    EXPLORING THE SELF-CONCEPT AND IDENTITY OF SYDNEY CONSERVATORIUM STUDENTS WITH AND WITHOUT ABSOLUTE PITCH. Julie O’Connor A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Bachelor of Music (Music Education) (Honours), Sydney Conservatorium of Music, University of Sydney. 2006 ii Abstract Absolute Pitch (AP) is the ability to identify pitches without external references (Parncutt & Levitin, 2001). It is a rare ability that is more prevalent among musicians. This qualitative study explored the perceptions of Sydney Conservatorium of Music students through interviews, focusing on the value of AP possession, and implications for music self-concept. The study involved 12 Conservatorium University and High School students; six participants were self- nominated absolute pitch possessors, and the remaining six were categorised as relative pitch (RP) users. Through discussions of the value, prevalence and practicality of AP, the data suggested that AP is a highly desirable ability among Conservatorium students, and particularly valued by those who possess it. The results also suggested that RP students tend to have less positive self-concepts in aural perception and music theory, while having more positive self-concepts in other musical arenas. The majority of the AP participants had a desire to become a solo performer, and the RP participants’ tended to plan broader musical goals such as combining teaching and ensemble performance. These results suggested that the possession of AP has had a significant effect on the identity of these individuals. iii Acknowledgements First and foremost, I would like to thank my supervisor, James Renwick, whose insightful advice (and subtle pushing) inspired and motivated me throughout the study.
    [Show full text]
  • Music Theory and Ear Training Course Syllabus Course: Advanced
    Music Theory and Ear Training Course Syllabus Course: Advanced Placement Music Theory Credit: One Carnegie Unit Course Description Advanced Placement Music Theory is a third or fourth year course for advanced instrumental and vocal seniors; it is equivalent to a first year college theory course. At the end of the year students are required to take the Advanced Placement Music Theory Examination, which may earn them credit at some colleges. Students who have completed this course generally place into college sophomore theory or above. A third or fourth year of music theory is required for graduation as a vocal or instrumental music major. Prerequisites for AP Music Theory are completion of requirements for Theory III and permission of the instructor. Content Standards DCPS music content standards make up the core skills, concepts and knowledge for Advanced Placement Music Theory: 1. Perform a variety of repertoire. 2. Improvise, compose, and arrange. 3. Read and notate music. 4. Listen, analyze, and evaluate. These standards are incorporated in the course outline below. Course Outline The student will: 1. read and notate pitch in treble, bass, alto, and tenor clefs. 2. identify and write all major and minor key signatures; explain and construct a diagram of the circle of fifths. 3. write, sing,* and identify within the context of a piece of music on the page and by listening: major, three forms of minor, chromatic, whole tone, pentatonic, and blues scales, and dorian, phrygian, lydian, and mixolydian modes. 4. identify on the page and by ear, construct, and sing all intervals; write interval inversions and their enharmonic equivalents; distinguish between consonant and dissonant intervals.
    [Show full text]
  • A Computer-Assisted Program in Timbrai Ear Training - a Preliminary S Tudy
    ", A Computer-Assisted Program in TimbraI Ear Training - A Preliminary S tudy by René Quesnel Department of Music Theory McGill University, Montréal September 1990. A Thesis submitted to the Faculty of Graduate Studies and Research in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Music © René Quesnel, 1990 11 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS \. 1 would like to thank my thesis advisor, Dr Wieslaw Woszczyk, who believed in the project from the start. His insights and advice were most helpful. Iowe man y thanks to Wayne Zelmer, the technician of the McGi1l Recording Studios, for his invaluable assistance in the assembling and maintenance of the system used for this thesis. 1 also want to thank my students in the Technical Ear Training course wno patiently used early and unstabk versions of the software. Finally, 1 would like to thank the Music Faculty at McGill University for providing the equipment used for this research. J'aimerais addresser un remerciement très spécial à mon frère Roger qui, le premier, m'a donné la chance d'apprendre l'informatique, ainsi qu'à ma famille pour son support continu Et à Marie, pour m'avoir donné une raison de plus de mener à terme ce projet. ü TABLE OF CONTENTS Acknowledgements ............................................................................... Ii Abstract .................. , ......................... '" .............................................. vi Résumé ............................................................................................. VII INTRODUCfION ............................
