Quick viewing(Text Mode)

Hippocampal and Amygdaloid Interactions in the Nucleus Accumbens

Hippocampal and Amygdaloid Interactions in the Nucleus Accumbens

Psychobiology 1999,27 (2), 149-164

Hippocampal and amygdaloid interactions in the

HENKJ.GROENEWEGEN Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

ANTONIUS B. MULDER University ofAmsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

ARNO V. J. BEIJER and CHRISTOPHER I. WRIGHT Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

FERNANDO H. WPES DA SILVA University ofAmsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands and CYRIEL M. A PENNARTZ Netherlands Institute for Research, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

The nucleus accumbens, in view of its afferent and efferent fiber connections, appears to hold a key position for "limbic" (e.g., hippocampal and amygdaloid) influences to reach somatomotor and auto­ nomic brain structures, and it has therefore been considered as a limbic-motor interface. The nucleus accumbens can be subdivided into a shell and a core region, which both contain further inhomo­ geneities. The present account first summarizes the detailed topographical anatomical relationships of inputs from different dorso-ventral parts of the hippocampus and different rostrocaudal parts of the basal amygdaloid complex at the level of the accumbens. Subsequently, the electrophysiological char­ acteristics of hippocampal and amygdaloid inputs in the accumbens are described. Interactions be­ tween hippocampal and amygdaloid inputs appear to exist primarily in the medial parts of both the shell and the core of the nucleus accumbens. In the short term, stimulating amygdaloid inputs appear to facilitate hippocampal throughput (heterosynaptic paired pulse facilitation), whereas stimulation of hippocampal inputs depresses amygdaloid throughput in a paired pulse paradigm. Tetanic stimulation of hippocampal inputs to the accumbens leads to a decrementallong-term potentiation (LTP) of this fiber pathway (homosynaptic LTP) but, along a similar time range, to a depression of amygdaloid in­ puts (heterosynaptic long-term depression). The involvement of dopaminergic, GABAergic, and gluta­ matergic mechanisms in these interactions is discussed. Finally, it is suggested that the interactions be­ tween hippocampal and amygdaloid inputs at the level of the nucleus accumbens playa role in different aspects of associative learning.

The nucleus accumbens (Acb) is a brain area located , which further includes the ventromedial parts in the rostroventral part of the . Following of the caudate- complex and the striatal elements the seminal paper by Heimer and Wilson in 1975, the Acb of the . The ventral striatum in general, has been considered a component of the so-called ventral and the Acb in particular, is thought to be involved in var­ ious complex behavioral functions, including sensori­ motor, motivational, and adaptational processes (Cador, The authors wish to thank Wil 1. A. J. Smeets for his critical comments Robbins, & Everitt, 1989; Everitt, Morris, O'Brien, & on the paper, Martijne Mendes-de Leon and Jolinda Kos for secretarial Robbins, 1991; Groenewegen, Berendse, Wolters, & Loh­ assistance, and Dirk de Jong for his photographic contribution. A.B.M. man, 1990; Groenewegen, Wright, & Beijer, 1996; Mogen­ was in the Institute for Neurobiology at the University of Amsterdam son, Jones, & Yim, 1980; Pennartz, Groenewegen, & at the time of this research; he is now at the Netherlands Institute for Lopes da Silva, 1994; Scheel-KrUger & Willner, 1991; Brain Research. A.Y.1.B. is in the Research Institute , De­ partment of Anatomy at the Vrije Universiteit, as was C.1. W. when this Zahm & Brog, 1992). Further, the Acb plays a prominent research was done; C.I.w. is now at the Department of , Har­ role in reward learning, and this nucleus has been impli­ vard Medical School, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Boston. F.H.L.S. cated in and other affective disorders, as well is in the Institute for Neurobiology at the University of Amsterdam. as in drug abuse (Koob, 1992; Robbins & Everitt, 1996). Correspondence should be addressed to H. 1. Groenewegen, Depart­ ment of Anatomy, Faculty of Medicine, Vrije Universiteit, Van der Boe­ In terms offiber connections, the Acb is characterized chorststraat 7, 1081 BT Amsterdam, the Netherlands (e-mail: hj.groe­ by strong inputs from -related structures, such [email protected]). as the , basal amygdaloid complex,

149 Copyright 1999 Psychonomic Society, Inc. 150 GROENEWEGEN ET AL. parahippocampal cortex, and anterior . (Groenewegen et aI., 1987). Amygdalostriatal projections Other inputs are derived from the , the have a much more widespread distribution than the hippo­ midline thalamic nuclei, the dopaminergic ventral teg­ campostriatal fibers and include extensive parts of the mental area and the serotonergic median raphe nucleus caudate-putamen complex (Kelley et aI., 1982; Kita & (Brog, Salyapongse, Deutch, & Zahm, 1993; Groenewe­ Kitai, 1990; Wright et aI., 1996). However, the Acb is gen, Becker, & Lohman, 1980; Groenewegen, Room, Wit­ reached by almost all parts ofthe basal amygdaloid com­ ter, & Lohman, 1982; Groenewegen, Vermeulen-Van der plex in a highly topographical way (Wright et aI., 1996). Zee, te Kortschot, & Witter, 1987; Kelley & Domesick, Thus, the Acb is a main potential convergence site of hip­ 1982; Kelley, Domesick, & Nauta, 1982; Newman & pocampal and amygdaloid influences on the basal gan­ Winans, 1980; Phillipson & Griffiths, 1985; Totterdell glia and, through the output of these structures, on be­ & Meredith, 1997). It projects, in turn, to various behav­ havioral functions. The hippocampal formation and ioral effector regions such as the ventral pallidum, the lat­ are thought to be involved in different aspects eral , the , the sub­ of certain behaviors. The amygdala, in forming stimulus­ stantia nigra pars compacta and caudal mesencephalic reward and stimulus-punishment associations, serves as areas, including the so-called mesencephalic locomotor a link between sensory systems and structures involved region, and adjacent, lateral parts of the central gray sub­ in the expression of emotional behavior (Adolphs, Tranel, stance (Berendse, Groenewegen, & Lohman, 1992; Damasio, & Damasio, 1995; Davies, 1992; Everitt et aI., Heimer, Zahm, & Alheid, 1995; Heimer, Zahm, Churchill, 1991; LeDoux, 1993). The hippocampal formation is im­ Kalivas, & Wohltmann, 1991; Nauta, Smith, Faull, & portant for functions, particularly those involving Domesick, 1978). The Acb appears to hold a key position spatial cues (Alvarez, Zola-Morgan, & Squire, 1995; Zola­ in the pathways by which motivational and emotional in­ Morgan, Squire, Alvarez-Royo, & Clower, 1991; Zola­ fluences reach somatomotor and autonomic brain struc­ Morgan, Squire, & Amaral, 1986, 1989). Interestingly, tures. In this respect, the nucleus has been considered a manipulations of either the hippocampal or the amygda­ limbic-motor interface (Groenewegen et aI., 1996; Mo­ loid systems, through the Acb, have led to different, and genson et aI., 1980). in some instances opposing, effects on behavior (e.g., lo­ The Acb has long been treated as a homogeneous struc­ comotor activity; for a review, see Pennartz et aI., 1994). ture, but recently it has been recognized that the Acb con­ In view of the topographical organization of both the sists of various subdivisions. Most notably, a peripherally hippocampal and amygdaloid inputs to the Acb, the in­ located "shell" and a centrally located "core" region have trinsic heterogeneity of the nucleus, and the differential been recognized (Voorn, Gerfen, & Groenewegen, 1989; behavioral roles of the hippocampal formation and amyg­ Zaborszky et aI., 1985; Zahm & Brog, 1992). This bipar­ dala, the present paper provides a brief review of the ana­ tition of the nucleus is primarily based on the differential tomical and physiological relationships between these histochemical characteristics of the shell and core. Results two limbic inputs at the level of the Acb. Unless otherwise of neuroanatomical tracing studies indicate that the var­ specified, the descriptions below relate to data obtained ious afferent systems ofthe Acb appear to be inhomoge­ in rats. neously distributed over the nucleus, forming an intricate pattern that to a certain degree is related to the shell-core ANATOMICAL RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN subdivision. Likewise, populations of output that HIPPOCAMPAL AND AMYGDALOID project to the various targets of the Acb appear to be in­ INPUTS IN THE NUCLEUS ACCUMBENS homogeneously distributed over the nucleus (Berendse et aI., 1992; Groenewegen et aI., 1996; Heimer et aI., 1997; Immunohistochemical and Cytoarchitectonic Herkenham, Moon-Edley, & Stuart, 1984). Moreover, re­ Framework of the Nucleus Accumbens sults of numerous pharmacological and behavioral studies The differential distribution of immunoreactivity for have revealed that there are major functional differences the calciumbinding protein Calbindin DZ8K (CaB) pro­ between Acb shell and core (e.g., Deutch & Cameron, vides the generally accepted means of subdividing the 1992; Kelley, Smith-Roe, & Holahan, 1997; Parkinson, Acb into a shell and a core region (longen-Relo, Voorn, Olmstead, Bums, Robbins, & Everitt, 1999; Stratford & & Groenewegen, 1994; Zahm & Brog, 1992; Figure 1). Kelley, 1997; Weiner, Gal, Rawlins, & Feldon, 1996). For the subsequent description of the termination pat­ One of the most distinguishing features of the Acb is terns of hippocampal and amygdaloid inputs, as well as that this part of the striatum receives the major input from for the location of the recording electrodes in the Acb, the hippocampal formation (Groenewegen et aI., 1982; we have used the pattern of immunoreactivity of CaB as a Groenewegen et aI., 1987; Kelley & Domesick, 1982). reference. In the following paragraph, a brief description Relatively minor projections have been described from of the Acb cytoarchitecture and the differential distribu­ ventral and dorsal parts of the hippocampal formation to tion of CaB immunoreactivity over the nucleus is given. the most ventral parts of the caudal half of the striatum, As shown in Figure 1, the centrally located core is in encompassing the interstitial nucleus ofthe posterior limb general strongly immunoreactive for CaB, whereas the of the anterior commissure (lPAC) and the extreme cau­ medially, ventrally, and, in part, laterally located shell has dal part of the caudate-putamen complex, respectively a much lower level of CaB immunoreactivity. CaB im- HIPPOCAMPAL-AMYGDALOID INTERACTIONS IN THE ACCUMBENS lSI

