Gestalt Experiments and Inductive Observations

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Gestalt Experiments and Inductive Observations Ingo Brigandt Gestalt Experiments and Inductive Observations Konrad Lorenz’s early Epistemological Writings and the Methods of Classical Ethology thology brought Abstract scientific knowledge. Ge- E some crucial insights stalt perception and in- and perspectives to the During the 1940s Konrad LORENZ formulated his ear- duction are the main ele- study of behavior, in par- ly epistemological views, focusing on the cognitive ments of LORENZ’s ticular the idea that be- mechanisms induction and Gestalt perception. After epistemology. These ideas havior can be studied the war he used this philosophical framework to de- were originally formu- within a comparative– fend the approach of classical ethology against other lated during the second evolutionary framework approaches to animal behavior. The present paper ex- world war (see for in- by means of homologiz- amines the relationship between LORENZ’s ethological stance the Russian Manu- ing components of be- methodology and his philosophy of science and knowl- script, LORENZ 1948). In havioral patterns and by edge. The main aim of LORENZ’s post-war epistemo- the post-war period causal analysis of behav- logical writings is to provide an epistemological and LORENZ emphasized his ior components and their cognitive theory of observation in order to defend the epistemological ideas as integration. Early ethol- observational approach of classical ethology against part of an attempt to jus- ogy is well-known for its the view that it was no rigorous science. tify his ethological ap- extensive use of qualita- proach and his views tive observations of ani- Key words about the role of observa- mals under their natural tion and experiment. The Konrad LORENZ, Gestalt perception, observation, conditions. These obser- aim of the present paper ethology. vations are combined is to call attention to with experiments that try LORENZ’s philosophical to analyze behavioral patterns and establish specific account and how it integrates with his biological claims about animal behavior. Nowadays, there is methodology. still disagreement about the significance of observa- tion and experiments and their relation. ALLEN Observation and Experiment (forthcoming) points out that in debates about the interpretation of animal cognition the interpreta- LORENZ’s account of the general character of obser- tions of those ethologists who have actually spent vation and experiment remains unchanged time watching the animals are favored by some biol- throughout his writings. The specific relationship ogists. In fact, some practitioners of cognitive ethol- between these two aspects is exhibited by LORENZ’s ogy feel that experiments at best just confirm what early biological work as well as his later explicit one already knows, while others are more skeptical methodological account of them (LORENZ 1935; about interpretations of observations without ex- LORENZ/TINBERGEN 1938; LORENZ 1948, 1981). periments. LORENZ emphasizes four distinctive features of clas- As a major representative of ethology Konrad sical observational practice: observations are to be LORENZ not only shaped its approach and made the carried out in a hypothesis-free way; the approach methods and theories of ethology known to the bi- consists in doing qualitative observations rather ological community and the interested public, he than quantitative measurements; animals are to be also embedded his defense of the ethological and observed in their natural environment; and obser- comparative approach in a philosophical theory of vations include all features of the organisms and Evolution and Cognition ❘ 157 ❘ 2003, Vol. 9, No. 2 Ingo Brigandt their environment and rely on a large observational tative measurements and recording them, observa- basis. One should not think that ethology (in partic- tions are recorded using written descriptions (1935, ular the more developed ethology of the post-war pp112ff), drawings and photographs (LORENZ/TIN- period) always proceeded in this fashion. Instead, BERGEN 1938). This qualitative approach of classical LORENZ’s bold claims about how ethology is done ethology has to be defended against an understand- are based on some of his opinions as to how science ing that only allows for quantitative measurement should be done or how a science should develop and statistical evaluation as a means of scientific ob- (see BURKHARDT 1981). In any case, the way classical jectivity (1958, pp246, 256; 1959, p281; 1963a, p1; ethology was done was noticeably different from 1981, pp40ff, 68ff). Despite LORENZ’s emphasis on other contemporary approaches to animal behavior qualitative observation, post-war ethology did very such as comparative psychology or later on sociobi- well make use of quantitative measurements, but ology. In fact, LORENZ encountered methodological connected them with qualitative observations critique from both groups. (SCHLEIDT/SCHLEIDT 1958 is an example). LORENZ defends the ethological approach of gath- A further feature of the ethological way of observ- ering observational data without having a hypothe- ing is to systematically study animals in their natu- sis in mind against the view that scientific accounts ral ecological environment. Animals are kept “in an must be based on a theory (1948, p216; unless oth- environment as close as possible to their natural erwise indicated, references refer to a publication of habitats, for the purpose of general biological and LORENZ). LORENZ thinks that it is not only possible to specifically ethological