New Conceptual Associative Learning in Amnesia: a Case Study

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

New Conceptual Associative Learning in Amnesia: a Case Study Journal of Memory and Language 43, 291–315 (2000) doi:10.1006/jmla.2000.2733, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on New Conceptual Associative Learning in Amnesia: A Case Study Suparna Rajaram State University of New York at Stony Brook and H. Branch Coslett Temple University School of Medicine We report two experiments in this article that were designed to investigate the role of retrieval constraints and interference in implicit learning of new verbal associations in a densely amnesic participant, C.V., who had presumably sustained medial temporal lobe damage secondary to an anoxic episode. In Experiment 1, repeatedly studied novel sentences produced significant priming with SentenceϩFragment retrieval cues that provided maximal perceptual support as well as per- ceptual priming for the single-word targets. However, little learning was observed when no percep- tual cues were provided for the target itself with the Sentenceϩ??? retrieval cues. In Experiment 2, the effects of intraexperimental interference were measured by examining new verbal learning under the Study-Only, Study-Immediate test, Test-Study training conditions. Unlike in the findings reported in prior studies, C.V. showed little learning with the Sentenceϩ??? retrieval cues even under the minimal interference, Study-Only, condition. Together, these results demonstrate that implicit access to novel verbal associations at a level more abstract than their perceptual configurations is not ubiquitously observed in dense amnesia even when the learning conditions are optimized. These results provide a window into the processes that mediate implicit learning of novel verbal associations when the explicit memory contribution is minimized. © 2000 Academic Press In recent years, empirical studies involving examine the role of these variables on the ac- memory-intact as well as memory-impaired (or quisition of new information in a densely am- amnesic) participants have informed theories of nesic individual. The purpose of our investiga- memory functions and processes. Investigations tion is to isolate the role of implicit processes in involving memory-intact participants have the acquisition and retrieval of new verbal in- demonstrated that repetition, interference, and formation. retrieval cues play a large role in modulating The amnesic syndrome is characterized by memory performance. In the present article, we the selective loss of memory for events subse- quent to the onset of amnesia (also known as We are grateful to C.V. and his family for their enthusi- astic and sustained participation in this project. The assis- anterograde amnesia) while other cognitive tance of Kimberly Feldman and Yoko Yahata in stimulus functions such as language, perception, atten- preparation, data collection, and scoring is gratefully ac- tion, reasoning, problem solving, intelligence, knowledged. Special thanks are due to Teresa Blaxton and and short-term memory are relatively intact. A Endel Tulving for sharing their stimuli. This article greatly loss of memory for events immediately preced- benefited from the constructive comments provided by Douglas Nelson, Endel Tulving, and three anonymous re- ing the onset of amnesia, or retrograde amnesia, viewers. Research reported in this manuscript and the prep- is also commonly reported. aration of this manuscript was supported by an NIH grant, The classic studies by Warrington and R29MH57345-01, to Suparna Rajaram and grant Weiskrantz (1968, 1970) demonstrated that de- RO1AG08870 and RO1DC02754 to H. Branch Coslett. spite grossly impaired explicit memory func- Correspondence may be addressed to Suparna Rajaram, Department of Psychology, State University of New York at tions, as those involved in recall and recogni- Stony Brook, Stony Brook, NY 11794-2500. E-mail: tion, implicit memory functions, as those [email protected]. involved in word fragment completion, may be 291 0749-596X/00 $35.00 Copyright © 2000 by Academic Press All rights of reproduction in any form reserved. 292 RAJARAM AND COSLETT largely intact in amnesia (Diamond & Rozin, Vaidya et al., 1995; Warrington & Weiskrantz, 1984; Graf & Schacter, 1985). In the word 1982; Winocur & Weiskrantz, 1976). Yet, oth- fragment completion task, participants are ers have reported impairment on conceptual shown a list of words at study (e.g., elephant) priming in amnesia on a similar group of tasks and are later given fragmented versions of the (Blaxton, 1992; Cermak et al., 1998; Keane et studied(_l_p_an_;solution: elephant) and al., 1997). The reasons for these discrepancies nonstudied words, and they are asked to com- are not clear, but one possibility may be the plete the fragments with the first word that differences in procedure and different groups of comes to mind. The advantage in completing amnesic participants employed across these studied fragments over nonstudied fragments studies. constitutes the measure of implicit memory, or The database on the perceptual and concep- priming (Tulving et al., 1982). Such implicit tual priming effects in amnesia has informed memory tasks are largely data-driven such that and refined our theories of intact memory func- changes in modality across study and test re- tions by providing a window into the operation duce priming (Blaxton, 1989; Rajaram & Roe- of implicit memory when the contribution of diger, 1993; Srinivas & Roediger, 1990), but explicit memory is minimized (Blaxton, 1995; meaningful encoding does not necessarily in- Cohen & Squire, 1980; Gabrieli, 1995; Mosco- crease priming relative to surface-level