Figure 1. Photomicrographs of four transverse sections through the Acb, immunostained for CaB. A, rostral; D, caudal. Arrowheads in B-D indicate the border between shell (Sh) and core (C) of the Acb; small dots in C and D mark the border between the septal nuclei and the Acb. Note that the shell shows much less CaB-immunoreactivity than the core but that the shell is inhomogeneous in itself, exhibiting moderate immunoreactivity in the lateral shell and almost no immunoreactivity for CaB in medial shell. The core contains patches of light or moderate levels of immunoreactivity for CaB. The rostral part of the Acb (A), with the exception of a lateral region, is lightly immunoreactive for CaB and has been included in the shell on the basis of this characteristic (Jongen-Relo, Voorn, & Groenewegen, 1994). ac, anterior commissure; Se, septal nuclei. munoreactivity is very low in the rostral part of the Acb lateral part of the Acb (Figure I C, I D). Although it can (Figure IA). The medial and ventral rim of the nucleus be stated in general terms that the shell exhibits low lev­ shows a higher level of CaB-immunoreactivity, whereas els of CaB-immunoreactivity and the core high levels of its most ventrolateral part exhibits a high intensity ofim­ this protein, both subregions are heterogeneous with re­ munoreactivity for CaB, comparable with that in the Acb spect to this and many other neuroactive substances. core (Figure IB-ID). The largest, CaB-poor, part of the Thus, in the caudal Acb the medial part of the shell is very rostral Acb has been included in the shell compartment low in CaB immunoreactivity with immunonegative areas, by Jongen-Relo et al. (1994) on the basis that, as seen in which coincide with cell clusters in Nissl-stained sec­ horizontal sections (cf. Figure 4 in Jongen-Relo et aI., tions and an area containing CaB-positive neurons in the 1994), this part of the nucleus is merely continuous with dorsomedial shell (Herkenham et aI., 1984; Wright & the more caudal parts of the CaB-poor shell (Figure I B­ Groenewegen, 1995). At the same levels, the ventral shell I C). Zahm and Heimer (1993) have shown that the rostral contains areas of moderate CaB immunoreactivity, part of the Acb has efferent connectional characteristics whereas the lateral shell exhibits a more homogeneous of both the shell and the core subdivisions, and they pro­ distribution of moderate CaB immunoreactivity (Fig­ posed calling this part of the Acb the "rostral pole." ure IC, ID). Within the Acb core, patches of moderate At more caudal levels (Figure IB-ID), the CaB-poor or low CaB immunoreactivity are present that are similar shell first occupies the medial and ventral parts of the to those in the ventromedial parts of the caudate-putamen nucleus (Figure 1B) and extends further caudally into the complex (Figure lB-lD). 152 GROENEWEGEN ET AL.

Figure 2. Photomicrographs of transverse sections through the Acb stained for Nissl substance (A, rostral; B, caudal) and myelinated fibers (C, rostral; D, caudal). Note the existence of clusters of neurons (arrows in A). In B, arrowheads mark the border between sheD (Sh) and core (C); small dots indicate the border between the septal nuclei and the Acb. ac, ante­ rior commissure; CPu, caudate-putamen complex; OT, olfactory tubercle; Se, septal nuclei. Bar in D = 1.0 mm.

Cytoarchitectonically, the Acb seems at first glance as tion of (Meredith, Pennartz, & Groenewe­ a rather homogeneous area (Figure 2A, 2B). Further­ gen, 1993). However, a more detailed study of the cyto­ more, the region of the Acb is largely devoid of myeli­ architecture shows that cells are not equally dispersed nated fibers, in contrast to the caudate-putamen complex, over the nucleus; they form clusters of more densely which, in rats, is characterized by multiple bundles of in­ packed cells in certain parts of the nucleus (Figure 2A, ternal capsule fibers (Figures 2C, 2D). The great major­ 2B; Chronister, Sikes, Trow, & DeFrance, 1981; Herken­ ity of the neurons in the Acb are of the medium-sized, ham et aI., 1984; Voorn et aI., 1989; for a review on the densely spiny type that form the output neurons of the ventral striatum, see Heimer et aI., in press). It is nucleus. Only a small percentage (approximately 5%) is of interest to note that the cellular density is in general larger, and these neurons form a heterogeneous popula- lower in the shell than in the core and that the shell-core HIPPOCAMPAL-AMYGDALOID INTERACTIONS IN THE ACCUMBENS 153 boundary, at least in the caudomedial part of the Acb, is is such that the ventral projects most densely marked by distinct cell clusters (Jongen-Relo, Groene­ to the caudomedial part of the Acb, whereas progressively wegen, & Voom, 1993; Jongen-Rel0 et aI., 1994; Mere­ more dorsal parts of the subiculum send fibers to success­ dith et aI., 1993; Wright & Groenewegen, 1995, 1996). ively more lateral and rostral parts of the Acb (Brog et aI., Therefore, in well-stained Nissl material, it is in princi­ 1993; Groenewegen et aI., 1987). The topography in the ple possible to distinguish between shell and core. projections from the basal amygdaloid complex to the Acb is rather complex, but a general trend is that caudal The Topographical Relationships of Hippocampal parts ofthe amygdala project medially in the Acb, whereas and Amygdaloid Afferents to the Acb more rostral parts of the amygdala send fibers to more The projections of the hippocampal formation and the lateral parts of the nucleus (McDonald, 1991; Shin onaga basal amygdaloid complex have been mapped in consid­ et aI., 1994; Wright et aI., 1996). Since the ventral and dor­ erable detail in previous antrograde and retrograde neuro­ sal hippocampus, as well as the caudal and rostral parts anatomical tracing studies (rat, Brog et aI., 1993; Groene­ of the basal amygdaloid complex, may sub serve differ­ wegen et aI., 1987; Kelley & Domesick, 1982; Kelley ent functional roles, it is of significant interest to iden­ et aI., 1982; Kita & Kitai, 1990; McDonald, 1991; Shino­ tify precisely their areas of termination within the Acb and naga, Takada, & Mizuno, 1994; Wright et aI., 1996; cat, to determine whether they remain segregated or con­ Groenewegen et aI., 1980; Groenewegen et aI., 1982; verge. In the following sections, the topographical arrange­ Ragsdale & Graybiel, 1988; monkey, Russchen, Bakst, ment of the hippocampal and amygdaloid projections to Amaral, & Price, 1985). Both afferent systems exhibit a the Acb is described using a series of four transverse stan­ clear topographical relationship in their projections to dard sections of the Acb (drawings based on the CaB­ the Acb. For the projections from the hippocampal for­ stained sections shown in Figure 1) onto which the label­ mation, in particular from the subiculum, the topography ing resulting from anterograde tracer injections in different