observations” (1935, pp108– make observations devoid of a theoretical frame- 109). Keeping animals under laboratory conditions work, he also points to the possibility of confirma- suffers from the drawback that the organisms do not tion bias as a disadvantage of basing one’s observa- exhibit their natural adaptive behavior patterns and tional approach on a hypothesis (1948, pp31, 71). thus functional and artifactual behavior are likely to Instead of forming theoretical considerations at an be confused. This approach of observing animals early stage of inquiry, scientific inquiry has to start under natural conditions continues the “amateur- with “observation pure and simple” (1958, p246). ism” of the forerunners of classical ethology (1981, LORENZ calls this “presuppositionless observation” p47). However, for LORENZ the optimal method is (1981, p47) or often “unbiased observation” (1958, not to study animals in the field. For in this case, it is p250; see also 1948, p213; 1950 p131; 1959, p281). difficult and extremely time-consuming to be able Now, even amateur naturalists who dedicated all to observe the relevant behavior patterns (1948, their time to field observations of animals did not p222). The chapter “Animal Keeping as a Research necessarily refrain from interpreting their observa- Method” of the Russian Manuscript gives a detailed tions and relate them to biological theories. Ed- account of how the study of animal behavior has to mund Selous, for instance, used his extensive obser- be organized (1948, pp221ff). Several animals of a vations to justify theories of sexual selection. In the species have to be kept so that they live in proximity case of LORENZ, the theoretical assumption that be- to the researchers and in a controllable environ- havioral characters can be used to characterize taxo- ment. Apart from conducting observations in that nomic groups and reconstruct phylogenies guided way, animal keeping makes it possible to conduct his observational practice from very early on. But experiments under otherwise natural conditions LORENZ is right insofar as before 1935 he and TINBER- (1948, p222). LORENZ’s practice of raising and keep- GEN completely lacked an interpretative framework, ing animals was quite similar to the practice of his unlike most people working in animal psychology. mentor Oskar HEINROTH or the American zoologist After the war ethology became more theoretical, but Charles Otis WHITMAN. WHITMAN’s and LORENZ’s compared to other approaches such as comparative practice of animal keeping enabled them to simulta- psychology or sociobiology ethology was not that neously observe the behavior of several closely re- much an enterprise driven by explicitly formulated lated species, which is hard to achieve in the field. theories and hypothesis that were put to empirical But LORENZ’s approach differed from the early field test. In particular sociobiology took quantitative hy- naturalist in the British tradition (Selous and potheses based on models from population genetics Howard) or in the Dutch tradition (RÖELL 2000). In as the starting point and used observations prima- particular Niko TINBERGEN heavily relied on field ob- rily to test these hypotheses. servations (and experiments) instead of animal In addition, early ethology relied on qualitative keeping (TINBERGEN 1932, 1935, 1951), while observations of behavior. Instead of making quanti- LORENZ viewed field work as a control for observa- Evolution and Cognition ❘ 158 ❘ 2003, Vol. 9, No. 2 Gestalt Experiments and Inductive Observations tions gained from animals kept in semi-natural con- Charles Otis WHITMAN, for instance, kept detailed ditions (1981, pp47ff). Thus TINBERGEN represented notes of his extensive observations, and carried out the ecological dimension of ethology more fully observations for years before he published his re- than LORENZ did.1 Despite the fact that LORENZ was sults (LILLIE 1911). Like LORENZ, he emphasized that not a field naturalist, his approach was quite dis- a detailed and extensive knowledge
Recommended publications
  • The Christmas Story of Brunswikians Lubomír Kostroň in Our
    The Christmas Story of Brunswikians Lubomír Kostroň In our country, Czechia (Bohemia) and Moravia, people bake an exclusive longish kind of Christmas cake, which is called ―vánočka‖. Vánočka is composed of three layers of dough braids: six of them are entangled together as the bottom flat layer, four are put on them, as the middle layer and then the last two on the very top. The raisins, almonds and nuts are included, of course. Since I began to collect the information, which the following story is composed of, purposefully at the Christmas time of 2017, this metaphor for the story seems to be appropriate: the individual destinies are entangled together for some time, then the next generation of folks comes in, adds to the story and in some sense continues … and so on. This way even the ideas are shared, connecting the individuals, for some time… This is the substance of this story. People, exciting ideas, work …. In Brunswik´s terminology, the focus is on wide ―distal to distal relationships‖. Therefore, the story will be composed of individual memories and impressions of those who participated – or at least of those – as far as I know – who played some significant role in the story. Now, we have to begin with the oldest layer of our story, which is on the top of the cake . As it is usual: once upon the time …. The history of Egon Brunwik´s stretches long way back. The origins of the Austrian Hungarian family of von Brunswick or de Brunswik are uncertain, though they point to the city of Braunschweig in Germany.