analysis vitch, 1992, 1994a; Roediger et al., in press; of study material (see review by Roediger & Schacter, 1990; Tulving & Schacter, 1990). The McDermott, 1995). studies reviewed thus far reveal both the pro- Priming is also observed in implicit memory cessing and neural requirements for the reacti- tasks when the task relies on conceptual, and vation of learned information under different not perceptual, processes. For example, in a retrieval conditions. Specifically, the percep- conceptual implicit task such as category pro- tual, lexical, and conceptual representations of duction, participants study exemplar names words such as “elephant” exist in the cognitive (e.g., elephant) of various categories. At test, system prior to the experimental context. only the category names are provided (e.g., an- A key question that has concerned research- imals), and participants are asked to write down ers in recent years is the role of implicit memory the first few exemplars of that category that in mediating the learning of new verbal infor- come to mind. This task is considered to be mation. It is obvious that explicit memory or conceptual in nature because the exemplars and conscious awareness plays a vital role in the category names are conceptually related, and no acquisition of new information, as evidenced by perceptual overlap exists between study and test the performance of memory-intact individuals stimuli. Predictably, modality changes do not in the laboratory and in the real world. Whether impair such priming whereas manipulation of implicit memory also supports the acquisition meaningful encoding enhances such priming of new verbal information, and if it does, what (e.g., Blaxton, 1989; Hamann, 1990; Srinivas & the nature of such learning might be, constitute Roediger, 1990). the questions examined in the present article. Both perceptual and conceptual forms of We investigated these questions in a densely priming, or implicit memory, have been exten- amnesic individual because the minimal opera- sively examined in amnesia and found to be tions of explicit memory may help isolate the generally preserved (see Moscovitch et al., implicit processes that mediate new verbal 1993, for a review), although the evidence for learning. preserved conceptual priming is somewhat Recent studies have investigated the role of mixed. Many studies have shown preserved implicit processes in mediating new verbal conceptual priming in amnesia across a variety learning in the memory-intact as well as the of conceptual implicit tasks (Gardner et al., amnesic populations. The latter set of studies is 1973; Hamann, 1990; Keane et al., 1997; directly relevant here, and these studies fall into Schacter, 1985; Shimamura & Squire, 1984; three general categories. In one set of studies, NEW LEARNING IN AMNESIA 293 the acquisition of hitherto unknown words was 1987), and name–face associations (Thoene & examined in the amnesic population. The results Glisky, 1995), the most extensively studied par- of these studies are mixed. Some studies have adigm in this domain involves the learning of shown little, if any, evidence of acquisition of novel word associations. In a typical experiment new vocabulary in the medial temporal lobe of this sort, participants are presented with un- patient H.M. (Gabrieli et al., 1988) or in Kor- related word pairs (window–reason, march– sakoff’s patients (Grossman, 1987). In contrast, shave) and are later presented with intact pairs positive evidence of new learning has been re- (window–rea_____) or recombined pairs ported in amnesics in some studies. For exam- (march–rea_____) during the stem completion ple, there have been reports of progress in the task. The advantage in stem completion priming academic training of a young amnesic girl for intact pairs over recombined pairs provides (Wood et al., 1989), normal acquisition of new the measure of new learning. The evidence with French vocabulary (Hirst et al., 1988),
Recommended publications
  • Benefits of Immediate Repetition Versus Long Study Presentation On
    Neuropsychology In the public domain 2010, Vol. 24, No. 4, 457–464 DOI: 10.1037/a0018625 Benefits of Immediate Repetition Versus Long Study Presentation on Memory in Amnesia Mieke Verfaellie and Karen F. LaRocque Suparna Rajaram Memory Disorders Research Center, VA Boston Healthcare Stony Brook University System, and Boston University School of Medicine Objective: This study aimed to resolve discrepant findings in the literature regarding the effects of massed repetition and a single long study presentation on memory in amnesia. Method: Experiment 1 assessed recognition memory in 9 amnesic patients and 18 controls following presentation of a study list that contained items shown for a single short study presentation, a single long study presentation, and three massed repetitions. In Experiment 2, the same encoding conditions were presented in a blocked rather than intermixed format to all participants from Experiment 1. Results: In Experiment 1, control participants showed benefits associated with both types of extended exposure, and massed repetition was more beneficial than long study presentation, F(2, 34) ϭ 14.03, p Ͻ .001, partial ␩2 ϭ .45. In contrast, amnesic participants failed to show benefits of either type of extended exposure, F Ͻ 1. In Experiment 2, both groups benefited from repetition, but did so in different ways, F(2, 50) ϭ 4.80, p ϭ .012, partial ␩2 ϭ .16. Amnesic patients showed significant and equivalent benefit associated with both types of extended exposure, F(2, 16) ϭ 5.58, p ϭ .015, partial ␩2 ϭ .41, but control participants again benefited more from massed repetition than from long study presentation, F(2, 34) ϭ 23.74, p Ͻ .001, partial ␩2 ϭ .58.
