Short Term Memory

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Short Term Memory SHORT TERM MEMORY Dissertation submitted for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the University of Stirling by Neil Thomson University of York November, 1979 ii ABSTRACT The eight experiments reported in this thesis are designed to investigate the idea that in verbal short-term memory (STM) material decays over time and this decay is prevented by rehearsal. It follows that the capacity of STM when measured in words should be inversely proportional to the time taken to rehearse the words. Consequently, subjects should be able to recall more short duration words than long duration words. In contrast to this hypothesis is the idea that the capacity of STM is a fixed number of chunks, where chunks are a structural character­ istic of the material. The first four experiments are designed so that these alternative hypotheses produce conflicting predictions and, in all cases, the hypotheses derived from decay theory are supported. It is shown that serial recall performance is very well predicted by the time taken to say the words and that the relationship between word duration and recall is of the type predicted by decay theory. The second set of experiments are based on the assump­ tion that both STM and long-term memory (LTM) contribute to performance in serial recall tasks. The purpose of the experiments is to determine whether it is the STM or LTM component that is sensitive to word duration. It is predicted, in line with a decay theory of forgetting in STM, that the STM component is sensitive to word duration. The experiments are designed to produce sizable contributions iii from both stores in order to test this hypothesis. The results support the hypothesis in showing that variables known to affect ST~ such as acoustic similarity, interact with word duration, while variables known to affect LTM, such as repeated presentations of the same list, show no such interaction. The results are interpreted in terms of decay theory and the different versions of this theory that have been proposed are considered. It is concluded that while no version of the theory is completely adequate, there is no evidence that invalidates the central assumptions, viz. that in STM items are forgotten by decay and that one of the functions of rehearsal is to prevent this decay. iv PREFACE The experiments reported here are original and were carried out by me and unless it is stated otherwise the ideas expressed are original. I would like to thank Dr. Alan Baddeley, who supervised this work while he was at Stirling University, for his support and encouragement. I am grateful to Dr. Peter Hamilton and Dr. Philip Smith for taking over the supervision of this work when Dr. Baddeley left Stirling. I should also like to thank Mrs. Marilyn Glicker who typed this thesis. Neither this dissertation nor any part of it has been submitted to any other university. v CONTENTS Chapter I Introduction 1 Research Review 4 II The Chunking HlEothesis 28 Experiment 1 32 Experiment 2 40 Experiment 3 45 Experiment 4 52 III The Word Length Effect 68 Experiment 5 69 Experiment 6 85 Experiment 7 96 Experiment 8 111 IV General Discussion 123 Summary of Experimental Results 123 Inferences drawn from the Results 124 Some Theoretical Implications 125 Some General Implications 1 ItS Conclusions 1",1 V References 14 t VI Appendices 15' - 1 - INTRODUCTION The variety of meaning in human communication is mainly achieved by combining simple elements into different sequences. This ability to use serial order has long been recognized as a question of central importance in Psychology (Lashley, 1951). It is still, however, not well understood despite the abundance of research into serial recall and the mechan­ isms underlying it. The latter have been most commonly conceptualized as short- and long-term memory (STM and LTM) and STM in particular has conventionally been regarded as an input system which receives and holds information prior to its encoding in LTM (Waugh & Norman, 1965; Atkinson and Shiffrin, 1968). An alternative proposal has been that STM is an output store involved in the production of speech (Morton, 1970; Ellis, 1976). Although placing STM at opposite ends of the information processing system, both proposals require that the store be capable of dealing with serially ordered information. Speech as a form of human communication is serial by nature while the permanent storage of information cannot be. ThUS, given that people decode serial information into a permanent message and vice-versa, the constraints of serial ordering will be felt most strongly at the points of perception and production. At input, the store would be responsible for holding serially organized information until it could be encoded into a permanent message, whereas at output, the store would hold material translated from a permanent message while it was being produced serially. In neither case would STM be - 2 - holding material in the code used for permanent storage and consequently the meaning of the message should not interact with the properties of the store. Further, it might be expected that the store would have a relatively small capacity as the "perceptual units" of speech are fairly short whether taken as syllables, words, tone groups or phrases. By this account, then, STM is a peripheral store with a limited capacity, employing some form of speech code and insensitive to the meaning of a message. This description of STM is very much in agreement with those derived from experimental work as will become apparent from the research review below. However, there is one very important experi­ mental result which would seem to contradict directly the assumption that STM is insensitive to the meaning of a message and it is this contradiction that provides the starting point for the thesis. Miller (1956) showed that