Figure 3. Schematic representation of the location of the injection sites of anterograde tracers in the subiculum of the hippocampal formation (Sub) and the basal amygdaloid complex (BAC). The resultant anterograde labeling in the Acb is shown in Figure 4. ABmg, magnoceUular accessory basal nucleus; ADpc, parviceUular AD; cBpc, cau­ dal part of the parviceUular basal nucleus; Ce, central nucleus; cBmg, caudal part of the magnoceUular basal nu­ cleus; dSub, dorsal subiculum; La, lateral nucleus; rBmg, rostral part of the Bmg; rBpc, rostral part of the Bpc; vSub, ventral subiculum. 154 GROENEWEGEN ET AL. parts of the hippocampal formation and the amygdala, I, II), termination of fibers from the parvicellular basal placed in different animals, is converted. The data pre­ amygdaloid nucleus is very scarce and restricted to the sented here are primarily based on the results of experi­ extreme dorsomedial corner of the shell. ments reported by Groenewegen et aI. (1987) for the pro­ Intermediate rostrocaudallevels of the basal amygda­ jections from the hippocampal formation and by Wright loid complex project more laterally and ventrally in the et aI. (1996) for those from the basal amygdaloid com­ Acb. The red label in Figure 4B represents the distribu­ plex. The reader is referred to these papers for details of tion of fibers and terminals in the Acb following an in­ the experimental procedures. jection in the caudal part of the magnocellular basal nu­ Topography of subicular projections to the Acb. cleus (Bmg; Figure 3). These projections are mostly The topographical arrangement of the projections from directed toward ventromedial parts of the shell through­ different parts of the subiculum along the septotemporal out the rostrocaudal extent of the nucleus (Figure 4BI axis ofthe hippocampal formation is summarized in Fig­ I-IV). Additional projections are present in the patches ures 3 and 4NI-IY. Thus, the distribution of fibers from of the medial core, as well as in the medial part of the ol­ three injection sites of an anterograde tracer (either factory tubercle. Injections at the same intermediate ros­ Phaseolus vulgaris-Ieucoagglutinin [PHA-L] or biotiny­ trocaudal levels of the amygdala, but located in the lated dextranamide [BDA]) in the subiculum (Figure 3) accessory basal nucleus, result in a distribution offibers is shown at four different rostrocaudallevels of the Acb. and terminals in the same parts of the shell and core as Ventral subicular fibers (blue in Figure 4) ascend described above for the parvicellular basal amygdaloid through the precommissural via the lateral part of nucleus. In the Acb core, however, the projections from the septum to massively enter the caudomedial part of the accessory basal nucleus are directed toward the ma­ the Acb (Figure 4NIV). The ventral subicular fibers are trix compartment as opposed to the parvicellular basal predominantly directed toward the medial shell, but they amygdaloid fibers, which target the patches (see Wright extend also into the medial part of the core of the Acb et aI., 1996). and the extreme ventromedial part of the caudate-puta­ The rostral part of the basal amygdaloid complex, rep­ men complex (Figure 4NII-IV). Whereas in the caudal resented by the rostral part of the magnocellular basal Acb the ventral subicular fibers occupy almost the entire nucleus (Figure 3), sends projections (green in Figure 4B) medial shell, at more rostral levels that termination area to the lateral shell of the Acb, the patches ofthe lateral core, becomes more and more restricted to the dorsomedial and the lateral part of the olfactory tubercle (Figure 4BI part of the shell and adjacent core (Figure 4NI, II). I-IV). In the lateral part ofthe core, fibers and terminals Fibers originating from an injection in the intermedi­ are also present in the matrix compartment, although to ate septotemporal part of the subiculum (Figure 3) ascend a lesser extent than in the patches (Figure 4BIII-IV). through more medial parts of the septum than those from Hippocampal-amygdaloid relationships. As can be the ventral subiculum. The intermediate subicular fibers deduced from a comparison of Figures 4A and 4B, the (red in Figure 4A) terminate primarily in the rostral half distribution of hippocampal inputs to the Acb is more re­ of the Acb in the ventromedial and rostral shell. Fewer stricted than that from the amygdala. Whereas the fibers projections are found in the ventromedial shell in the from the basal amygdaloid complex reach virtually all caudal Acb, as well as in the ventral parts of the core parts of the shell and core of the Acb, hippocampal pro­ (Figure 4A/I-III). jections are restricted to the medial parts of both the shell The dorsal part of the subiculum (Figure 3) sends and the core in the caudal half of the Acb. In the rostral fibers (green in Figure 4A) via the dorsal fornix (Groene­ half of the Acb, hippocampal fibers, in addition, extend wegen et aI., 1987) through the most medial part of the into the lateral parts of the shell and, very restrictedly, septum to rostrolateral and rostroventral parts of the Acb, the ventrolateral part of the core. The central and lateral in particular its shell compartment (Figure 4NI, II). Only parts of the core do not receive substantial numbers of di­ a few fibers are present in the lateral shell in the caudal rect hippocampal afferents. These observations, based on halfofthe Acb. In the rostral Acb, fibers from the dorsal anterograde tracing, are in good agreement with the re­ subiculum extend into the lateral core (Figure 4NI, II). sults of retrograde tracing experiments with small injec­ Topography of basal amygdaloid projections to the tions in different parts of the Acb (Brog et aI., 1993; Phil­ Acb. Amygdaloid fibers enter the Acb either from a cau­ lipson & Griffiths, 1985). The latter studies further show dal direction (from caudal parts of the basal amygdaloid that hippocampal fibers to the Acb arise not only from the complex) via the and its bed nucleus or subiculum but also from neurons in CA I, adjacent to the from a lateral direction (from rostral parts of the basal subiculum (Brog et aI., 1993; Groenewegen et aI., 1987). amygdaloid complex) via the (see also Convergence of hippocampal and amygdaloid inputs Wright et aI., 1996). occurs primarily in the medial shell and core, as well as The caudal part of the parvicellular basal amygdaloid in more lateral parts of both Acb subdivisions in the ros­ nucleus (Bpc; Figure 3) sends fibers (blue in Figure 4B) tral half of the nucleus. However, as noted, inputs from primarily to the caudomedial part of the shell of the Acb specific parts of the hippocampus converge with inputs as well as to the adjacent medial part of the core (Fig­ from particular subnuclei (or parts thereof) of the basal ure 4BIII, IV). In the rostral half of the Acb (Figure 4BI amygdaloid complex. Thus, the ventral hippocampus and HIPPOCAMPAL-AMYGDALOID INTERACTIONS IN THE ACCUMBENS 155