    [Show full text]
  • The Unconscious Mind: Kinder and Gentler Than That
    3/20/2013 The Freudian 20th Century “Freud in 21st-Century America” The Unconscious Mind: Kinder and Gentler Than That John F. Kihlstrom Forbidden Planet (1956) Department of Psychology University of California, Berkeley 1 2 The Discovery of the Unconscious Petites Perceptions Ellenberger (1970) Leibniz (1704/1981); Ellenberger (1970) • At every moment there is in us an infinity of perceptions, unaccompanied by awareness or reflection…. That is why we are never indifferent, even when we appear to be most so…. The choice that we make arises from these insensible stimuli, which… make us find one direction of movement more comfortable than the 3 other. 4 The Limen Unconscious Inferences Herbart (1819) Helmholtz (1866/1968) • One of the older ideas can… be • The psychic activities that lead us to infer that completely driven out of consciousness by there in front of us at a certain place there is a a new much weaker idea. On the other certain object of a certain character, are generally hand its pressure there is not to be not conscious activities, but unconscious ones. In regarded as without effect; rather it works their result they are the equivalent to conclusion…. with full power against the ideas which are But what seems to differentiate them from a conclusion, in the ordinary sense of that word, is present in consciousness. It thus causes that a conclusion is an act of conscious thought…. a particular state of consciousness, though Still it may be permissible to speak of the psychic its object is in no sense really imagined. acts of ordinary perception as unconscious 5 conclusions….
    [Show full text]
  • Implicit and Explicit Knowledge About Language
    Comp. by: TPrasath Date:27/12/06 Time:22:59:29 Stage:First Proof File Path:// spiina1001z/womat/production/PRODENV/0000000005/0000001817/0000000016/ 0000233189.3D Proof by: QC by: NICK ELLIS IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT KNOWLEDGE ABOUT LANGUAGE INTRODUCTION Children acquire their first language (L1) by engaging with their caretakers in natural meaningful communication. From this “evidence” they automatically acquire complex knowledge of the structure of their language. Yet paradoxically they cannot describe this knowledge, the discovery of which forms the object of the disciplines of theoretical lin- guistics, psycholinguistics, and child language acquisition. This is a difference between explicit and implicit knowledge—ask a young child how to form a plural and she says she does not know; ask her “here is a wug, here is another wug, what have you got?” and she is able to reply, “two wugs.” The acquisition of L1 grammar is implicit and is extracted from experience of usage rather than from explicit rules—simple expo- sure o normal linguistic input suffices and no explicit instruction is needed. Adult acquisition of second language (L2) is a different matter in that what can be acquired implicitly from communicative contexts is typically quite limited in comparison to native speaker norms, and adult attainment of L2 accuracy usually requires additional resources of explicit learning. The various roles of consciousness in second language acquisi- tion (SLA) include: the learner noticing negative evidence; their attending to language form, their perception focused by social scaffolding or explicit instruction; their voluntary use of pedagogical grammatical descriptions and analogical reasoning; their reflective induction of metalinguistic insights about language; and their consciously guided practice which results, eventually, in unconscious, automatized skill.