    [Show full text]
  • Friedrich Stadler, Leslie Topp, Chair: William Johnston
    1 Workshop VIENNA 1900: CURRENT DISCOURSES ON FIN-DE-SIECLE VIENNA International Center, University of New Orleans (UNO), October 24-25, 2016 SLIDE 1 Roundtable IV: Interdisciplinary Models Friedrich Stadler, Leslie Topp, Chair: William Johnston Friedrich Stadler (University of Vienna): “The Sciences and Humanities as Culture” SLIDE 2 INTRODUCTION Based on William Johnston’s path breaking trilogy of books on Austrian-Hungarian intellectual history I will focus mainly on the role of philosophy, the sciences and humanities from a trans- and interdisciplinary point of view. (Of course, the publications of Carl Schorske, Allan Janik/Stephen Toulmin, Edward Timms, David Luft, and Steven Beller and many others are to be mentioned as essential background knowledge). SLIDES 3-4: Constructive Unrest. Austrian Conference on Contemporary History Graz 2016 According to the new model of the “Long 20th Century” in Austrian history (from Habsburg Monarchy to the Republic) in general, and as applied to the history of the University of Vienna, specifically, I make a plea for this conception more or less also regarding Vienna 1900 / Fin-de-Siécle Vienna. This can be illustrated by a short report on a panel dealing with the “Paradigmenwechsel zum langen 20. Jahrhundert” (paradigm shift on the long 20th century) at the last “Österreichischer Zeitgeschichtetag” (ÖZT) in Graz, June 2016. SLIDE 5: “Wissenschaft als Kultur” (Frankfurt 1995) Following this perspective, I will argue for the need to cover all sciences (including humanities) under the umbrella of (Austro-Hungarian) culture, which seems to me the main deficit in the related historiography. Instead, the image of all sciences as an essential part of culture (Wissenschaft als Kultur) is leading up to transgressing disciplinary boundaries.
    [Show full text]
  • The Place of Egon Brunswik in the History of Psychology*
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by University of Richmond University of Richmond UR Scholarship Repository Psychology Faculty Publications Psychology 1987 From Act Psychology to Problematic Functionalism: The lP ace of Egon Brunswik in the History of Psychology David E. Leary University of Richmond, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: http://scholarship.richmond.edu/psychology-faculty- publications Part of the Theory and Philosophy Commons Recommended Citation Leary, David E. "From Act Psychology to Problematic Functionalism: The lP ace of Egon Brunswik in the History of Psychology." Psychology in Twentieth-Century Thought and Society. Ed. Mitchell G. Ash and William Ray Woodward. Cambridge: Cambridge UP, 1987. 115-42. Print. This Book Chapter is brought to you for free and open access by the Psychology at UR Scholarship Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Psychology Faculty Publications by an authorized administrator of UR Scholarship Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. 5 FROM ACT PSYCHOLOGY TO PROBABILISTIC FUNCTIONALISM: THE PLACE OF EGON BRUNSWIK IN THE HISTORY OF PSYCHOLOGY* DAVIDE. LEARY In the coming years, Egon Brunswik will hold an ever increasingly significant and important position in the history of psychology. (from Edward C. Tolman's eulogy, 1956) Despite Tolman's prediction, Egon Brunswik's place in the history of psy­ chology has yet to be firmly established. Although his name and his con­ cepts are frequently invoked, they are rarely used in defense of positions that he would have recognized as his own. And although most contem­ porary psychologists have failed to comprehend either the details or the underlying rationale of his psychological theory, historians of psychology have done even less to clarify the context, development, and import of his life and work.' This neglect is unfortunate.