    [Show full text]
  • Comprehensive Course Syllabus-Music Theory COURSE
    1 Comprehensive Course Syllabus-Music Theory COURSE DESCRIPTION: In Music Theory, the student will implement higher-level musical language and grammar skills including musical notation, harmonic analysis, and part writing which will lead to a thorough understanding of music composition and music theory. The student will obtain and practice ear training skills and skills required for sight reading musical literature. They will recognize the development of music from an historical and cultural perspective and extend musical awareness beyond music currently familiar to the student. INSTRUCTOR: Emily Sites D105 OFFICE HOURS: A-D days: 9-10am Also available by Appointment Contact Information: OFFICE PHONE: 630-907-5915 E-MAIL: [email protected] MEETING TIMES: A, C, D days (4): 11-11:55am MEETING ROOM: D110 TEXTS/MATERIALS: Benward and Saker Music in Theory and Practice 8th Ed. Vol. 1 (Textbook and Workbook) Tablet/Laptop Blank staff paper Pencil Occasionally students will need to bring a pair of headphones to class (some pairs may be provided) Students will periodically need to spend time outside of class using a piano keyboard—these are available in the music department for student use. All additional materials will be provided throughout the semester 2 ESSENTIAL CONTENT: The essential content in this course will include aspects of each of the six Fine Arts Learning Standards. See IMSA Core Curriculum Template for models. 1. Experience different kinds of art or music A. Accurately recognize the principles and practices of art or musical composition. B. Examine, evaluate and analyze sounds, images, and ideas. C. Use a variety of senses to experience art or music.
    [Show full text]
  • Selective Hearing – Ear Training in Academia Bronwyn Schuman MU485 Faith, Music, and Society Dr. Don Quantz April 8, 2017
    Selective Hearing – Ear Training in Academia Bronwyn Schuman MU485 Faith, Music, and Society Dr. Don Quantz April 8, 2017 Schuman 1 When people train, they work to get better at something in order to achieve a goal within a specific system. Within the academic world, listening is taught through means of an ear “training” class. Ear training class does not necessarily mean that students will be listening, but rather students will be training to hear correctly within a system. In these classes across universities, students learn to identify several things within tonal and even non-tonal theoretical contexts. In both contexts, students come to class to learn how to identify pitches in relation to one another. While there are other components to ear training that do not focus on pitch relations, such as rhythmic dictation, the emphasis is put on harmonies and pitches in a relative framework. The pitch relation system for ear training in academia has developed because most students (and people, for that matter) hear in relative terms. Their knowledge of pitches and sound are based off of other pitches that they hear within that same musical context. In Musicophilia, Oliver Sacks quotes Diana Deutsch discussing relative pitch, saying: “Take color naming as an analogy. Suppose you showed someone a red object and asked him to name the color. And suppose he answered ‘I can recognize the color, and I can discriminate it from other colors, but I just can’t name it.’ Then you juxtaposed a blue object and named its color, and he responded, ‘OK, since the second color is blue, the first one must be red.’ … This is precisely how most people name pitches—they evaluate the relationship between the pitch to be named and another pitch whose name they already know.”1 1 Oliver W.
    [Show full text]
  • EAR TRAINING SUGGESTIONS for USING the CHROMATIC PITCH PIPE by Jamey Aebersold
    EAR TRAINING SUGGESTIONS FOR USING THE CHROMATIC PITCH PIPE By Jamey Aebersold Jamey Aebersold Jazz® offers The “Pocket Tones” Electronic Pitch Pipe. The following information will help you get the most out of it. .................................................................................................................................................................................. EXERCISES The following are just a few of the many exercises available. As you try some of these, I’m sure others will come to mind. 1. Play any tone. With that tone in mind, make it the root/tonic/first note of any scale. Sing the scale up, then down, then check to see that you ended on the note you began. You can do this with triads (major, minor, diminished, augmented); the chromatic scale; short melodies (such as Twinkle, Twinkle Little Star, Happy Birthday, or Ole Man River); the first three to give notes of any scale; or just sing an interval such as a perfect 4th (the first two notes of Here Comes The Bride). Constantly check your pitch with the pitch pipe to see if you are straying from the original starting tone. I find it best to sing out loud so I can hear myself. If you seem to be having trouble, slow the exercise down or try fewer notes. 2. Pick out a note from anything you hear. It cold be from the radio, CDs, TV, cassettes or even just a tone from your mind. After thinking about it for a few seconds, blow any tone on the pitch pipe. If it doesn’t match the original tone, try to figure out how far away it is and label (name) the distance (the interval) in your mind.