    [Show full text]
  • Implicit and Explicit Learning of Languages Patrick Rebuschat
    Introduction: Implicit and explicit learning of languages Patrick Rebuschat (Lancaster University) Introduction to the forthcoming volume: Rebuschat, P. (Ed.) (in press, 2015). Implicit and explicit learning of languages. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Implicit learning, essentially the process of acquiring unconscious (implicit) knowledge, is a fundamental feature of human cognition (Cleeremans, Destrebecqz, & Boyer, 1998; Dienes, 2012; Perruchet, 2008; Shanks, 2005; Reber, 1993). Many complex behaviors, including language comprehension and production (Berry & Dienes, 1993; Winter & Reber, 1994), music cognition (Rohrmeier & Rebuschat, 2012), intuitive decision making (Plessner, Betsch, & Betsch, 2008), and social interaction (Lewicki, 1986), are thought to be largely dependent on implicit knowledge. The term implicit learning was first used by Arthur Reber (1967) to describe a process during which subjects acquire knowledge about a complex, rule-governed stimulus environment without intending to and without becoming aware of the knowledge they have acquired. In contrast, the term explicit learning refers to a process during which participants acquire conscious (explicit) knowledge; this is generally associated with intentional learning conditions, e.g., when participants are instructed to look for rules or patterns. In his seminal study, Reber (1967) exposed subjects to letter sequences (e.g., TPTS, VXXVPS and TPTXXVS) by means of a memorization task. In experiment 1, subjects were presented with letter sequences and simply asked to commit them to memory. One group of subjects was given sequences that were generated by means of a finite-state grammar (Chomsky, 1956, 1957; Chomsky & Miller, 1958), while the other group received randomly 1 constructed sequences. The results showed that grammatical letter sequences were learned more rapidly than random letter sequences.
    [Show full text]
  • Evidence of Automatic Processing in Sequence Learning Using Process-Dissociation
    RESEARCH ARTICLE ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY Evidence of automatic processing in sequence learning using process-dissociation Heather M. Mong, David P. McCabe, and Benjamin A. Clegg Department of Psychology, Colorado State University, Fort Collins, CO, USA ABSTRACT This paper proposes a way to apply process-dissociation to sequence learning in addition and ex- tension to the approach used by Destrebecqz and Cleeremans (2001). Participants were trained on two sequences separated from each other by a short break. Following training, participants self-reported their knowledge of the sequences. A recognition test was then performed which re- quired discrimination of two trained sequences, either under the instructions to call any sequence encountered in the experiment “old” (the inclusion condition), or only sequence fragments from one half of the experiment “old” (the exclusion condition). The recognition test elicited automatic KEYWORDS and controlled process estimates using the process dissociation procedure, and suggested both implicit learning, sequence processes were involved. Examining the underlying processes supporting performance may pro- learning, process- vide more information on the fundamental aspects of the implicit and explicit constructs than has dissociation, consciousness been attainable through awareness testing. INTRODUCTION Shanks, 2005), and numerous definitions of the term itself have been offered (Dienes & Perner, 1999; Frensch, 1998). This variety is further The serial reaction time task (SRTT) has become an extremely pro- reflected in an array of methods for assessing the presence or absence ductive method for studying sequence learning (for reviews, see of explicit knowledge. Issues raised have included whether explicit Abrahamse, Jimenez, Verwey, & Clegg, 2010; Clegg, DiGirolamo, knowledge is necessary for learning, what awareness tests should be & Keele, 1998).
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Personality and Cognitions Underlying Entrepreneurial Intentions Benjamin R. Walker a Thesis Submitted in Partial Fulfilment O
    1 Personality and Cognitions underlying Entrepreneurial Intentions Benjamin R. Walker A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy School of Management UNSW Business School March 30, 2015 2 Table of Contents Acknowledgements .................................................................................................................... 6 Originality statement .................................................................................................................. 7 Publications and conference presentations arising from this thesis ........................................... 8 List of abbreviations .................................................................................................................. 9 Thesis Abstract......................................................................................................................... 10 Chapter 1: Introduction ............................................................................................................ 11 Chapter 2: Assessing the impact of revised Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory ...................... 20 Table 1: Articles with original Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (o-RST) and revised Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (r-RST) measures .......................................................... 26 Table 2: Categorization of original Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (o-RST) and revised Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (r-RST) studies in the five years from 2010-2014 ........ 29 Chapter 3: How
    [Show full text]
  • Implicit Learning and Tacit Knowledge
    Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Copyrighl 1989 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1989, Vol. 118, No. 3, 219-235 0096-3445/89/500,75 Implicit Learning and Tacit Knowledge Arthur S. Reber Brooklyn College and the Graduate Center City University of New York I examine the phenomenon of implicit learning, the process by which knowledge about the rale- governed complexities of the stimulus environment is acquired independently of conscious attempts to do so. Our research with the two, seemingly disparate experimental paradigms of synthetic grammar learning and probability learning is reviewed and integrated with other approaches to the general problem of unconscious cognition. The conclusions reached are as follows: (a) Implicit learning produces a tacit knowledge base that is abstract and representative of the structure of the environment; (b) such knowledge is optimally acquired independently of conscious efforts to learn; and (c) it can be used implicitly to solve problems and make accurate decisions about novel stimulus circumstances. Various epistemological issues and related prob- 1 lems such as intuition, neuroclinical disorders of learning and memory, and the relationship of evolutionary processes to cognitive science are also discussed. Some two decades ago the term implicit learning was first lined, and analyzed, but virtually no attention was paid to the used to characterize how one develops intuitive knowledge processes by which these implicit memories "got there." This about the underlying structure of a complex stimulus envi- general lack of attention to the acquisition problem may be ronment (Reber, 1965, 1967). In those early writings, I argued one of the reasons why much recent theorizing has been that implicit learning is characterized by two critical features: oriented toward a nativist position (e.g., Chomsky, 1980; (a) It is an unconscious process and (b) it yields abstract Fodor, 1975, 1983; Gleitman & Wanner, 1982).