    [Show full text]
  • History of the University of California Psychology Department at Berkeley
    A HISTORY OF THE UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA PSYCHOLOGY DEPARTMENT AT BERKELEY Joan D. Grold, Berkeley, 1961 Introduction In the process of tracing the development of the Psychology Department at Berkeley, my original desire for a good grade has gradually receded to the background, and a number of other reasons have arisen to take its place. Perhaps this has occurred because^ of an increasing realization on my part of the importance of a history such as this —an importance which goes even deeper than a simple desire to know the past of one's institution of learning. Its significance lies rather in the fact that it is difficult for both students and faculty to appreciate the Department's present status unless they can see the process of growth which has culminated in its accomplishments today. For the faculty a history seen through a student's eyes (plus the observant eyes of Jean Macfarlanei) should be of interest if only for the fact that it may point up the problems that growth has produced in the widening of a student- facuity bond, and the need that the students, both graduates and under­ graduates, have for a close identification with their faculty. On the other hand, for the students a record of the Department's history should make it easier to understand situations that exist now, whether they be current research and trends or ideological controversies. Unless one can appreciate the facts of inevitable growth and development, it is difficult to have as much pride in the current accomplishments of the Department as is desired.
    [Show full text]
  • Teaching Guide for the History of Psychology Instructor Compiled by Jennifer Bazar, Elissa Rodkey, and Jacy Young
    A Teaching Guide for History of Psychology www.feministvoices.com 1 Psychology’s Feminist Voices in the Classroom A Teaching Guide for the History of Psychology Instructor Compiled by Jennifer Bazar, Elissa Rodkey, and Jacy Young One of the goals of Psychology’s Feminist Voices is to serve as a teaching resource. To facilitate the process of incorporating PFV into the History of Psychology course, we have created this document. Below you will find two primary sections: (1) Lectures and (2) Assignments. (1) Lectures: In this section you will find subject headings of topics often covered in History of Psychology courses. Under each heading, we have provided an example of the relevant career, research, and/or life experiences of a woman featured on the Psychology’s Feminist Voices site that would augment a lecture on that particular topic. Below this description is a list of additional women whose histories would be well-suited to lectures on that topic. (2) Assignments: In this section you will find several suggestions for assignments that draw on the material and content available on Psychology’s Feminist Voices that you can use in your History of Psychology course. The material in this guide is intended only as a suggestion and should not be read as a “complete” list of all the ways Psychology’s Feminist Voices could be used in your courses. We would love to hear all of the different ideas you think of for how to include the site in your classroom - please share you thoughts with us by emailing Alexandra Rutherford, our project coordinator,
    [Show full text]
  • Egon Brunswik (1903-1955) David E
    University of Richmond UR Scholarship Repository Psychology Faculty Publications Psychology 2000 Egon Brunswik (1903-1955) David E. Leary University of Richmond, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: http://scholarship.richmond.edu/psychology-faculty- publications Part of the Other Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Leary, David E. "Egon Brunswik." In Encyclopedia of Pscyhology, edited by Alan E. Kazdin, 480-481. Vol. 1. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 2000. This Book Chapter is brought to you for free and open access by the Psychology at UR Scholarship Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Psychology Faculty Publications by an authorized administrator of UR Scholarship Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. 480 BRUNSWIK, EGON BRUNSWIK, EGON (1903-1955), Austrian and Amer­ ican psychologist. Brunswik stood at the nexus of sev­ eral philosophical and psychological traditions, created his own distinctive psychology, and died without fore­ seeing the influence of his· concepts and methods. Yet more than forty years after his death by suicide, certain of his ideas and techniques are still being explored and used. Brunswik was born in Budapest, Hungary, on 18 March 1903. In 1923, he undertook the study of psy­ chology under Karl Buhler at the University of Vienna. In the same period, he participated in a discussion group (led by Moritz Schlick) that gave birth to logical positivism. He was also aware of contemporary devel­ opments in psychoanalysis, especially through his fu­ ture wife, Else Frenkel-Brunswik. After earning his Ph.D. in 1927, Brunswik remained in Biihler's Psycho­ logical Institute in order to continue his research in perception.