the amount people could remember in a serial recall task was 7±2 chunks where a chunk was defined as a subjective meaningful unit, e.g. HPD might be three chunks while PHD would be one. In so far as chunks are "meaningful units" they are units of permanent storage rather than units of some peripheral code and consequently, Miller's result would not be predicted by the view that STM is a peripheral store. Thus, there is a contradiction in the literature between the experimentally supported idea that STM is a peripheral store employing a speech code and the fact that the capacity of STM is constant when measured in units of permanent storage. The general aim of this thesis is to resolve this contradiction by - 3 - testing the chunking hypothesis. This is not as straight­ forward as it might seem, because the chunk is defined subjectively and hence the hypothesis is essentially irrefutable. Typically this problem has been avoided by making the simplifying assumption that such experimenter defined units as words, digits and letters constitute chunks to the subject. Where lists are composed of sequences of these items chosen to be unrelated one to another this assumption seems plausible and allows a weaker version of Miller's hypothesis to be tested: that memory span is a constant number of chunks where chunks are defined experimental units. It will be shown that for tasks which rely heavily on STM this hypothesis fails for a variety of definitions of a chunk and Miller's results will be explained by arguing that he used tasks in which there was a large LTM contribution to recall. It will be assumed that, because LTM is responsible for the storage of a permanent message, it employs a semantic code and consequently is responsible for chunking. The experimental work is based on two main assumptions: first, thatat least two stores mediate performance on memory span and serial recall tasks, viz. STM and LTM, and secondly, that STM is a peripheral store responsible for holding the units of perception or production in a prescribed order. Before introducing the experiments, these two assumptions will be evaluated in the context of previous research. - 4 - RESEARCH REVIEW The idea that STM is a peripheral store and particularly one involved in speech perception or production implies that it should employ a speech code; acoustic if it is an input store and articulatory if it is an output store. Conrad (1964) showed that errors produced in immediate serial recall were similar to those produced when attempting to identify verbal material in noise and, in particular, items that sounded similar tended to be confused. He called this the acoustic similarity effect (AS) and it implies that STM is sensitive to the physical properties of a message as would be expected of a peripheral store. Further, he showed that the AS effect was not dependent on the material being presented auditonally, but also occurred with visually presented material. This latter result is particularly important as it suggests that the store mediating performance is not modality specific. Thus, if it is to be argued that STM is an input store, it must occur after the material has been encoded in a form that is independent of modality. This would mean that the store is not subject to the same constraints of serial ordering as a truly peripheral store responsible for receiving serially organized messages. However, the experimental evidence reviewed below suggests that STM is a peripheral output store responsible for holding serially organized articulatory commands while they are being produced and consequently would not be modality specific. - 5 - Baddeley (1966a; 1966b) extended Conrad's results and showed that, while AS was detrimental to immediate serial recall, semantic similarity had little effect on performance. However, when list length was increased and a rehearsal preventing task interpolated between presentation and recall, AS had little effect while semantic similarity consistently impaired performance.
Recommended publications
  • Implications of Short-Term Memory for a General Theory of Memory 1
    3"OURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR 2, 1-21 (1963) ADDRESS OF CHAIRMAN OF SECTION I (Psychology) Implications of Short-Term Memory for a General Theory of Memory 1 ARTHUR W. MELTON University o/Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan Memory has never enjoyed even a small The confluence of forces responsible for fraction of the interdisciplinary interest that this sanguine prediction about future progress has been expressed in symposia, discoveries, is reflected in this AAAS program on memory and methodological innovations during the (see other articles in this issue of this last five years. Therefore, it seems probable JOURNAL). Advances in biochemistry and that the next ten years will see major, perhaps neurophysiology are permitting the formula- even definitive, advances in our understanding tion and testing of meaningful theories about of the biochemistry, neurophysiology, and the palpable stuff that is the correlate of the psychology of memory, especially if these memory trace as an hypothetical construct disciplines communicate with one another (Deutsch, 1962; Gerard, 1963; Thomas, and seek a unified theory. My thesis is, of 1962). In this work there is heavy emphasis course, that psychological studies of human on the storage mechanism and its properties, short-term memory, and particularly the especially the consolidation process, and it further exploitation of new techniques for may be expected that findings here will investigating human short-term memory, will offer important guide lines for the refine- play an important role in these advances ment of the psychologist's construct once we toward a general theory of memory. Even are clear as to what our human performance now, some critical issues are being sharpened data say it should be.