    [Show full text]
  • The Digital Expansion of the Mind: Implications of Internet Usage For
    Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition 8 (2019) 1–14 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition journa l homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jarmac The Digital Expansion of the Mind: Implications of Internet Usage for Memory and Cognition a,∗ b Elizabeth J. Marsh , Suparna Rajaram a Duke University, United States b Stony Brook University, United States The internet is rapidly changing what information is available as well as how we find it and share it with others. Here we examine how this “digital expansion of the mind” changes cognition. We begin by identifying ten properties of the internet that likely affect cognition, roughly organized around internet content (e.g., the sheer amount of information available), internet usage (e.g., the requirement to search for information), and the people and communities who create and propagate content (e.g., people are connected in an unprecedented fashion). We use these properties to explain (or ask questions about) internet-related phenomena, such as habitual reliance on the internet, the propagation of misinformation, and consequences for autobiographical memory, among others. Our goal is to consider the impact of internet usage on many aspects of cognition, as people increasingly rely on the internet to seek, post, and share information. Keywords: Memory, Cognition, Internet, External memory, Metacognition, Social memory General Audience Summary The internet is rapidly changing what information is available to us as well as how we find that information and share it with others. Here we ask how this “digital expansion of the mind” may change cognition.
    [Show full text]
  • Implicit Learning, Development, and Education
    Implicit learning, development, and education A. Vinter, S. Pacton, A. Witt and P. Perruchet Introduction The present chapter focuses on implicit learning processes, and aims at showing that these processes could be used to design new methods of education or reeducation. After a brief definition of what we intend by implicit learning, we will show that these proc- esses operate efficiently in development, from infancy to aging. Then, we will discuss the question of their resistance to neurological or psychiatric diseases. Finally, in a last section, we will comment on their potential use within an applied perspective. The fundamental role of learning, once neglected by cognitive psychologists a few decades ago, is now acknowledged in most areas of research, including language, cate- gorization and object perception (2). Of course, nobody has ever claimed that language development is independent of infants’ experience. However, the dominant Chom- skian tradition has confined learning to subsidiary functions, such as setting the values of parameters in a hardwired system. Recent work shows that fundamental components of language such as word segmentation (69) can be learned in an incidental way similar to that involved in the acquisition of other human abilities. Likewise, it has never been denied that learning plays a role in categorization and object perception, but acquisition processes were thought to be limited to new combinations of preestablished features (7). However, Schyns and Rodet (77) have shown that elementary features can them- selves be created with experience (for a review, see 74, 75). The type of learning process that receives the most attention in the current literature relates to implicit learning.
