Statistical Power, Sample Size, and Their Reporting in Randomized Controlled Trials

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Statistical Power, Sample Size, and Their Reporting in Randomized Controlled Trials Statistical Power, Sample Size, and Their Reporting in Randomized Controlled Trials David Moher, MSc; Corinne S. Dulberg, PhD, MPH; George A. Wells, PhD Objective.\p=m-\Todescribe the pattern over time in the level of statistical power and least 80% power to detect a 25% relative the reporting of sample size calculations in published randomized controlled trials change between treatment groups and (RCTs) with negative results. that 31% (22/71) had a 50% relative Design.\p=m-\Ourstudy was a descriptive survey. Power to detect 25% and 50% change, as statistically significant (a=.05, relative differences was calculated for the subset of trials with results in one tailed). negative Since its the of which a was used. Criteria were both publication, report simple two-group parallel design developed Freiman and has been cited to trial results as or and to the outcomes. colleagues2 classify positive negative identify primary more than 700 times, possibly indicating Power calculations were based on results from the primary outcomes reported in the seriousness with which investi¬ the trials. gators have taken the findings. Given Population.\p=m-\Wereviewed all 383 RCTs published in JAMA, Lancet, and the this citation record, one might expect an New England Journal of Medicine in 1975, 1980, 1985, and 1990. increase over time in the awareness of Results.\p=m-\Twenty-sevenpercent of the 383 RCTs (n=102) were classified as the consequences of low power in pub¬ having negative results. The number of published RCTs more than doubled from lished RCTs and, hence, an increase in 1975 to 1990, with the proportion of trials with negative results remaining fairly the reporting of sample size calcula¬ stable. Of the trials results with tions made before clinical trials are con¬ simple two-group parallel design having negative ducted. dichotomous or continuous outcomes 16% and 36% had suf- primary (n=70), only Our was to describe the ficient statistical to detect a 25% or 50% relative objective pat¬ power (80%) difference, respec- tern, over time, in the level ofpower and tively. These percentages did not consistently increase overtime. Overall, only 32% in the reporting of sample size calcula¬ of the trials with negative results reported sample size calculations, but the tions in published RCTs with negative percentage doing so has improved over time from 0% in 1975 to 43% in 1990. Only results since the publication of Freiman 20 of the 102 reports made any statement related to the clinical significance of the and colleagues'2 report. We did this by observed differences. extending Frieman and colleagues' ob¬ Conclusions.\p=m-\Mosttrials with negative results did not have large jectives during the period from 1975 to enough 1990. sample sizes to detect a 25% or a 50% relative difference. This result has not changed over time. Few trials discussed whether the observed differences were METHODS There are reasons to clinically important. important change this practice. The Study Sample reporting of statistical power and sample size also needs to be improved. (JAMA. 1994;272:122-124) We reviewed RCTs published in JAMA, Lancet, and the New England Journal of Medicine. These were the three ofthe 20journals from which more THE EFFICACY of new interventions probability of making a type II error, or than half (36/71) of the trials with nega¬ are most readily accepted if the results concluding that the result is not statis¬ tive results reported by Frieman and are from randomized controlled trials tically significant, when the observed colleagues were drawn.2 To capture the (RCTs).1 Essential to the planning of an effect is clinically meaningful. denominator, each volume published in RCT is estimation ofthe required sample The relationship between negative 1975, 1980, 1985, and 1990 was hand- size. The investigator should ensure that findings (ie, when statistical significance searched to extract the RCTs. To be there is sufficient power to detect, as was not reached) and statistical power considered an RCT, the study being as¬ statistically significant, a treatment ef¬ has been well illustrated in Freiman sessed had to contain an explicit state¬ fect of an a priori specified size. The and colleagues'2 review of 71 RCTs with ment about randomization. The identi¬ opposite perspective of conceiving the negative results published during 1960 fied RCTs were consecutively coded and problem is that the investigator should to 1977. These RCTs were drawn from divided into three equal groups for re¬ ensure that there is a low ß value, or a collection of300 simple two-group par¬ view by members of the study team, allel design trials with dichotomous pri¬ with use of a structured data collection From the Clinical Epidemiology Unit, Loeb Medical mary outcomes in 