    [Show full text]
  • Auditory and Visual Memory Skills in Normal Children
    University of Montana ScholarWorks at University of Montana Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers Graduate School 1977 Auditory and visual memory skills in normal children Joanne Mae Moulton The University of Montana Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umt.edu/etd Let us know how access to this document benefits ou.y Recommended Citation Moulton, Joanne Mae, "Auditory and visual memory skills in normal children" (1977). Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers. 4956. https://scholarworks.umt.edu/etd/4956 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at ScholarWorks at University of Montana. It has been accepted for inclusion in Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks at University of Montana. For more information, please contact [email protected]. AUDITORY AND VISUAL MEMORY SKILLS IN NORMAL CHILDREN By Joanne M. Moulton B.A., University of Montana, 1973 Presented in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts UNIVERSITY OF MONTANA 1977 Approved by: an, Graduate Schoo. UMI Number: EP40420 All rights reserved INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. In the uniikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Oisssrtatteft PsiMisMng UMI EP40420 Published by ProQuest LLC (2014). Copyright in the Dissertation held by the Author. Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC. All rights reserved. This work is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code ProQuest LLC.
    [Show full text]
  • The Many Faces of Forgetting: Toward a Constructive View Of
    +Model ARTICLE IN PRESS Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition xxx (2020) xxx–xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition j ournal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jarmac The Many Faces of Forgetting: Toward a Constructive View of Forgetting in Everyday Life ∗ Jonathan M. Fawcett Memorial University of Newfoundland, Canada Justin C. Hulbert Bard College, USA Forgetting is often considered a fundamental cognitive failure, reflecting the undesirable and potentially embar- rassing inability to retrieve a sought-after experience or fact. For this reason, forgetfulness has been argued to form the basis of many problems associated with our memory system. We highlight instead how forgetfulness serves many purposes within our everyday experience, giving rise to some of our best characteristics. Drawing from cognitive, neuroscientific, and applied research, we contextualize our findings in terms of their contributions along three important (if not entirely independent) roles supported by forgetting, namely (a) the maintenance of a positive and coherent self-image (“Guardian”), (b) the facilitation of efficient cognitive function (“Librarian”), and (c) the development of a creative and flexible worldview (“Inventor”). Together, these roles depict an expanded understanding of how forgetting provides memory with many of its cardinal virtues. General Audience Summary Our inability to remember the name of an acquaintance or an important date is both embarrassing and frustrat- ing. For that reason, forgetting is often viewed as a sign of impending cognitive decline or even as a character flaw. These fears have even driven some toward the promise of memory-enhancing pharmaceuticals and digital technologies designed to preserve memories indefinitely.