Figure 4. Schematic representation of the distribution of the anterograde labeUng in the Acb following injections in different parts ofthe hippocampal formation (All-IV) and the basal amygdaloid complex (BII-IV). The drawings of the Acb are based on the sections stained for CaB shown in Figure 1. Fibers and terminals originating from the ventral subiculum (All-IV) and those from the caudal part ofthe parvicellular basal amygdaloid nucleus (BII-IV) are represented in blue, those from the intermediate part of the subicu­ lum (All-IV) and from the midrostrocaudal amygdala (BII-IV) are shown in red, and the fibers and terminals from the dorsal subiculum (All-IV) and the rostral part of the mag­ nocellular basal amygdaloid nucleus (B I-IV) are depicted in green. Note that the projec­ tions from the midrostrocaudal amygdala depicted here are derived from an injection site in the caudal part ofthe magnocellular basal nucleus (cBmg). This nucleus projects in the Acb core and the adjacent ventral part of the caudate-putamen complex to the so-called striatal patch compartment. Projections from the accessory basal nucleus, located at ap­ proximately the same rostrocaudallevel in the amygdala, reach the same region ofthe shell and core ofthe Acb, but in the core target primarily the striatal matrix compartment (for details, see Wright, Beijer, & Groenewegen, 1996). Note that the area of the Acb with con­ verging hippocampal and amygdaloid projections includes the medial shell and core in the caudal half of the nucleus, and the ventral and lateral shell as well as the ventral core in the rostral half of the Acb. ac, anterior commissure; AOp, posterior part of the anterior olfactory nucleus; ccg, genu of the corpus callosum; ILA, infralimbic area; lot, lateral ol­ factory tract; LV, lateral ventricle; OT, olfactory tubercle; S, septum; TTd, tenia tecta. 156 GROENEWEGEN ET AL. the caudal part ofthe basal amygdaloid complex converge basal amygdaloid complex in the Acb, these projections largely in the caudomedial shell and, to a lesser degree were studied simultaneously in rats under halothane in the medial core (Figure 4A1III, IV vs. 4B/III, IV). The anaesthesia (Mulder, Gijsberti Hodenpijl, & Lopes da intermediate septotemporal hippocampal and intermedi­ Silva, 1998). The core of the data discussed in the follow­ ate rostrocaudal amygdaloid regions converge in the ven­ ing paragraphs is largely derived from this paper, in which tromedial shell and the adjacent ventral core of the Acb, the details of the experimental procedures are given. this pattern being most obvious in rostral parts ofthe nu­ The hippocampal formation projections to the cleus (Figure 4A1I, II vs. 4B/I, II). The projections from Acb. Before the physiological interactions of the hippo­ the dorsal hippocampus converge with those from the ros­ campal and amygdaloid inputs to the Acb can be appre­ tral part of the basal amygdaloid complex (rostral Bmg) ciated, the electrophysiological characteristics of both in the lateral shell and adjacent lateral core, and this occurs pathways need to be discussed separately. As was dis­ almost exclusively in the rostral Acb (Figure 4A1I, II vs. cussed in previous sections, the main projections of the 4B/I, II). The results of double anterograde tracing stud­ hippocampal formation to the Acb arise from the sub­ ies demonstrate that areas of converging hippocampal iculum and course through the fornix/fimbria fiber bun­ and amygdaloid projections contain detailed patterns of dle (Fo/Fi). Electrical stimulation of these fibers results overlap and segregation, indicating that within such in a characteristic evoked field potential (EFP; Figure 5A) areas, subregions with preferential hippocampal or amyg­ that consists of a number of components: an initial neg­ daloid inputs exist (Beijer & Groenewegen, 1996; Beijer, ativity with a maximum at 6 msec followed by two short­ Wright, Witter, Smeets, & Groenewegen, unpublished latency positive synaptic components peaking at about observations ). 10 and 22 msec (PIO and P22) (Boeijinga, Mulder, Pen­ nartz, Manshanden, & Lopes da Silva, 1993; Mulder, PHYSIOLOGICAL INTERACTIONS Arts, & Lopes da Silva, 1997). Whereas the first compo­ BETWEEN HIPPOCAMPAL AND nent is the result of a monosynaptic activation, the P22, AMYGDALOID INPUTS IN THE of polysynaptic origin, arises from a loop involving in­ NUCLEUSACC~BENS trahippocampal circuits (Boeijinga et aI., 1993). The oc­ currence of action potentials always coincides with the Electrophysiological Characteristics of rising phase or the peak of the positive components of Hippocampal and Amygdaloid Inputs in the Acb the EFP (e.g., either the PIO or the P22). Accumbens neu­ In view ofthe role of the Acb as.a limbic-motor inter­ rons always respond with one single upon face, it is important to determine how the hippocampal a single stimulus, so no burst activity could be detected. and amygdaloid inputs are physiologically characterized This is in contrast to neuronal activation by glutamate and to establish their possible interactions. The electro­ iontophoresis, where bursts up to five action potentials physiological characteristics of the projections of the could be elicited (Mulder, Zuiderwijk, & Lopes da Silva, hippocampal formation and of the basal amygdaloid 1995). This reflects the existence of a strong inhibitory complex have been mapped in detail in rats (Boeijinga, GABAergic feedforward inhibition, which was also found Pennartz, & Lopes da Silva, 1990; Calloway, Hakan, & in Acb slice preparations (Pennartz & Kitai, 1991). The Henriksen, 1991; Finch, 1996; Hakan & Henriksen 1987; EFPs throughout the entire medial Acb are of similar O'Donnell & Grace, 1995; Yang & Mogenson, 1984, amplitude and latency with polarity changes only at the 1985; Yim & Mogenson, 1982, 1986, 1989), but also in dorsal and ventral borders (DeFrance et aI., 1985; Lopes rabbits (DeFrance, Marchand, Stanley, Sikes, & Chro­ da Silva et aI., 1984; Mulder et aI., 1997). This strongly nister, 1980; DeFrance, Marchand, Sikes, Chronister, & suggests that the input from the hippocampal formation Hubbard, 1985) and cats (Lopes da Silva, Arnolds, & to the region of the Acb is restricted to this nucleus, which Neijt, 1984). However, the interactions between these is in line with the anatomical data (see above). The lo­ two inputs were analyzed in only a few studies. calization of the Acb neurons activated by Fo/Fi stimu­ One of the earliest electrophysiological studies in lation clearly shows that the majority of these neurons which the amygdaloid and hippocampal projections to are located in the medial shell and core regions (Figure 6). the Acb were examined simultaneously was performed The lateral shell and the ventrolateral core are devoid of by DeFrance, Marchand, Stanley, Sikes, and Chronister Fo/Fi driven units. This is in accordance with the termi­ (1980). These authors described convergence of these nation patterns described on the basis of anatomical trac­ two inputs in nine cells in the dorsal part of the caudal ing studies (cf. Figure 4). The distribution oflatencies of Acb, but their precise localization in the nucleus is un­ single units responding to FolFi stimulation (i.e., the short­ known. More recent studies have confirmed the conver­ est latencies in the caudomedial area of the Acb and an gence of amygdaloid and hippocampal inputs in the Acb increase in latency in a rostral and ventrolateral direction; but either without a clear localization of the target cells Mulder et aI., 1998), is compatible with the anatomical (O'Donnell & Grace, 1995), or in only a very small num­ observations that the Fo/Fi fibers enter the Acb from a ber of neurons (Finch, 1996). Therefore, to provide a caudal dorsomedial position, fanning out in rostral and more comprehensive account of the interactions of the lateral directions (see also Groenewegen et aI., 1987). inputs arising from the hippocampal formation and the However, no clear division between shell and core, in HIPPOCAMPAL-AMYGDALOID INTERACTIONS IN THE ACCUMBENS 157

A: Fo/Fi-Acb C: BAC-Acb

> > - E E I,{) N a a

14 .::!2 B .sa 0

Figure 5. Evoked responses recorded in the Acb following FofFi or amygdala (BAC) stimulation. Recording in the medial shell and medial core regions of the Acb resulted in EFPs with positive peak latencies of approximately 10 and 22 msec to FofFi stimulation (trace in A) and negative peak latencies of approximately 16 msec to amygdala stimulation (C, black trace). In the ventrolateral shell and core regions, upon BAC stimulation, EFPs with longer latencies were recorded (C, gray trace). The distribution of the latencies of Acb single units driven by FofFl (B) and/or by amygdala (D) stimulation are given. Clearly visible are the two distinct latencies for the FofFi driven neurons that correspond to the two peaks of the evoked field potential PIO and P22. The amygdala-driven units recorded in the medial shell and core ofthe Acb display latencies with peaks at IS and 19 msec (in black). Neurons displaying, on the average, longer latencies to amygdala stimulation were exclusively found in the ventrolateral parts of the Acb in both shell and core (in gray). BAC, basal amygdaloid complex. any direction, can be made with respect to the latency of driven neurons have the tendency to be more sponta­ firing. Following these initial excitatory components, a neously active. Two separate populations of neurons re­ long-latency positive deflection can be seen in the EFP sponding to amygdaloid stimulation can be found in the that can last between 300 and 600 msec. Spontaneously Acb. In the medial part of the Acb, such neurons display active neurons in the Acb are inhibited during this period relatively short latencies (around 15 and 19 msec), whereas (Mulder et aI., 1997). Stimulation of the ventral subicu­ in the ventrolateral Acb, neurons with significantly longer lum, basal amygdaloid complex, and lateral entorhinal latencies (up to 26 msec) were found (Figure 5C, Figure 6). cortex also leads to a short excitation followed by an in­ These observations are in line with earlier reports, al­ hibitory phase that can therefore be considered a com­ though no relationships between latencies and locations mon feature of all Acb excitatory inputs (Finch, Gigg, Tan, in the Acb have been reported previously (Finch, 1996; & Kosoyan, 1995; Yang & Mogenson, 1984; Yim & Mo­ O'Donnell & Grace, 1995; Yim & Mogenson, 1982, genson, 1982). The late inhibition is thought to be medi­ 1986, 1989). The distinctive difference in latency corre­ ated by GABA-B receptors (Finch et aI., 1995; Gigg, sponds with the two routes by which the amygdaloid Tan, & Finch, 1994). fibers course toward the Acb; that is, the caudal amygdala The basal amygdaloid projections to the Acb. In projects via the stria terminalis to the caudomedial Acb contrast to the EFPs evoked by Fo/Fi activation, basal and the rostral amygdala via the external capsule to the amygdaloid stimulation results in EFPs displaying a prom­ lateral Acb (see Kita & Kitai, 1990; Shinonaga et aI., inent negative field potential component (Figure 5Q, on 1994; Wright et aI., 1996). Since these pathways have top of which the monosynaptic action potentials tend to different latencies, it is likely that they have different ride (Mulder et aI., 1998). In general, cells respond with conduction times. In addition, the different peak laten­ a single spike upon one stimulus, although occasionally cies in the medial shell and core neurons may be due to bursts of two or three action potentials were recorded, the fact that the stria terrninalis contains slower, as well especially to higher stimulation intensities. In contrast to as faster, conducting fibers (Fernandez de Molina & the neurons excited by Fo/Fi stimulation, the amygdaloid- Garcia-Sanchez, 1967). 158 GROENEWEGEN ET AL.