20 form. The data collected included infor¬ Research Institute (Mr Moher and Dr Wells), and the published journals. Faculties of Medicine (Mr Moher and Dr Wells) and Freiman and colleagues were interested mation on whether the trial results were Health Sciences (Dr Dulberg), University of Ottawa, in assessing whether RCTs with nega¬ positive or negative, the study design, Ottawa, Ontario. tive results had sufficient statistical whether a or hoc size Presented in part at the Second International Con- priori post sample detect a 25% a were gress on Peer Review in Biomedical Publication, Chi- power to and 50% rela¬ calculations performed, and the cago, Ill, September 10, 1993. tive difference between treatment in¬ elements necessary for us to calculate to the Reprint requests Clinical Epidemiology Unit, terventions. Their review indicated that observed or Loeb Medical Research Institute, Ottawa Civic Hospi- power (eg, proportions tal, 1053 Carling Ave, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada K1Y most of the trials had low power to de¬ means and SDs of the primary outcome 4E9 (Mr Moher). tect these effects: only 7% (5/71) had at obtained in each group). Downloaded From: by a The Bodleian Libraries of the University of Oxford User on 08/16/2018 Published Reports of Simple Two-Group Parallel ample, ifoutcomes included both disease- calculation was based. Only 45% re¬ Design Randomized Controlled Trials With Negative free survival and overall mortality, mor¬ ported the control group event rate, but With Dichotomous or Continuous Results (n=52) tality was considered to be the primary 79% specified the power level. Slightly (n=18) Primary Outcomes That Had at Least 80% outcome. more than half (58%) reported the Power to Detect Two Effect Sizes Between Control but few indicated whether and Experimental Treatment Groups* level, (18%) RESULTS the value was one tailed or two tailed. Effect Size: Relative Difference In fact, in only 30% of these trials was I I Description of Study Sample there sufficient detail to enable us to Year_25%_50% total 393 were in the calculated 1975 12(16) 25(16) A of RCTs published replicate reported sample 1980 13(15) 47(15) JAMA, Lancet, and the New England size. 1985 7(15) 27(15) Journal Medicine the 4 1990 of during years 25(24) 42(24) of our review. Ten trials were COMMENT Overall 16(70) 36(70) excluded from the analysis for the following rea¬ If a trial with negative results has a "Sample size calculations were based on a two-tailed sons: results based on invalid statistical sufficient sample size to detect a clini¬ a value of .05 with use of either a test orar test, as appropriate for the scale of measurement of the primary analyses precluded classification of the cally important effect, then the nega¬ outcome measure. Values are percentages (number). trial (n=5), randomization was not ex¬ tive results are interpretable—the treat¬ plicit or not all patients were random¬ ment did not have an effect at least as After closely following Frieman and ized (n=2), and no statistical analysis large as the effect considered to be clini¬ colleagues'2 selection criteria, our power was performed (n=3). cally relevant. If a trial with negative calculations were performed on the sub¬ Twenty-seven percent (n=102) of the results has insufficient power, a clini¬ set of the trials with negative results in remaining 383 trials had negative re¬ cally important but statistically nonsig¬ which a simple two-group parallel de¬ sults. Although the number of RCTs nificant effect is usually ignored or, sign was used. However, rather than published has more than doubled be¬ worse, is taken to mean that the treat¬ calculating power only for trials with tween 1975 and 1990 (67 vs 148), the ment under study made no difference. dichotomous outcomes, we also calcu¬ percentage that had negative results has Thus, there are important scientific rea¬ lated power for trials with continuous remained fairly stable over time: 33%, sons to report sample size and/or power primary outcomes. We calculated each 27%, 25%, and 25% in 1975,1980,1985, calculations. trial's power to detect a 25% and a 50% and 1990, respectively. There are also ethical reasons to es¬ relative with an value of timate size when a trial. change, .05, Statistical Power sample planning using a test or a t test, as appropriate Altman5 noted that ethics committees for the scale of measurement of the pri¬ We calculated power for 70 of the 102 may not want to approve the rare over¬ mary outcome. Our calculations differed RCTs with negative results that em¬ sized trial because of the unnecessary from those of Frieman and colleagues in ployed a simple two-group parallel de¬ costs and involvement of additional pa¬ that we employed a two-tailed rather sign with dichotomous (n=52) and con¬ tients. More commonly, ethics commit¬ than a one-tailed a. A standard program3 tinuous (n=18) primary outcomes. The tees may not want to approve trials that was used for our power calculations.