    [Show full text]
  • Memory Processes
    6 CHAPTER Memory Processes CHAPTER OUTLINE Encoding and Transfer of Information The Constructive Nature of Memory Forms of Encoding Autobiographical Memory Short-Term Storage Memory Distortions Long-Term Storage The Eyewitness Testimony Paradigm Transfer of Information from Short-Term Memory Repressed Memories to Long-Term Memory The Effect of Context on Memory Rehearsal Key Themes Organization of Information Summary Retrieval Retrieval from Short-Term Memory Thinking about Thinking: Analytical, Parallel or Serial Processing? Creative, and Practical Questions Exhaustive or Self-Terminating Processing? Key Terms The Winner—a Serial Exhaustive Model—with Some Qualifications Media Resources Retrieval from Long-Term Memory Intelligence and Retrieval Processes of Forgetting and Memory Distortion Interference Theory Decay Theory 228 CHAPTER 6 • Memory Processes 229 Here are some of the questions we will explore in this chapter: 1. What have cognitive psychologists discovered regarding how we encode information for storing it in memory? 2. What affects our ability to retrieve information from memory? 3. How does what we know or what we learn affect what we remember? n BELIEVE IT OR NOT THERE’SAREASON YOU REMEMBER THOSE ANNOYING SONGS that strengthens the connections associated with that Having a song or part of a song stuck in your head is phrase. In turn, this increases the likelihood that you will incredibly frustrating. We’ve all had the experience of the recall it, which leads to more reinforcement. song from a commercial repeatedly running through our You could break this unending cycle of repeated recall minds, even though we wanted to forget it. But sequence and reinforcement—even though this is a necessary and recall—remembering episodes or information in sequen- normal process for the strengthening and cementing of tial order (like the notes to a song)—has a special and memories—by introducing other sequences.
    [Show full text]
  • The Loss of Short-Term Visual Representations Over Time: Decay Or
    TIME-BASED FORGETTING IN VISUAL MEMORY 1 The loss of short-term visual representations over time: Decay or temporal distinctiveness? Tom Mercer Institute of Psychology, Faculty of Education, Health and Wellbeing, University of Wolverhampton, United Kingdom Correspondence address: Tom Mercer Institute of Psychology Faculty of Education, Health and Wellbeing University of Wolverhampton Wolverhampton, UK WV1 1AD Email: [email protected] Telephone: +44(0)1902 321368 Running head: Time-based forgetting in visual memory. NB This article may not exactly replicate the final version published in the APA journal. It is not the copy of record. TIME-BASED FORGETTING IN VISUAL MEMORY 2 ABSTRACT There has been much recent interest in the loss of visual short-term memories over the passage of time. According to decay theory, visual representations are gradually forgotten as time passes, reflecting a slow and steady distortion of the memory trace. However, this is controversial and decay effects can be explained in other ways. The present experiment aimed to re-examine the maintenance and loss of visual information over the short-term. Decay and temporal distinctiveness models were tested using a delayed discrimination task, where participants compared complex and novel objects over unfilled retention intervals of variable length. Experiment 1 found no significant change in the accuracy of visual memory from 2 to 6 s, but the gap separating trials reliably influenced task performance. Experiment 2 found evidence for information loss at a 10 s retention interval, but temporally separating trials restored the fidelity of visual memory, possibly because temporally isolated representations are distinct from older memory traces.
    [Show full text]
  • Mechanisms of Forgetting in Short-Term Memory
    COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2, 185-195 (1971) Mechanisms of Forgetting in Short-Term Memory JUDITH SPENCER REITMAS' The Unioersity of Michigan This paper introduces a technique applicable to the question: Does in- formation in short-term memory disappear with time? The technique appears to eliminate Ss’ rehearsal in the retention interval without introducing potentially interfering material. In the experiment, Ss read aloud three words, then engaged in a difficult auditory signal detection task intended to keep them from rehearsing for 15 set, and then attempted to recall the three words. The results provide no support for the principle of loss with time as an explanation of forgetting in short-term memory. Current theories of forgetting in short-term memory (STM) include one or more of the following four basic operating principles: Displace- ment (Waugh & Norman, 1965, 196S), decay (Brown, 1958, 1964), associative interference (Adams, 1967; Keppel & Underwood, 1962; Postman, 1961), and acid-bath interference (Posner & Konick, 1966; Reicher, Ligon, & Conrad, 1969). The latter three have in common the notion that without rehearsal information in STIM is lost in time. In the case of decay, it is straightforward. In the associative interference prin- ciple, loss in time occurs through the mechanism of unlearning; in the acid-bath principle, through the postulate that the greater the time an item is in the acid, the greater the loss. The displacement principle, on the other hand, says that it takes succeeding inputs to a limited-capacity buffer store to produce forgetting; nothing will be lost if time passes without new inputs displacing the resident items.