    [Show full text]
  • The Hawk/Goose Story: the Classical Ethological Experiments of Lorenz and Tinbergen, Revisited
    Journal of Comparative Psychology © 2011 American Psychological Association 2011, Vol. 125, No. 2, 121–133 0735-7036/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0022068 The Hawk/Goose Story: The Classical Ethological Experiments of Lorenz and Tinbergen, Revisited Wolfgang Schleidt Michael D. Shalter University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria Independent Practice, Castres, France Humberto Moura-Neto Independent Practice, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil We present a historical account of the story behind the famous hawk/goose experiments of Lorenz and Tinbergen in a wider context of cognitive ethology. We discuss their significance, for ethological experimentation in general, and specifically for understanding innate constraints on cognition. As examples of the continuing significance of the hawk/goose paradigm of selective habituation, we discuss its relation to “exposure therapy” of human phobias and the use of hawk silhouettes as deterrents for songbirds. Finally we rephrase Uexku¨ll’s thesis of taxon-specific worlds (“Umwelten”) as a “Theory of World.” Keywords: selective habituation, hawk/goose experiments, cognitive ethology, theory of world, phobias, predator-prey Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0022068.supp Based largely on the involvement of the senior author of this scientists, who were already well set on their individual courses. For paper in his 1961 attempt to replicate the famous 1937 hawk/goose Lorenz and Tinbergen, however, it proved pivotal . The sympo- experiments (Figure 1) of Lorenz and Tinbergen (Lorenz, 1939; sium’s significance was that it provided the occasion for Lorenz and Tinbergen, 1939; Schleidt, 1961a, 1961b, English translation, Tinbergen to meet one another. Each of them had the chance to begin Schleidt, 1974) we present a historical account of the story behind to recognize how well the other’s strength complemented his own.
    [Show full text]
  • The Christmas Story of Brunswikians Lubomír Kostroň in Our
    The Christmas Story of Brunswikians Lubomír Kostroň In our country, Czechia (Bohemia) and Moravia, people bake an exclusive longish kind of Christmas cake, which is called “vánočka”. Vánočka is composed of three layers of dough braids: six of them are entangled together as the bottom flat layer, four are put on them, as the middle layer and then the last two on the very top. The raisins, almonds and nuts are included, of course. Since I began to collect the information, which the following story is composed of, purposefully at the Christmas time of 2017, this metaphor for the story seems to be appropriate: the individual destinies are entangled together for some time, then the next generation of folks comes in, adds to the story and in some sense continues … and so on. This way even the ideas are shared, connecting the individuals, for some time… This is the substance of this story. People, exciting ideas, work …. In Brunswik´s terminology, the focus is on wide “distal to distal relationships”. Therefore, the story will be composed of individual memories and impressions of those who participated – or at least of those – as far as I know – who played some significant role in the story. Now, we have to begin with the oldest layer of our story, which is on the top of the cake . As it is usual: once upon the time …. The history of Egon Brunwik´s family stretches long way back. The origins of the Austrian Hungarian family of von Brunswick or de Brunswik are uncertain, though they point to the city of Braunschweig in Germany.
    [Show full text]
  • Five Theories of Voting Action Strategy and Structure of Psychological Explanation
    Five theories of voting action strategy and structure of psychological explanation M. Visser Contents Preface 3 1 The study of voting behavior 5 1.1 The behavioral persuasion 7 1.2 Psychology and behavior 9 1.3 Voting behavior and action 13 1.4 Strategies of explanation 14 1.5 Research questions and outline 16 2 The empirical analysis of voting action 19 2.1 Introduction 21 2.2 The Würzburg school 21 2.3 Lazarsfeld and the empirical analysis of action 23 2.4 The Columbia approach to voting action 26 2.5 Discussion and conclusions 29 3 The field theory of voting action 31 3.1 Introduction 33 3.2 The Gestalt school 33 3.3 Lewin's Gestalt theory of motivation 36 3.4 The Michigan approach to voting action 39 3.5 From Michigan model to `mainstream' research 41 3.6 Dicussion and conclusions 43 4 The cognitive theory of voting action 45 4.1 Introduction 47 4.2 Theories of balance and dissonance 47 4.3 The New Look 50 4.4 The rise of cognitive science 52 4.5 The cognitive approach to voting action 54 4.6 Discussion and conclusions 58 5 The psychodynamic theory of voting action 61 5.1 Introduction 63 5.2 Freud's theory of unconscious motivation 63 5.3 Analytical social psychology and authoritarianism 65 5.4 The `authoritarian personality' 67 5.5 Authoritarianism and voting action 69 5.6 Discussion and conclusions 72 6 The humanistic theory of voting action 75 6.1 Introduction 77 6.2 The Vienna school and humanistic psychology 77 6.3 Goldstein's Gestalt theory of personality 79 6.4 The hierarchy of needs 81 6.5 Needs, values and voting action 84
    [Show full text]
  • Whatever Happened to Karl Buhler?