Recommended publications
  • Effect Size (ES)
    Lee A. Becker <http://web.uccs.edu/lbecker/Psy590/es.htm> Effect Size (ES) © 2000 Lee A. Becker I. Overview II. Effect Size Measures for Two Independent Groups 1. Standardized difference between two groups. 2. Correlation measures of effect size. 3. Computational examples III. Effect Size Measures for Two Dependent Groups. IV. Meta Analysis V. Effect Size Measures in Analysis of Variance VI. References Effect Size Calculators Answers to the Effect Size Computation Questions I. Overview Effect size (ES) is a name given to a family of indices that measure the magnitude of a treatment effect. Unlike significance tests, these indices are independent of sample size. ES measures are the common currency of meta-analysis studies that summarize the findings from a specific area of research. See, for example, the influential meta- analysis of psychological, educational, and behavioral treatments by Lipsey and Wilson (1993). There is a wide array of formulas used to measure ES. For the occasional reader of meta-analysis studies, like myself, this diversity can be confusing. One of my objectives in putting together this set of lecture notes was to organize and summarize the various measures of ES. In general, ES can be measured in two ways: a) as the standardized difference between two means, or b) as the correlation between the independent variable classification and the individual scores on the dependent variable. This correlation is called the "effect size correlation" (Rosnow & Rosenthal, 1996). These notes begin with the presentation of the basic ES measures for studies with two independent groups. The issues involved when assessing ES for two dependent groups are then described.
    [Show full text]
  • Statistical Inference Bibliography 1920-Present 1. Pearson, K
    StatisticalInferenceBiblio.doc © 2006, Timothy G. Gregoire, Yale University http://www.yale.edu/forestry/gregoire/downloads/stats/StatisticalInferenceBiblio.pdf Last revised: July 2006 Statistical Inference Bibliography 1920-Present 1. Pearson, K. (1920) “The Fundamental Problem in Practical Statistics.” Biometrika, 13(1): 1- 16. 2. Edgeworth, F.Y. (1921) “Molecular Statistics.” Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, 84(1): 71-89. 3. Fisher, R. A. (1922) “On the Mathematical Foundations of Theoretical Statistics.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series A, Containing Papers of a Mathematical or Physical Character, 222: 309-268. 4. Neyman, J. and E. S. Pearson. (1928) “On the Use and Interpretation of Certain Test Criteria for Purposes of Statistical Inference: Part I.” Biometrika, 20A(1/2): 175-240. 5. Fisher, R. A. (1933) “The Concepts of Inverse Probability and Fiducial Probability Referring to Unknown Parameters.” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series A, Containing Papers of Mathematical and Physical Character, 139(838): 343-348. 6. Fisher, R. A. (1935) “The Logic of Inductive Inference.” Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, 98(1): 39-82. 7. Fisher, R. A. (1936) “Uncertain inference.” Proceedings of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences, 71: 245-258. 8. Berkson, J. (1942) “Tests of Significance Considered as Evidence.” Journal of the American Statistical Association, 37(219): 325-335. 9. Barnard, G. A. (1949) “Statistical Inference.” Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B (Methodological), 11(2): 115-149. 10. Fisher, R. (1955) “Statistical Methods and Scientific Induction.” Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B (Methodological), 17(1): 69-78.