A B c

Figure 6. Distribution of single-unit activity within the Acb to FolFi and amygdala stimulation. The drawings of the sections are based on three CaB-stained sections shown in Figure 1. Single units driven by FolFi stimulation are repre­ sented as nlled circles and amygdala-driven single units are represented as triangles. Single units responding to both FolFi and BLA stimulation are represented by open star shapes. Units driven by the FolFi only were found in the dor­ sal shell and the ventromedial caudate putamen, whereas only amygdala-driven activity was recorded in the ventro­ lateral part of the Acb in both shell and core regions. Single units responding to both stimuli were recorded in the me­ dial shell and medial core regions. (A) rostral section; (C) c~udal section. From "Electrophysiology of the Hippocampal and Amygdaloid Projections to the Nucleus Accumbens of the Rat: Convergence, Segregation and Interaction of In­ puts," by A. B. Mulder, M. Gijsberti Hodenpijl, and F. H. Lopes da Silva, 1998, Journal ofNeuro science, 18, p. 5098. Copyright 1998 by Society for . Adapted with permission.

As was seen for the Acb neurons responding to FoIFi, ventrolateral areas of the Acb, in both shell and core, ex­ the projections from the basal amygdaloid complex to clusively single units driven by amygdaloid stimulation the Acb present shorter latencies in caudal areas and were found. No differences in response latencies between longer latencies in rostral areas ofthe nucleus. In line with cells activated only by FolFi or cells that also received the results of the anatomical tracing studies, a few single amygdaloid inputs were apparent. All amygdala-driven units (n = 3, latency 14 ± 1 msec) in the olfactory tu­ cells that presented convergence of inputs displayed bercle were found to respond to amygdaloid stimulation. short latencies, which were not different from those of In the field potential evoked by amygdaloid stimulation, cells solely activated by the amygdala, if recorded in the similar to that evoked by stimulation of FolFi fibers, a same accumbal region. The single units recorded in the long-latency positive deflection was found. Spontaneously ventrolateral areas, which showed no convergence, had active, amygdala-driven neurons in the Acb are inhibited long latencies. Close comparison of anatomical and elec­ during this period (Yim & Mogenson, 1982). trophysiological results indicates that the units in the Convergence and segregation of inputs within the ventrolateral shell and the ventral core regions are all nucleus accumbens. The convergence of inputs arising driven by afferents from the rostral part of the amygdala from the FolFi and from the basal amygdaloid complex (Figure 4, depicted in green). Units that showed conver­ was found mainly in the medial shell and medial core re­ gence of inputs received inputs from more midrostro­ gions (Figure 6), but only rarely in the dorsomedial part caudal and caudal parts ofthe basal amygdaloid complex of the shell of the Acb. No convergence was found in the and from ventral or intermediate parts of the subiculum ventrolateral shell and ventral core, or in the ventrome­ (Figure 4). dial part of the caudate-putamen complex. In general, this The typical polarity of the EFP generated by excitatory is in agreement with the results of other electrophysiolog­ postsynaptic potentials in the Acb (i.e., positive after FolFi ical studies (DeFrance et ai., 1985; Finch, 1996; O'Donnell stimulation and negative after amygdaloid stimulation) & Grace, 1995). In the dorsomedial shell of the Acb and and the coincidence of unit firing with both these fields the ventromedial part of the caudate-putamen complex, allows us to make some suggestions about the possible only FolFi driven cells were encountered. In contrast, in . distribution of synaptic inputs. Throughout the entire HIPPOCAMPAL-AMYGDALOID INTERACTIONS IN THE ACCUMBENS 159

A: Fo/Fi stimulation B: BAC stimulation

Figure 7. Model of terminal localization of hippocampal and basal amygdaloid input to the Acb, based on the po­ larity of the EFPs (see Figure 5) following stimulation of the respective inputs. In A and B, the Acb is represented by three stellate cells, drawn after an accumbal medium-sized spiny . The pronounced positive field potentials after Fo/Fi stimulation can be the result of hippocampal formation being localized on the distal parts of the dendrites of the Acb neurons. In this way, after FolFi stimulation (A), a distally localized current sink (-, dark gray) is present that forms a ring around the Acb neuron. The rest of the proximal dendrites of the Acb neuron act as an extended current source (+, light gray), which is reflected in the strong positive evoked potentials throughout the nu­ cleus. The negative field potential seen after amygdaloid stimulation (B) can be explained by BAC inputs terminat­ ing around the and along large areas of the proximal dendrites. This results in current sinks on large parts of the dendrites (-, dark gray), surrounded by rings of sources (+, light gray). The sinks are dominant, and this is re­ flected in the negative-going deflection of the local field potential.