    Whatever Happened to Karl Buhler? ADRIAN BROCK example of Karl Buhler (1879-1963). One York University suspects that Buhler was chosen because the example is such a striking one. If Buhler is mentioned at all in texts on the history of psychology, it is in connection with his early Abstract work as Oswald Kiilpc's assistant at the Uni- The work of Karl Buhler is not particularly versity of Wiirzburg (e.g. Hothersall, 1990; well known in English-speaking countries. If Brcnnan, 1991; Hcrgcnhahn, 1992; Leahcy, Buhler is mentioned at all in texts on the his- 1992a; Schultz & Schultz, 1992). It was tory of psychology, it is in connection with his Buhler's work on 'imageless thought' which early work at the University of Wiirzburg. This sparked off the famous controversy with work represents only the start of Buhler's Wilhelm Wundt over the methods of the career in psychology. He subsequently pro- 'Wiirzburg School'. Boring (1950) wrote: duced important work on perception, Geslall theory, developmental psychology, theoretical Buhler was severely criticized by Wundt, by psychology and the psychology of language. Durr, who was one of his observers, and by Buhler's work has had a profound — though von Aster. Titchcncr, of course, criticized the largely unnoticed — influence on later entile movement. Buhler, however, left the thinkers. It has also been the subject of a total picture of thought without important revival of interest in Europe in recent years. change, and we may thus close our account of This article provides an overview of Buhler's the Wurzburg school and return once again life and work.
    [Show full text]
  • Egon Brunswik's Research Perspectives in Relationship to Educational Knowledge Development : Historical and Metamethodological Considerations
    University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Doctoral Dissertations 1896 - February 2014 1-1-1975 Egon Brunswik's research perspectives in relationship to educational knowledge development : historical and metamethodological considerations. Candido Genovart-Rosello University of Massachusetts Amherst Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umass.edu/dissertations_1 Recommended Citation Genovart-Rosello, Candido, "Egon Brunswik's research perspectives in relationship to educational knowledge development : historical and metamethodological considerations." (1975). Doctoral Dissertations 1896 - February 2014. 2946. https://scholarworks.umass.edu/dissertations_1/2946 This Open Access Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. It has been accepted for inclusion in Doctoral Dissertations 1896 - February 2014 by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. For more information, please contact [email protected]. EGON BRUNSWIK'S RESEARCH PERSPECTIVES IN RELATIONSHIP TO EDUCATIONAL KNOWLEDGE DEVELOPMENT: HISTORICAL AND METAMETHODOLOGICAL CONSIDERATIONS A Dissertation Presented by CANDIDO GENOVART-ROSELLO Submitted to the Graduate School of the University of Massachusetts in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF EDUCATION September 1975 Major Subject: Educational Psychology j EGON BRUNSWIK'S RESEARCH PERSPECTIVES IN RELATIONSHIP TO EDUCATIONAL KNOWLEDGE DEVELOPMENT: HISTORICAL AND METAMETHODOLOGICAL CONSIDERATIONS A Dissertation By CANDIDO GENOVART-ROSELLO Approved as to style and content by: / •/ fy/> Vtl lUcJ K/ U' X David G. Coffing, .Chairman of Committee x / / x/ ^7 X/; x. -7?::* t! ,X;vt /Juan P. Caban, Member /rx f? f'An£- Alan J. Gross, Member Louis Fischer, Acting Dean School of Education September 1975 This Dissertation has been possible, thanks to: Dr. David G. Coffing Dr.
    [Show full text]