    [Show full text]
  • Effect Sizes (ES) for Meta-Analyses the Standardized Mean Difference
    Kinds of Effect Sizes The effect size (ES) is the DV in the meta analysis. d - standardized mean difference Effect Sizes (ES) for Meta-Analyses – quantitative DV – between groups designs standardized gain score – pre-post differences • ES – d, r/eta & OR – quantitative DV • computing ESs – within-groups design • estimating ESs r – correlation/eta • ESs to beware! – converted from sig test (e.g., F, t, X2)or set of means/stds • interpreting ES – between or within-groups designs or tests of association • ES transformations odds ratio A useful ES: • ES adustments – binary DVs • is standardized – between groups designs • outlier identification Univariate (proportion or mean) • a standard error can be – prevalence rates calculated The Standardized Mean Difference (d) • A Z-like summary statistic that tells the size of the difference between the means of the two groups • Expresses the mean difference in Standard Deviation units – d = 1.00 Tx mean is 1 std larger than Cx mean – d = .50 Tx mean is 1/2 std larger than Cx mean – d = -.33 Tx mean is 1/3 std smaller than Cx mean • Null effect = 0.00 • Range from -∞ to ∞ • Cohen’s effect size categories – small = 0.20 medium = 0.50 large = 0.80 The Standardized Mean Difference (d) Equivalent formulas to calculate The Standardized Mean Difference (d) •Calculate Spooled using MSerror from a 2BG ANOVA √MSerror = Spooled • Represents a standardized group mean difference on an inherently continuous (quantitative) DV. • Uses the pooled standard deviation •Calculate Spooled from F, condition means & ns • There is a wide variety of d-like ESs – not all are equivalent – Some intended as sample descriptions while some intended as population estimates – define and use “n,” “nk” or “N” in different ways – compute the variability of mean difference differently – correct for various potential biases Equivalent formulas to calculate The Standardized Mean Difference (d) • Calculate d directly from significance tests – t or F • Calculate t or F from exact p-value & df.
    [Show full text]
  • One-Way Analysis of Variance F-Tests Using Effect Size
    PASS Sample Size Software NCSS.com Chapter 597 One-Way Analysis of Variance F-Tests using Effect Size Introduction A common task in research is to compare the averages of two or more populations (groups). We might want to compare the income level of two regions, the nitrogen content of three lakes, or the effectiveness of four drugs. The one-way analysis of variance compares the means of two or more groups to determine if at least one mean is different from the others. The F test is used to determine statistical significance. F tests are non-directional in that the null hypothesis specifies that all means are equal, and the alternative hypothesis simply states that at least one mean is different from the rest. The methods described here are usually applied to the one-way experimental design. This design is an extension of the design used for the two-sample t test. Instead of two groups, there are three or more groups. In our more advanced one-way ANOVA procedure, you are required to enter hypothesized means and variances. This simplified procedure only requires the input of an effect size, usually f, as proposed by Cohen (1988). Assumptions Using the F test requires certain assumptions. One reason for the popularity of the F test is its robustness in the face of assumption violation. However, if an assumption is not even approximately met, the significance levels and the power of the F test are invalidated. Unfortunately, in practice it often happens that several assumptions are not met. This makes matters even worse.