    [Show full text]
  • UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society
    UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society Title Workshop: The Psychology of Negotiation: When, Why and How Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/20s8493w Journal Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 33(33) ISSN 1069-7977 Authors Feldman, Laurie Beth Van Hell, Janet Kroll, Judith et al. Publication Date 2011 Peer reviewed eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California Workshop: The Psychology of Negotiation: When, Why and How Keynote Speaker: Hannah Riley Bowles Kennedy School Harvard University Laurie Beth Feldman ([email protected]) Janet van Hell ([email protected]) Department of Psychology; SS 369 619 Moore Building The University at Albany, SUNY Department of Psychology Albany, NY 12222, USA The Pennsylvania State University University Park, PA 16802 USA Judith Kroll ([email protected]) Suparna Rajaram ([email protected]) 641 Moore Building Department of Psychology Department of Psychology Stony Brook University The Pennsylvania State University Stony Brook, NY 11794-2500 USA University Park, PA 16802 USA Keywords: professional development; negotiation; start up Stuhlmacher & Walters, 1999). Further, gender based packages. differences around willingness to negotiate are not restricted Economic conditions are placing unprecedented pressures to people with lower levels of education. In fact, recent male on universities and funding agencies. For young scholars, this PhDs are almost three times more likely than recent female translates into fewer postdoctoral and faculty lines. For PhDs to initiate financial negotiations in their first highly experienced senior scholars, it emerges as fewer professional position. Finally, the pattern is as common opportunities for new programs of research and among twenty year olds as among 40 year olds (Babcock & collaborations, and this in turn limits the options available to Laschever, 2003), and gender differences are observed both senior scientists for training young scientists.
    [Show full text]
  • Implicit Learning in Children Is Not Related to Age: Evidence from Drawing Behavior
    Child Development, September/October 2000, Volume 71, Number 5, Pages 1223–1240 Implicit Learning in Children Is Not Related to Age: Evidence from Drawing Behavior Annie Vinter and Pierre Perruchet Three experiments are reported on implicit learning in 432 children between the ages of 4 and 10 years, using a new paradigm (“the neutral parameter procedure”) based on drawing behavior. The first two experiments demonstrated that children modified their drawing behavior following specially devised practice in such a way that these modifications could not be viewed as the result of deliberate adaptive strategies. The third ex- periment showed that these behavioral modifications lasted for at least 1 hr after the training phase. No age- related differences appeared in the experiments. A comparison of children’s data with similar adults’ data also failed to reveal any age differences. These results provide compelling evidence that implicit learning processes are age independent. Some implications of these results for developmental issues are discussed, notably the hypothesis of the formation of implicit knowledge in the course of learning. INTRODUCTION in which children’s behavior becomes sensitive to the structural features of an experienced situation, with- The fundamental role of learning, neglected by cogni- out the adaptation being due to an intentional exploi- tive psychologists a few decades ago, is now ac- tation of some pieces of explicit knowledge about knowledged in most areas of research, including lan- these features (Perruchet & Vinter, 1998). The two im- guage, categorization, and object perception (Bates & portant components of implicit learning are thus the Elman, 1996). Of course, nobody has ever claimed behavioral sensitivity to the structure of the situation that language development is independent of infants’ and the lack of intentional causes for this sensitivity.
    [Show full text]
  • 2018 APS Janet Taylor Spence Award Recipients Observervol
    2018 APS Janet Taylor Spence Award Recipients ObserverVol. 31, No. 5 May/June 2018 T H E M A N Y S H A P E S O F A P P L I E D a publication ofPSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE www.psychologicalscience.org/observer Advances in Methods Association for Psychological Science | 30th Annual Convention NEW APS and Practices in JOURNAL Psychological Science FEATURED SPEAKERS NOW AVAILABLE Fred Kavli Keynote Address Bring the Family Address Making and Remaking Memory: The Paradox of Diversity: Past, Present, and Future Promise, Pitfalls, and Advances in Methods and Practices in LYNN NADEL Implications for Racial Progress Psychological Science (AMPPS) The University of Arizona JENNIFER RICHESON Yale University AMPPS, a unique and innovative new journal, presents methodological advances from across all areas in our diverse field of psychological science. Presidential Symposium Memory: From Neurons to Nations AMPPS articles are highly accessible and include: SUPARNA RAJARAM Stony Brook University, • Empirical research that exemplifies scientific The State University of New York (Chair) best practices CHARAN RANGANATH University of California, Davis • Articles that communicate advances in DORTHE BERNTSEN Henry L. Roediger, III, will be the methods, practices, and meta-science featured scientist in this year’s Inside the Aarhus University, Denmark Psychologist’s Studio. He will be interviewed • Tutorials, commentaries, and simulation studies QI WANG by his former student, APS President Suparna for new techniques and research tools Cornell University Rajaram. • Papers that bring advances from a specialized HENRY L. ROEDIGER, III Washington University in St. Louis subfield to a broader audience • Registered Replication Reports, an innovative APS-David Myers Distinguished Lecture On article type originated by APS The Science And Craft Of Teaching Psychological Science Improving the Use of Psychological Science in K-12 Education Check out the first issue now available at: DANIEL T.
    [Show full text]