Acb, similar EFPs were recorded with polarity changes found to ride local negative field potential components. only at the dorsal and ventral borders (Lopes da Silva et aI., Accordingly, we assume that amygdaloid excitatory in­ 1984; Mulder et aI., 1997). This is in line with the fact that puts result in proximal current sinks, distributed around the medium-sized spiny Acb neurons do not form well­ the soma and along large areas of the proximal dendrites defined layers, but have cell bodies occupying a central (Figure 7). Thus, it may be hypothesized that the hippo­ position surrounded by dendrites in all directions (Arts campal inputs make synaptic contacts primarily with dis­ & Groenewegen, 1992; DeFrance et aI., 1985; Pennartz tal dendrites, whereas the amygdaloid synapses are pre­ & Kitai, 1991). Both hippocampal and amygdaloid ter­ dominantly located more proximally on the dendrites. minals in the Acb have been shown to make asymmetri­ cal synapses primarily on dendritic spines (Johnson, Ayl­ Interactions Between Hippocampal and ward, Hussain, & Totterdell, 1994; Kita & Kitai, 1990; Amygdaloid Inputs in the Acb Meredith et aI., 1993; Totterdell & Smith, 1989). In gen­ An examination of the interactions between both lim­ eral terms, extracellular fields are generated by a com­ bic inputs in the areas of convergence of the monosynap­ bination of sinks, at the site of active excitatory synaptic tically driven Fo/Fi and amygdaloid activity in the Acb contacts, and sources at the remainder part of the soma­ discloses striking effects, both on a short- and a long­ dendritic membrane (Lopes da Silva, 1996). Therefore, duration scale. The former was studied by measuring we hypothesize that the hippocampal afferents terminate paired pulse interaction between inputs, whereas the lat­ on distal parts ofthe dendritic tree of Acb neurons, form­ ter was examined using long-term potentiation (LTP) ing a distal sink that surrounds the cell body as a narrow protocols. Paired pulse facilitation (PPF) was readily ring, whereas the main part of the cell (i.e., the soma and found in the Acb in response to stimulation of the hip­ the proximal dendrites) behaves like an extensive source pocampal inputs, as well as to that of the amygdaloid (Figure 7). Because the neurons with all their dendritic projections to the Acb, when both stimuli were given to arborizations overlap, the resultant field will be dominated the same pathway (Boeijinga et aI., 1990; Mulder et aI., by these sources. The finding of negative population 1997; Mulder et aI., 1998). This homosynaptic PPF is most spikes riding on the positive peak of the local field poten­ likely modulated by a strong presynaptic facilitatory tial strengthens this interpretation. However, after stim­ mechanism (Kuhnt & Voronin, 1994) that appears to be ulation of the amygdala, negative population spikes are able to mask the long-latency GABA-B receptor-mediated 160 GROENEWEGEN ET AL. inhibitory component found in both pathways. However, shell and core region of the Acb only, integration of these when the excitability of the amygdaloid pathway to the inputs is apparent. Here, strong interactions are shown to Acb was tested in the 200-msec period after stimulation exist between the pathways from the hippocampal forma­ of the Fo/Fi fibers, the firing probability was markedly tion and the basal amygdaloid complex. Although we are lower than after a single amygdaloid stimulation. In con­ just at the beginning of unraveling the underlying mech­ trast, testing Fo/Fi to Acb excitability just after amygdala anisms, it may well be that GABAergic and dopaminer­ stimulation resulted in a marked increase in firing prob­ gic receptors play leading roles in these phenomena. ability of the Fo/Fi response relative to one single FolFi stimulation. This facilitation, however, was less than in MECHANISMS OF SYNAPTIC the case of paired Fo/Fi stimulation. The mechanism of TRANSMISSION AND PLASTICITY IN the facilitatory heterosynaptic effect of a conditioning AMYGDALOID AND HIPPOCAMPAL amygdaloid input on the test hippocampal response may INPUTS TO ACCUMBENS be explained by considering the suggested model ofter­ minallocation presented earlier (Figure 7). Amygdaloid The hippocampal input to the Acb has been more ex­ inputs depolarize the proximal part of the soma-dendritic tensively studied in in vitro slice preparations than the membrane ofthe medium-sized spiny neurons of the Acb amygdaloid input. Intracellular recordings from Acb neu­ that have a stellate configuration. Accordingly, the hippo­ rons in slices have revealed a predominant EPSP-IPSP campal inputs that follow an amygdaloid stimulus en­ sequence evoked by electrical stimulation of Fo/Fi in a counter the soma of the Acb neurons that are already in parasagittal slice preparation (Pennartz & Kitai, 1991; a depolarized state. This could account for a postsynap­ cf. Chang & Kitai, 1986). The EPSP was shown to be me­ tic facilitation ofthis form of paired responses. The amyg­ diated primarily by AMPA receptors and the IPSP by dala to Acb suppression, however, is not easily accounted GABAA receptors. The glutamatergic excitation evoked for. In part, it can be explained by the recent results of by stimulation of hippocampal afferents is mediated via Blaha, Yang, Floresco, Barr, and Phillips (1997). They monosynaptic contacts on medium-sized spiny neurons, showed that ventral subiculum stimulation results in pro­ whereas GABAergic inhibition is mediated via a disynap­ longed efflux in the Acb. This increase, if spe­ tic pathway (feedforward inhibition; Pennartz & Kitai, cific for the pathway from the hippocampal formation to 1991). At present, the most likely candidate mechanism the Acb, could explain the attenuation of amygdala re­ underlying this feedforward inhibition is a glutamatergic sponses after Fo/Fi excitation. However, these phenom­ excitation by hippocampal output of GABAergic inter­ ena have not been studied for the amygdaloid inputs into neurons in the Acb, which subsequently inhibit medium­ the Acb. sized spiny neurons (see Cowan, Wilson, Emson, & Heiz­ Apart from these short-term interactions, interactions mann, 1990; Kita, Kosaka, & Heizmann, 1990). This oflonger duration can also be found between the two path­ GABAergic input not only delimits the duration of effec­ ways. As noted, tetanization of the Fo/Fi resulted in tive AMPA receptor-mediated excitation, but also keeps decremental LTP in the Acb with a duration of approxi­ NMDA receptor activity depressed (Pennartz, Boeijinga, mately 60 min (Boeijinga et aI., 1993; Mulder et aI., 1997). Kitai, & Lopes da Silva, 1991). O'Donnell and Grace Examined in the same rat, the nontetanized amygdala­ (1995) confirmed direct excitation of accumbens neurons to-Acb pathway showed long-term depression (LTD) for by hippocampal input in vivo and also showed hippo­ the entire duration of the Fo/Fi-induced LTP in the Acb campal inputs to regulate an electrophysiological bista­ (Mulder et aI., 1998). This heterosynaptic depression may bility in medium-sized spiny neurons, referred to as the be due to the fact that potentiation of the population of alternate occurrence of "up" and "down" states in mem­ cells driven by Fo/Fi may cause a potentiation of GAB A­ brane potential (see also C. J. Wilson & Kawaguchi, 1996). ergic inhibition in surrounding cells. Indeed, we found In anaesthetized or awake animals, the mean sponta­ that this form of LTP is accompanied by strong GABA­ neous firing rate of Acb neurons is relatively low as com­ ergic feedforward inhibition in this neuronal popUlation pared with that in other brain areas, which is likely to re­ (Mulder, Gijsberti Hodenpijl, & Lopes da Silva, 1995). late to the presence of a strong inward rectifier K + current Strikingly, the time period of dopamine efflux in the Acb in these cells, keeping them in a quiescent, hyperpolar­ after multiple ventral subiculum stimulations (Blaha et aI., ized state until a barrage of excitatory input arrives (Pen­ 1997) is similar to the duration of the LTP in the Fo/Fi nartz et aI., 1991; Pennartz et aI., 1994; Uchimura, Cheru­ pathway and the LTD in the amygdala to Acb pathway. bini, & North, 1989). As noted, stimulation of hippocampal This indicates that dopamine may also playa role in these inputs to the Acb in vivo evokes a transient excitation or phenomena. However, it cannot be ruled out that the het­ an excitation followed by inhibition in many cells. These erosynaptic LTD results from a postsynaptic mechanism response patterns are consistent with, and can also largely (Abraham & Goddard, 1983) that may depress the activ­ be explained, by the sequence of glutamatergic EPSPs ity of synapses of a separate input lying in close proxim­ and GABAA receptor-mediated IPSPs observed intracel­ ity to the tetanized one. In summary, strong similarities lularly in vitro. exist in the physiological responses of Acb neurons to Much less is known about the intracellular events un­ hippocampal and amygdaloid stimulation. In the medial derlying neuronal responses to amygdala input in Acb, al- HIPPOCAMPAL-AMYGDALOID INTERACTIONS IN THE ACCUMBENS 161 though the basic pattern of responding seems to be simi­ Stiiubli, & LeDoux, 1997). Thus, temporal coincidence lar. Yim and Mogenson (1988) recorded fast depolarizing­ of a particular environmental configuration with a pri­ hyperpolarizing sequences in vivo that resembled the mary reinforcer or reward-predicting stimulus may be EPSP-IPSP response pattern described earlier in terms of hypothesized to result in enhanced activation of Acb neu­ kinetics, thus making it likely that glutamate and GABA rons receiving the convergent information. If such con­ are also the main transmitters shaping fast information verging, temporally coinciding input is sufficiently in­ transmission from amygdala to Acb. tense to induce long-lasting changes in synaptic gain at Intracellular mechanisms underlying synaptic plastic­ the level of the medium-sized spiny neuron, then associ­ ity as observed in EFPs, as described, have not yet been ations between hippocampal and amygdaloid inputs can addressed for the hippocampal projection to the Acb, but in principle be stored in this limbic-Acb synaptic inter­ have been examined for the pathway from the prelimbic/ face. For instance, when a particular environment is con­ infralimbic cortex to Acb (Kombian & Malenka, 1994; sistently paired in time with a primary reinforcer that Pennartz, Ameerun, & Lopes da Silva, 1993). AMPA re­ elicits approach behavior, associative potentiation of hip­ ceptors mediate fast excitation of medium-sized spiny pocampal inputs carrying contextual information may be neurons in the latter projection, and GABAergic inhibi­ hypothesized to occur. Thus, on a future occasion where tion acts to attenuate NMDA receptor-mediated activity. the environment is presented without the primary rein­ In vitro, long- and short-term potentiation of the AMPA forcer, the environment alone would be sufficient in trig­ receptor component can be elicited by tetanic stimulation gering a behavioral approach. Associating these pieces of of prelimbic afferents, and induction is facilitated by information obviously relates to conditioned place pref­ GABAA receptor blockade. LTP induction was shown to erence (see Everitt et aI., 1991). In a similar way, hippo­ be mediated by NMDA receptors (Pennartz et aI., 1993), campal inputs may be associated with information about thus in principle endowing the system with a capacity for secondary reinforcers, and this associative principle by associative memory (Bliss & Collingridge, 1993). no means needs to be restricted to information derived Kombian and Malenka (1994) showed that the long-lasting from the hippocampus and amygdala. As a final remark, enhancement of the AMPA component is accompanied it should be emphasized that, although the cellular ma­ by a decrement of the NMDA component, suggesting chinery for associative storage capacity is present in Acb, that intense afferent activity may not only alter the it is unknown whether long-term potentiation and NMDA synaptic efficacy of limbic input to Acb, but also affect receptors in Acb do indeed subserve this function. Further­ the ability of the synaptic interface to induce and sustain more, this scheme of reasoning does not offer a func­ further changes in strength (). It is not yet tional explanation of the depression of amygdala inputs clear to what extent these findings apply to hippocampal upon potentiation of the hippocampal pathway, as de­ or amygdaloid inputs to Acb. However, the central role scribed above. of NMDA receptors in inducing plasticity in the hippo­ campal projection has been confirmed by NMDA recep­ REFERENCES tor applications in vivo by Feasey-Truger and ten Brug­ gencate (1994). ABRAHAM, W. c., & GODDARD, G. V. (1983). Asymmetric relationships between homosynaptic long-term potentiation and heterosynaptic long-term depression. Nature, 305, 717-719. FUNCTIONAL IMPLICATIONS AOOLPHS, R., TRANEL, D., DAMASIO, H., & DAMASIO, A. R. (1995). and the human amygdala. Journal o/Neuroscience, 15, 5879-5891. The relatively large degree of convergence between ALVAREZ, P., ZoLA-MORGAN, S., & SQUIRE, L. R. (1995). Damage to amygdaloid and hippocampal inputs to single Acb neu­ the hippocampal region produces long-lasting memory impairment in monkeys. Journal 0/ Neuroscience, 15,3796-3807. rons permits further thinking about integration and as­ ARTS, M. P. M., & GROENEWEGEN, H. J. (1992). Relationships of the sociation of information coming from these two distinct dendritic arborations of ventral striatomesencephalic projection sources. Evidence has grown to suggest that especially neurons with bounderies of striatal compartments. An in vitro intra­ the dorsal hippocampus of the rat encodes spatial infor­ cellular labelling study in the rat. European Journal o/Neuroscience, 4,574-588. mation (McNaughton et aI., 1996; O'Keefe & Dostro­ BEUER, A. V. J., & GROENEWEGEN, H. J. (1996). Specific anatomical re­ vsky, 1971; O'Keefe & Nadel, 1978; M. A. Wilson & lationships between hippocampal and basal amygdaloid afferents and McNaughton, 1993), in particular the allocentric posi­ different populations of projection neurons in the nucleus accumbens tion of the animal. Other studies on hippocampal func­ of rats. Society/or Neuroscience Abstracts, 22, 413. BERENDSE, H. W., GROENEWEGEN, H. J., & LOHMAN, A. H. M. (1992). tion suggest a broader role, specifically, in establishing Compartmental distribution of ventral striatal neurons projecting to the and storing relational configurations of objects and envi­ mesencephalon in the rat. Journal o/Neuroscience, 12,2079-2103. ronmental features (Eichenbaum & Cohen, 1988; Wiener, BLAHA, C. D., YANG, C. R., FLORESCO, S. 8., BARR, A. M., & PHILLIPS, 1996). The amygdaloid complex, including its basal A. G. (1997). Stimulation of the ventral subiculum of the hippocampus complex, is strongly involved in making associations be­ evokes -mediated changes in dopamine efflux in the rat nucleus accumbens. European Journal o/Neuroscience, 9, 902-911. tween primary reinforcers and conditioned stimuli (Gaf­ BLISS, T. V. P., & COLLINGRIDGE, G. L. (1993). A synaptic model of fan, Murray, & Fabre-Thorpe, 1993; Killcross, Robbins, memory: Long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature, 361, & Everitt, 1997; Nishijo, Ono, & Nishino, 1988; Rogan, 31-39. 162 GROENEWEGEN ET AL.