    [Show full text]
  • NBER TECHNICAL WORKING PAPER SERIES USING RANDOMIZATION in DEVELOPMENT ECONOMICS RESEARCH: a TOOLKIT Esther Duflo Rachel Glenner
    NBER TECHNICAL WORKING PAPER SERIES USING RANDOMIZATION IN DEVELOPMENT ECONOMICS RESEARCH: A TOOLKIT Esther Duflo Rachel Glennerster Michael Kremer Technical Working Paper 333 http://www.nber.org/papers/t0333 NATIONAL BUREAU OF ECONOMIC RESEARCH 1050 Massachusetts Avenue Cambridge, MA 02138 December 2006 We thank the editor T.Paul Schultz, as well Abhijit Banerjee, Guido Imbens and Jeffrey Kling for extensive discussions, David Clingingsmith, Greg Fischer, Trang Nguyen and Heidi Williams for outstanding research assistance, and Paul Glewwe and Emmanuel Saez, whose previous collaboration with us inspired parts of this chapter. The views expressed herein are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Bureau of Economic Research. © 2006 by Esther Duflo, Rachel Glennerster, and Michael Kremer. All rights reserved. Short sections of text, not to exceed two paragraphs, may be quoted without explicit permission provided that full credit, including © notice, is given to the source. Using Randomization in Development Economics Research: A Toolkit Esther Duflo, Rachel Glennerster, and Michael Kremer NBER Technical Working Paper No. 333 December 2006 JEL No. C93,I0,J0,O0 ABSTRACT This paper is a practical guide (a toolkit) for researchers, students and practitioners wishing to introduce randomization as part of a research design in the field. It first covers the rationale for the use of randomization, as a solution to selection bias and a partial solution to publication biases. Second, it discusses various ways in which randomization can be practically introduced in a field settings. Third, it discusses designs issues such as sample size requirements, stratification, level of randomization and data collection methods.
    [Show full text]
  • Effect Size Reporting Among Prominent Health Journals: a Case Study of Odds Ratios
    Original research BMJ EBM: first published as 10.1136/bmjebm-2020-111569 on 10 December 2020. Downloaded from Effect size reporting among prominent health journals: a case study of odds ratios Brian Chu ,1 Michael Liu,2 Eric C Leas,3 Benjamin M Althouse,4 John W Ayers5 10.1136/bmjebm-2020-111569 ABSTRACT Summary box Background The accuracy of statistical reporting 1 that informs medical and public health practice Perelman School of What is already known about this has generated extensive debate, but no studies Medicine, University of subject? Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, have evaluated the frequency or accuracy of ► Although odds ratios (ORs) are Pennsylvania, USA effect size (the magnitude of change in outcome frequently used, it is unknown 2University of Oxford, Oxford, as a function of change in predictor) reporting in whether ORs are reported correctly in UK prominent health journals. 3 peer- reviewed articles in prominent Department of Family Objective To evaluate effect size reporting Medicine and Public Health, health journals. practices in prominent health journals using the Division of Health Policy, University of California San case study of ORs. What are the new findings? Diego, La Jolla, California, USA Design Articles published in the American ► In a random sample of 400 articles 4Epidemiology, Institute for Journal of Public Health (AJPH), Journal of the from four journals, reporting of Disease Modeling, Bellevue, American Medical Association (JAMA), New ORs frequently omitted reporting Washington, USA England Journal of Medicine (NEJM) and PLOS effect sizes (magnitude of change 5 Department of Medicine, One from 1 January 2010 through 31 December in outcome as a function of change Division of Infectious 2019 mentioning the term ‘odds ratio’ in all in predictor).
    [Show full text]
  • Methods Research Report: Empirical Evidence of Associations Between
    Methods Research Report Empirical Evidence of Associations Between Trial Quality and Effect Size Methods Research Report Empirical Evidence of Associations Between Trial Quality and Effect Size Prepared for: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality U.S. Department of Health and Human Services 540 Gaither Road Rockville, MD 20850 http://www.ahrq.gov Contract No. HHSA 290-2007-10062-I Prepared by: Southern California Evidence-based Practice Center Santa Monica, CA Investigators: Susanne Hempel, Ph.D. Marika J. Suttorp, M.S. Jeremy N.V. Miles, Ph.D. Zhen Wang, M.S. Margaret Maglione, M.P.P. Sally Morton, Ph.D. Breanne Johnsen, B.A. Diane Valentine, J.D. Paul G. Shekelle, M.D., Ph.D. AHRQ Publication No. 11-EHC045-EF June 2011 This report is based on research conducted by the Southern California Evidence-based Practice Center (EPC) under contract to the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ), Rockville, MD (Contract No. 290-2007-10062-I). The findings and conclusions in this document are those of the author(s), who are responsible for its contents; the findings and conclusions do not necessarily represent the views of AHRQ. Therefore, no statement in this report should be construed as an official position of AHRQ or of the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. The information in this report is intended to help health care decisionmakers—patients and clinicians, health system leaders, and policymakers, among others—make well-informed decisions and thereby improve the quality of health care services. This report is not intended to be a substitute for the application of clinical judgment.