    [Show full text]
  • Fund Ear Train Pract ANSWER KEY.Mus
    Fundamentals Diagnostic: Ear Training Portion (practice version) ANSWER KEY Part I: A scale will be played for you (ascending and descending). Identify the scale type, assuming the following options: major, natural minor, harmonic minor, melodic minor; each scale will be played ONE time. # b œ#œ œ œ œ ## # œ œ œ œ œ œ œ & œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ 1. ____harmonic minor____ 2. _______Major______ œ b œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ # #œ#œ œ nœ nœ & b œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ œ 3. _____natural minor____ 4. _____melodic minor__ (10 pts.) Part II: You will hear a series of melodic fragments, all in the key of D major. Each melodic fragment consists of three pitches. For each fragment, notate the three pitches on the staff, using whole notes, as shown in the sample below. Each fragment will be played TWO times. D major scale will be played at the beginning of the exercise, and then again after the fifth fragment. 1 2 3 4 5 ## w w w w & w w w w w w w w w w w (7.5 pts.) 6 7 8 9 10 # # w w w w w w w w & w w w w w w w (15 pts.) Part III: Two brief melodic phrases will be played for you. You should notate the pitches on the staff provided below. The key and the first pitch for each melody is provided for you, and the rhythm is notated above the staff.
    [Show full text]
  • Links Between Musicality and Vocal Emotion Perception
    Musicality and Vocal Emotion Perception 1 Links between Musicality and Vocal Emotion Perception Christine Nussbaum and Stefan R. Schweinberger Department for General Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience, Friedrich Schiller University Jena, Germany Correspondence should be addressed to Christine Nussbaum, Department for General Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience, Friedrich Schiller University Jena, Leutragraben 1, 07743 Jena, Germany. Tel: +49 (0) 3641 945939, E-Mail: [email protected]; or to Stefan R. Schweinberger, Department for General Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience, Friedrich Schiller University Jena, Am Steiger 3/Haus 1, 07743 Jena, Germany. Tel: +49 (0) 3641 945181, E-Mail: [email protected]. Note: This manuscript has not yet been peer-reviewed. The paper has been submitted on September 24, 2020, to Emotion Review, and is currently being under review there. Musicality and Vocal Emotion Perception 2 Abstract Links between musicality and vocal emotion perception skills have only recently emerged as a focus of study. Here we review current evidence for or against such links. Based on a systematic literature search, we identified 33 studies that addressed either (1) vocal emotion perception in musicians and non-musicians, (2) effects of musical training intervention on vocal emotion perception, (3) potentials of musical training for cochlear implant users, (4) vocal emotion perception in individuals with congenital amusia, or (5) the role of individual differences (e.g., musical interests, psychoacoustic abilities). Overall, the evidence supports a link between musicality and vocal emotion perception abilities. We discuss potential underlying factors, including origins of musicality in nature and nurture. Keywords Vocal emotion perception, musicality, musical training, amusia Musicality and Vocal Emotion Perception 3 Introduction and Review of Related Literature Human social communication depends on the exchange and mutual representation of multiple social signals.
    [Show full text]
  • The Effects of Computerized and Traditional Ear Training Programs
    Running head: COMPUTERIZED AND TRADITIONAL EAR TRAINING The Effects of Computerized and Tradi tional Ear Training Programs on Aural Skills of Elementary Students Patrick N. Kariuki And Zachary R. Ross Milligan College A Paper Presented at the Annual Conference of the Mid - South Educational Research Association, Starkville, Mississippi, November 8 - 10, 2017 COMPUTERIZED AND TRADITIONAL EAR TRAINING 2 Abstract The purpose of this study was to investigate the effects of computerized and traditional ear t raining methods on the aural skills abilities of elementary music students. The sample consisted of 20 students who were randomly assigned to either an experimental or control group. The experimental group was taught for five sessions using computerized ea r training program while the control group was taught for five sessions using traditional, non - computerized ear training methods. At the end of the five sessions, students were tested. Data were collected by administering a test to both experimental and co ntrol groups that measured students’ ability to identify by ear eleven different pitch intervals and three different qualities of chords. Students were also administered a survey to measure their attitudes toward their experience in the ear training progra m. Data were analyzed using independent t - tests. The results indicated a significant difference between the test scores of the control and experimental groups. There was no significant difference found between the experimental and control groups regarding their attitude survey
    [Show full text]