BOEIJINGA, P. H., MULDER, A B., PENNARTZ, C. M. A, MANSHAN­ cat, studied with retrograde axonal transport of horseradish peroxi­ DEN, I., & LoPES DA SILVA, E H. (1993). Response of the nucleus ac­ dase and bisbenzimid. Neuroscience,S, 1903-1916. cumbens following fornix/fimbria stimulation of the rat. Identifica­ GROENEWEGEN, H. J., BERENDSE, H. W., WOLTERS, J. G., & LOHMAN, tion and long-term potentiation ofmono- and polysynaptic pathways. A. H. M. (1990). The anatomical relationship of the prefrontal cor­ Neuroscience, 53, 1049-1058. tex with the striatopallidal system, the and the amygdala: BOEIJINGA, P. H., PENNARTz, C. M. A., & LOPES DA SILVA, E H. (1990). Evidence for a parallel organization. In H. B. M. Uijlings, C. G. Paired-pulse facilitation in the nucleus accumbens following stimu­ VanEden,1. P. C. DeBruin, M. A. Comer, & M. P. G. Feenstra (Eds.), lation of the subicular inputs in the rat. Neuroscience, 35, 301-311. The prefrontal cortex: Its structure, function and pathology (Progress BROG, J. S., SALYAPONGSE, A., DEUTCH, A Y., & ZAHM, D. S. (1993). in Brain Research, Vol. 85, pp. 95-118). Amsterdam: Elsevier. The patterns of afferent innervation of the core and shell of the "ac­ GROENEWEGEN, H. J., ROOM, P., WITTER, M. P., & LoHMAN, A H. M. cumbens" part of the rat ventral striatum: Immunohistochemical de­ (1982). Cortical afferents of the nucleus accumbens in the cat: Studied tection of retrogradely transported fluoro-gold. Journal of Compar­ with anterograde and retrograde transport techniques. Neuroscience, ative Neurology, 338, 255-278. 7,977-996. CADOR, M., ROBBINS, T. w., & EVERITT, B. J. (\ 989). Involvement of the GROENEWEGEN, H. J., VERMEULEN-VAN DER ZEE, E., TE KORT­ amygdala in stimulus-reward associations: Interactions with the ven­ SCHOT, A, & WITTER, M. P. (\987). Organization of the projections tral striatum. Neuroscience, 30, 77-86. from the subiculum to the ventral striatum in the rat: A study using CALLOWAY, C., HAKAN, R. L., HENRIKSEN, S. J. (1991). Distribution of anterograde transport of Phaseolus vulgaris-Ieucoagglutinin. Neuro­ amygdala input to the nucleus septi: An electrophysiological investi­ science, 23, 103-120. gation. Journal ofNeural Transmission, 83, 215-225. GROENEWEGEN, H. J., WRIGHT, C. I., & BEIJER, AY. J. (1996). The nu­ CHANG, H. T., & KITAI, S. T. (\986). Intracellular recordings from rat cleus accumbens: Gateway for limbic structures to reach the motor nucleus accumbens neurons in vitro. Brain Research, 366, 392-396. system? In G. Holstege, R. Bandler, & C. B. Saper (Eds.), The emo­ CHRONISTER, R. B., SIKES, R. W., TRow, T. W., & DEFRANCE, J. E tional motor system (Progress in Brain Research, Vol. 107, pp. 485- (1981). The organization of the nucleus accumbens. In R. B. Chro­ 5\1). Amsterdam: Elsevier. nister & 1. F. DeFrance (Eds.), The neurobiology of the nucleus ac­ HAKAN, R. L., & HENRIKSEN, S. J. (1987). Systemic opiate administra­ cumbens (pp. 97-146). Brunswick, ME: Haer Institute for Physio­ tion has heterogeneous effects on activity recorded from nucleus ac­ logical Research. cumbens neurons in vivo. Neuroscience Letters, 83, 307-312. COWAN, R. L., WILSON, C. J., EMSON, P. C., & HEIZMANN, C. W. (1990). HEIMER, L., ALHEID, G. E, DE OLMOS, J. S., GROENEWEGEN, H. J., -containing GABAergic interneurons in the rat neo­ HABER, S. N., HARLAN, R. E., & ZAHM, D. S. (1997). The accumbens, striatum. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 302, 197-205. beyond the core-shell dichotomy. Journal of & DAVIES, M. (1992). The role of the amygdala in conditioned fear. In 1. P. Clinical Neurosciences, 9, 354-381. Aggleton (Ed.), The amygdala: Neurobiological aspects ofemotion, HEIMER, L., DE OLMOS, J. S., ALHEID, G. F., PEARSON, J., SAKA­ memory and mental dysfunction (pp. 255-305). New York: Wiley. MOTO, N., MARKSTEINER, J., & SWITZER, III, R. C. (in press). The DEFRANCE, J. E, MARCHAND, J. E, SIKES, R. W., CHRONISTER, R. B., & human , Part 2. In F. Bloom, A. Bjorklund, & T. Hokfelt HUBBARD, 1. I. (1985). Characterization of fimbria input to nucleus (Eds.), Handbook ofchemical (Vol. 15). Amsterdam: accumbens. Journal ofNeurophysiology, 54, 1553-1567. Elsevier. DEFRANCE, J. E, MARCHAND, J., STANLEY, J. C., SIKES, R. W., & CHRO­ HEIMER, L., & WILSON, R. D. (1975). The subcortical projections of the NISTER, R. B. (1980). Convergence of excitatory amygdaloid and hip­ : Similarities in the neural associations of the hippocam­ pocampal input in the nucleus accumbens septi. Brain Research, 185, pus, the , and the . In M. Santini (Ed.), Golgi 183-186. Centennial Symposium: Perspectives in Neurobiology (pp. 177-193). DEUTCH, A. Y., & CAMERON, D. S. (J 992). Pharmacological character­ New York: Raven. ization of dopamine systems in the nucleus accumbens core and shell. HEIMER, L., ZAHM, D. S., & ALHEID, G. F. (1995). Basal ganglia. In Neuroscience, 46, 49-56. G. Paxinos (Ed.), The rat nervous system (2nd ed., pp. 579-628). Syd­ EICHENBAUM, H., & COHEN, N. J. (1988). Representation in the hippo­ ney: Academic Press. campus: What do hippocampal neurons code? Trends in Neuro­ HEIMER, L., ZAHM, D. S., CHURCHILL, L., KALIVAS, P. W., & WOHLT­ sciences, 11,244-248. MANN, C. (1991). Specificity in the projection patterns of the accum­ EVERITT, B. J., MORRIS, K. A., O'BRIEN, A., & ROBBINS, T. W. (1991). bal core and shell in the rat. Neuroscience, 41, 89-125. The -ventral striatal system and conditioned HERKENHAM, M., MOON-EDLEY, S., & STUART, J. (1984). Cell clusters place preference: Further evidence oflimbic-striatal interactions un­ in the nucleus accumbens of the rat, and the mosaic relationship of derlying reward-related processes. Neuroscience, 42,1-18. opiate, acetylcholinesterase, and subcortical afferent terminations. FEASEY-TRuGER, K. J., & TEN BRUGGENCATE, G. (1994). The NMDA Neuroscience, 11,561-593. receptor antagonist CPP suppresses long-term potentiation in the rat JOHNSON, L. R., AYLWARD, R. L., HUSSAIN, Z., & TOTTERDELL, S. hippocampal-accumbens pathway in vivo. European Journal ofNeuro­ (1994). Input from the amygdala to the rat nucleus accumbens: Its re­ science, 6, 1247-1254. lationship with tyrosine hydroxylase immunoreactivity and identi­ FERNANDEZ DE MOLINA, A, & GARCIA-SANCHEZ, 1. L. (1967). The prop­ fied neurons. NeurOSCience, 61, 851-865. erties of the stria terminalis fibers. Physiology Behavior, 2, 225-227. JONGEN-RELO, A., GROENEWEGEN, H. J., & VOORN, P. (1993). Evidence FINCH, D. M. (1996). of converging synaptic inputs for a multicompartmental histochemical organization of the nucleus from rat prefrontal cortex, amygdala, midline thalamus, and hippo­ accumbens in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 337, 267- campal formation onto single neurons of the caudate/putamen and 276. nucleus accumbens. Hippocampus, 6, 495-512. JONGEN-Rho, A, VOORN, P., & GROENEWEGEN, H. J. (1994). Immuno­ FiNCH, D. M., GIGG, J., TAN, A. M., & KOSOYAN, O. P. (1995). Neuro­ histochemical characterization of the shell and core territories of the physiology and neuropharmacology of projections from entorhinal nucleus accumbens in the rat. European Journal ofNeuroscience, 6, cortex to striatum in the rat. Brain Research, 670, 233-247. 1255-1264. GAFFAN, D., MURRAY, E. A, & FABRE-THORPE, M. (1993). Interaction KELLEY, A. E., & DOMESICK, V. B. (1982). The distribution of the pro­ of the amygdala with the frontal lobe in reward memory. European jection from the hippocampal formation to the nucleus accumbens in Journal ofNeuroscience, 5, 968-975. the rat: An anterograde and retrograde-horseradish peroxidase study. GIGG, J., TAN, A. M., & FINCH, D. M. (1994). Glutamatergic hippo­ Neuroscience, 7, 2321-2335. campal formation projections to prefrontal cortex in the rat are regu­ KELLEY, A. E., DOMESICK, V. B., & NAUTA, W. J. H. (1982). Theamyg­ lated by GABAergic inhibition and show convergence with glutama­ dalastriatal projection in the rat-An anatomical study by anterograde tergic projections from the limbic thalamus. Hippocampus, 4, 189-198. and retrograde methods. Neuroscience, 7, 615-630. GROENEWEGEN, H. J., BECKER, N. E. H. M., & LOHMAN, A. H. M. KELLEY, A. E., SMITH-ROE, S. L., & HOLAHAN, M. R. (1997). Response­ (1980). Subcortical afferents to the nucleus accumbens septi in the reinforcement learning is dependent on N-methyl-D-aspartate recep- HIPPOCAMPAL-AMYGDALOID INTERACTIONS IN THE ACCUMBENS 163