    [Show full text]
  • Effect Size and Eta Squared James Dean Brown (University of Hawai‘I at Manoa)
    Shiken: JALT Testing & Evaluation SIG Newsletter. 12 (2) April 2008 (p. 38 - 43) Statistics Corner Questions and answers about language testing statistics: Effect size and eta squared James Dean Brown (University of Hawai‘i at Manoa) Question: In Chapter 6 of the 2008 book on heritage language learning that you co- edited with Kimi-Kondo Brown, a study comparing how three different groups of informants use intersentential referencing is outlined. On page 147 of that book, a MANOVA with a partial eta2 of .29 is outlined. There are several questions about this statistic. What does a “partial eta” measure? Are there other forms of eta that readers should know about? And how should one interpret a partial eta2 value of .29? Answer: I will answer your question about partial eta2 in two parts. I will start by defining and explaining eta2. Then I will circle back and do the same for partial eta2. Eta2 Eta2 can be defined as the proportion of variance associated with or accounted for by each of the main effects, interactions, and error in an ANOVA study (see Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001, pp. 54-55, and Thompson, 2006, pp. 317-319). Formulaically, eta2, or η 2 , is defined as follows: SS η 2 = effect SStotal Where: SSeffect = the sums of squares for whatever effect is of interest SStotal = the total sums of squares for all effects, interactions, and errors in the ANOVA Eta2 is most often reported for straightforward ANOVA designs that (a) are balanced (i.e., have equal cell sizes) and (b) have independent cells (i.e., different people appear in each cell).
    [Show full text]
  • How to Work with a Biostatistician for Power and Sample Size Calculations
    How to Work with a Biostatistician for Power and Sample Size Calculations Amber W. Trickey, PhD, MS, CPH Senior Biostatistician, S-SPIRE 1070 Arastradero #225 [email protected] Goal: Effective Statistical Collaboration Essentially all models are wrong but some are useful George E. P. Box Topics Research • Questions & Measures Fundamentals • Hypothesis Testing Power and • Estimation Parameters Sample Size • Assumptions Statistical • Communication Collaboration • Ethical Considerations Pro Tips Health Services/Surgical Outcomes Research Patient/Disease Hospital Health Policy • Comparative Effectiveness • Disparities Research • Policy Evaluation • Meta-analyses • Quality Measurement • Cost Effectiveness Analysis • Patient-centered Outcomes • Quality Improvement • Workforce • Decision Analysis • Patient Safety • Implementation Science [Ban 2016: Is Health Services Research Important to Surgeons?] Where to Begin? Research Question! Research Question (PICO) Patient population • Condition / disease, demographics, setting, time Intervention • Procedure, policy, process, treatment Comparison/Control group • No treatment, standard of care, non-exposed Outcome of interest • Treatment effects, patient-centered outcomes, healthcare utilization Example Research Question • Do hospitals with 200+ beds perform better than smaller hospitals? More developed question: specify population & outcome • Do large California hospitals with 200+ beds have lower surgical site infection rates for adults undergoing inpatient surgical procedures? • Population: California
    [Show full text]
  • Effect Size and Statistical Power Remember Type I and II Errors
    Dr. Robergs Fall, 2008 Effect Size and Statistical Power Prior to conducting the study (apriori), researchers should; • estimate the size of a mean difference that is meaningful • identify a type I error probability that is acceptable to them and the study/DV’s. • identify a type II error probability that is acceptable to them and the study/DV’s. • estimate the sample size needed to detect this mean difference, given the aforementioned type I and type II errors. “With a large enough sample size we can detect even a very small difference between the value of the population parameter stated in the null hypothesis and the true value, but the difference may be of no practical importance. (Conversely, with too small a sample size, a researcher may have little chance to detect an important difference.) PEP507: Research Methods Remember Type I and II Errors Type I Error: Probability of rejecting Ho when Ho is true (α) Stating that there is a difference when there really is not!!! Type II Error: Probability of retaining Ho when Ho is false (β) Stating that there is no difference Null Hypothesis when there really is!!! Reject Accept Yes correct Type II Mean error Difference No Type I correct error PEP507: Research Methods Effect Size 7 Stat Power 1 Dr. Robergs Fall, 2008 Effect Size and Statistical Power The Power of a test The probability of correctly rejecting a false Ho. Power = 1 - β Probability of type II error PEP507: Research Methods Factors Affecting Power 1. Size of the effect PEP507: Research Methods Effect Size 7 Stat Power 2 Dr.