    [Show full text]
  • Is Neurocognition Crucial in STEM Education in the Present Scenario? S.Amutha* Bharathidasan University, Tiruchirappalli *Corresponding Author: S
    2020 Review Article Journal of Cognitive Neuropsychology Vol.4 No.2 Is Neurocognition Crucial in STEM Education in the Present Scenario? S.Amutha* Bharathidasan University, Tiruchirappalli *Corresponding author: S. Amutha, Bharathidasan University, Tiruchirappalli, E-mail: [email protected] Tel No: +91 9443145648 1. Abstract Countries around the globe moving away from traditional rote and regurgitation learning to experiential learning. Science, Technology, Engineering and Mathematics are the subjects interlinked with each other. Most of the countries in the world prefer to adopt STEM (Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics) education because it infuses every part of our lives. Without Science and Technology our survival in this globe is questioned. Technology is constantly escalating into every aspect of our lives. Engineering is the basic subject for designing the houses, machineries, construction etc. Basically everyone need Mathematics in every occupation and every activity of lives. STEM based curriculum comprise of hands-on and minds-on activities for the student. By divulging students to explore STEM-related concepts, they will develop a passion towards it. Nevertheless of voluminous initiatives by all the countries, are on the way to impart STEM education to students due to their difference in perception, knowledge and memory. Neurocognition is the process which activates the cognitive process. Depending on the needs of the individual Neurocognitive strategies will restructure or modify their thought, feelings, perceptions and emotions. This article addresses the theoretical back drop of Neurocognition in education and how the issues related to educational performance can be solved with the help of Neurocognitive practice. 2. Keywords: Science, Technology, Engineering, Mathematics, (STEM) Brain, Neurocognition 3.
    [Show full text]
  • 1965 Mathematical Models for Memory and Learning. Shiffrin
    MATHEMATICAL MODELS FOR MEMORY AND LEARNING by R. C. Atkinson and R. M. Shiffrin TECHNICAL REPORT NO. 79 September 20, 1965 PSYCHOLOGY SERIES Reproduction in Whole or in Part is Permitted for any purpose of the United States Government INSTITUTE FOR MATHEMATICAL STUDIES IN THE SOCIAL SCIENCES STANFORD UNIVERSITY STANFORD, CALIFORNIA MATHEMATICAL MODELS FOR ME:MORY AND LEARNING* by R. C. Atkinson and R. M. Shiffrin stanford University In recent years a number of models have been proposed to account for retention phenomena, with the emphasis primarily on short-term memory ex- periments. There has also been an active development of models for verbal learning, with the focus on experiments dealing with serial and paired- associate learning. Except for a few notable exceptions, most of these theoretical developments have been applicable either to memory or learning experiments, and no attempt has been made to bridge the gap. It is our feeling that theoretical and experimental work in these two areas is suffi- ciently well advanced to warrant the development of a general theory that encompasses both sets of phenomena. This, then, is the goal of the paper. We must admit, however, that the term "general theory" may not be entirely appropriate, for many features of the system are still vague and undefined. Nevertheless, the work has progressed to a point where it is possible to use the general conceptual framework to specify several mathematical models *This paper was prepared for the "Third Conference on Learning, Remembering, and Forgetting" sponsored by the New York Academy of Science at Princeton, New Jersey, October 3 to 6, 1965.
    [Show full text]
  • Decay Theory of Immediate Memory: from Brown (1958) to Today (2014)
    The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology ISSN: 1747-0218 (Print) 1747-0226 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pqje20 Decay theory of immediate memory: From Brown (1958) to today (2014) Timothy J. Ricker, Evie Vergauwe & Nelson Cowan To cite this article: Timothy J. Ricker, Evie Vergauwe & Nelson Cowan (2016) Decay theory of immediate memory: From Brown (1958) to today (2014), The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69:10, 1969-1995, DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2014.914546 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2014.914546 Published online: 22 May 2014. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 585 View related articles View Crossmark data Citing articles: 5 View citing articles Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=pqje20 Download by: [University of Missouri-Columbia] Date: 26 August 2016, At: 06:00 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2016 Vol. 69, No. 10, 1969–1995, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2014.914546 Decay theory of immediate memory: From Brown (1958) to today (2014) Timothy J. Ricker, Evie Vergauwe, and Nelson Cowan Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA (Received 30 September 2013; accepted 29 December 2013; first published online 22 May 2014) This work takes a historical approach to discussing Brown’s (1958) paper, “Some Tests of the Decay Theory of Immediate Memory”. This work was and continues to be extremely influential in the field of forgetting over the short term. Its primary importance is in establishing a theoretical basis to consider a process of fundamental importance: memory decay.
    [Show full text]
  • Wixted, J. T. (2004)
    2 Dec 2003 18:44 AR AR207-PS55-09.tex AR207-PS55-09.sgm LaTeX2e(2002/01/18) P1: GCE 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141555 Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2004. 55:235–69 doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141555 Copyright c 2004 by Annual Reviews. All rights reserved First published online as a Review in Advance on November 3, 2003 THE PSYCHOLOGY AND NEUROSCIENCE OF FORGETTING John T. Wixted Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093-0109; email: [email protected] Key Words interference, consolidation, psychopharmacology, LTP, sleep ■ Abstract Traditional theories of forgetting are wedded to the notion that cue- overload interference procedures (often involving the A-B, A-C list-learning paradigm) capture the most important elements of forgetting in everyday life. However, findings from a century of work in psychology, psychopharmacology, and neuroscience con- verge on the notion that such procedures may pertain mainly to forgetting in the lab- oratory and that everyday forgetting is attributable to an altogether different form of interference. According to this idea, recently formed memories that have not yet had a chance to consolidate are vulnerable to the interfering force of mental activity and mem- ory formation (even if the interfering activity is not similar to the previously learned material). This account helps to explain why sleep, alcohol, and benzodiazepines all improve memory for a recently learned list, and it is consistent with recent work on the variables that affect the induction and
    [Show full text]
  • Memory Structures and Processes 107
    Chapter 5 distribute Memory Structuresor and Processes post, Questions to Consider •• Is memory a process, a structure, or a system? •• How many types of memoriescopy, are there? •• Are there differences in the ways we store and retrieve memories based on how old the memories are?not •• What kind of memory helps us focus on a task? •• How Dodoes our memory influence us unintentionally? •• What are the limits of our memory? 105 Copyright ©2019 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. 106 Cognitive Psychology Introduction: The Pervasiveness of Memory Memory is pervasive. It is important for so many things we do in our everyday lives that it is difficult to think of something humans do that doesn’t involve memory. To better understand its importance, imagine trying to do your everyday tasks without memory. When you first wake up in the morning you know whether you need to jump out of bed and hurry to get ready to leave or whether you can lounge in bed for a while because you remember what you have to do that day and what time your first task of the day begins. Without memory, you would not know what you needed to do that day. In fact, you would not know who you are, where you are, or what you are supposed to be doing at any given moment. It would be like waking up disoriented every minute. There are, in fact, individuals who must live their lives without the aid of these kinds of memories.
    [Show full text]
  • The Relationship Between Attention and Working Memory
    In: New Research on Short-Term Memory ISBN 978-1-60456-548-5 Editor: Noah B. Johansen, pp. © 2008 Nova Science Publishers, Inc. Chapter 1 The Relationship between Attention and Working Memory Daryl Fougnie Vanderbilt University Abstract The ability to selectively process information (attention) and to retain information in an accessible state (working memory) are critical aspects of our cognitive capacities. While there has been much work devoted to understanding attention and working memory, the nature of the relationship between these constructs is not well understood. Indeed, while neither attention nor working memory represent a uniform set of processes, theories of their relationship tend to focus on only some aspects. This review of the literature examines the role of perceptual and central attention in the encoding, maintenance, and manipulation of information in working memory. While attention and working memory were found to interact closely during encoding and manipulation, the evidence suggests a limited role of attention in the maintenance of information. Additionally, only central attention was found to be necessary for manipulating information in working memory. This suggests that theories should consider the multifaceted nature of attention and working memory. The review concludes with a model describing how attention and working memory interact. I. Introduction The capacity to perform some complex tasks depends critically on the ability to retain task-relevant information in an accessible state over time (working memory) and to selectively process information in the environment (attention). As one example, consider driving a car in an unfamiliar city. In order to get to your destination, directions have to be retained and kept in working memory.
    [Show full text]