    [Show full text]
  • Some Practical Guidelines for Effective Sample-Size Determination
    Some Practical Guidelines for Effective Sample-Size Determination Russell V. Lenth∗ Department of Statistics University of Iowa March 1, 2001 Abstract Sample-size determination is often an important step in planning a statistical study—and it is usually a difficult one. Among the important hurdles to be surpassed, one must obtain an estimate of one or more error variances, and specify an effect size of importance. There is the temptation to take some shortcuts. This paper offers some suggestions for successful and meaningful sample-size determination. Also discussed is the possibility that sample size may not be the main issue, that the real goal is to design a high-quality study. Finally, criticism is made of some ill-advised shortcuts relating to power and sample size. Key words: Power; Sample size; Observed power; Retrospective power; Study design; Cohen’s effect measures; Equivalence testing; ∗I wish to thank John Castelloe, Kate Cowles, Steve Simon, two referees, an editor, and an associate editor for their helpful comments on earlier drafts of this paper. Much of this work was done with the support of the Obermann Center for Advanced Studies at the University of Iowa. 1 1 Sample size and power Statistical studies (surveys, experiments, observational studies, etc.) are always better when they are care- fully planned. Good planning has many aspects. The problem should be carefully defined and operational- ized. Experimental or observational units must be selected from the appropriate population. The study must be randomized correctly. The procedures must be followed carefully. Reliable instruments should be used to obtain measurements. Finally, the study must be of adequate size, relative to the goals of the study.
    [Show full text]
  • Meta-Analysis
    Meta-Analysis Susan Stewart, Ph.D. Division of Biostatistics Department of Public Health Sciences May, 2017 Acknowledgements Jimmy Hwang, Ph.D. UCSF Biostatistics Core 2 Topics Covered • Definitions: systematic review and meta-analysis • PRISMA checklist: guide to good practice • Example 1: Recently published meta-analysis • Example 2: Meta-analysis calculations 3 Systematic Review • Attempts to collate all empirical evidence that fits pre- specified eligibility criteria in order to answer a specific research question. • Uses explicit, systematic methods that are selected with a view to minimizing bias, thus providing more reliable findings from which conclusions can be drawn and decisions made. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. Version 5.1.0. Updated March 2011. 4 Key Characteristics • Clearly stated set of objectives with pre-defined eligibility criteria for studies • Explicit, reproducible methodology • Systematic search that attempts to identify all studies that would meet the eligibility criteria • Assessment of the validity of the findings of the included studies • Systematic presentation and synthesis of the characteristics and findings of the included studies. 5 What is meta-analysis? • Meta-analysis is the use of statistical methods to summarize the results of independent studies. – Many systematic reviews contain meta- analyses, but not all. – Can provide more precise estimates than individual studies. • Purpose: arrive at conclusions about the body of research 6 The Logic of Meta-Analysis • Traditional
    [Show full text]