tor activation in the nucleus accumbens core. Proceedings ofthe Na­ O'DoNNELL, P., & GRACE, A. A. (1995). Synaptic interactions among ex­ tional Academy ofSciences, 94, 12174-12179. citatory afferents to nucleus accumbens neurons: Hippocampal gating KILLCROSS, S., ROBBINS, T. w., & EVERITT, B. I. (1997). Different types of prefrontal cortical input. Journal ofNeuroscience, 15, 3622-3639. of fear-conditioned behaviour mediated by separate nuclei within O'KEEFE, I., & DoSTROVSKY, I. (1971). The hippocampus as a spatial amygdala. Nature, 388, 377-380. map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. KITA, H., & KITAI, S. T. (1990). Amygdaloid projections to the frontal Brain Research, 34, 171-175. cortex and striatum in the rat. Journal ofComparutive Neurology, 298, O'KEEFE, I., & NADEL, L. (1978). The hippocampus as a . 40-49. Oxford: Oxford University Press, Clarendon Press. KITA, H., KOSAKA, T., & HEIZMANN, C. W. (1990). Parvalbumin­ PARKJNSON, I. A., OLMSTEAD, M. c., BURNS, L. H., ROBBINS, T. W., & immunoreactive neurons in the rat neostriatum: A light and electron EVERITT, B. I. (1999). Dissociation in effects of lesions of the nu­ microscopic study. Brain Research, 536, I-IS. cleus accumbens core and shell on appetitive Pavlovian approach be­ KOMBIAN, S. B., & MALENKA, R. C. (1994). Simultaneous LTP of non­ havior and the potentiation of conditioned reinforcement and loco­ NMDA and LTD ofNMDA-receptor mediated responses in the nu­ motor activity by d-amphetamine. Journal of Neuroscience, 19, cleus accumbens. Nature, 368, 242-246. 2401-2411. KOOB, G. F. (1992). Drugs of abuse: Anatomy, pharmacology and function PENNARTZ, C. M. A., AMEERUN, R. F., & LOPES DA SILVA, F. H. (1993). of reward pathways. Trends in Pharmacological Sciences, 13, 177-184. in an in vitro slice preparation of the rat nucleus KUHNT, U., & VORONIN, L. L. (1994). Interaction between paired pulse accumbens. European Journal ofNeuroscience, 5, 107-117. facilitation and long-term potentiation in area CAl of guinea-pig PENNARTZ, C. M. A., BOEIJINGA, P. H., KITAI, S. T., & loPES DA SILVA, hippocampal slices: Application of quantal analysis. Neuroscience, F. H. (1991). Contribution ofNMDA receptors to postsynaptic poten­ 62,391-397. tials and paired-pulse facilitation in identified neurons ofthe rat nu­ LEDoux, I. E. (1993). Emotional memory systems in the brain. Behav­ cleus accumbens in vitro. Experimental Bruin Research, 86, 190-198. ioural Brain Research, 58, 69-79. PENNARTZ, C. M. A., GROENEWEGEN, H. I., & loPES DA SILVA, F. H. LOPES DA SILVA, F. H. (1996). The generation of electric and magnetic (1994). The nucleus accumbens as a complex of functionally distinct signals of the brain by local networks. In R. Gregor & U. Windhorst neuronal ensembles: An integration of behavioural, electrophysiolog­ (Eds.), Comprehensive human physiology (Vol. 1, pp. 509-531). ical and anatomic data. Progress in Neurobiology, 42, 719-761. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. PENNARTZ, C. M. A., & KITAI, S. T. (1991). Hippocampal inputs to LOPES DA SILVA, F. H., ARNOLDS, D. E., & NEUT, H. C. (1984). A func­ identified neurons in an in vitro slice preparation of the rat nucleus ac­ tional link between the limbic cortex and ventral striatum: Physiol­ cumbens: Evidence for feed-forward inhibition. Journal of Neuro­ ogy of the subiculum-accumbens pathway. Experimental Brain Re­ science, 11, 2838-2847. search, 55, 205-214. PHILLIPSON, O. T., & GRIFFITHS, A. C. (1985). The topographic order of McDONALD, A. I. (1991). Topographic organization of amygdaloid pro­ inputs to the nucleus accumbens in the rat. Neuroscience, 16,275-296. jections to the caudate putamen, nucleus accumbens, and related RAGSDALE, C. w., & GRA YBIEL, A. M. (1988). Fibers from the basolateral striatal-like areas of the rat brain. Neuroscience, 44, 15-33. nucleus ofthe amygdala selectively innervate striosomes in the caudate McNAUGHTON, B. L., BARNES, C. A., GERRARD, I. L., GOTHARD, K., nucleus of the cat. Journal ofComparative Neurology, 269, 506-522. lUNG, M. W., KNIERIM, I. I., KUDRIMOTI, H., QIN, Y., SKAGGS, W. E., ROBBINS, T. W., & EVERITT, B. I. (1996). Neurobehavioural mecha­ SUSTER, M., & WEAVER, K. L. (1996). Deciphering the hippocampal nisms of reward and motivation. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 6, polyglot: The hippocampus as a system. Journal of 228-236. Experimental Biology, 199, 173-185. ROGAN, M., STAUBLI, U. V., & LEDoux, I. E. (1997). Fear condition­ MEREDITH, G. E., PENNARTZ, C. M. A., & GROENEWEGEN, H. I. (1993). ing induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala. Na­ The cellular framework for chemical signalling in the nucleus accum­ ture, 390, 604-607. bens. In G. W. Arbuthnott & P. C. Emson (Eds.), Chemical signalling RUSSCHEN, F. T., BAKST, I., AMARAL, D. G., & PRICE, I. L. (1985). The in the basal ganglia (Progress in brain research, Vol. 99, pp. 3-24). amygdalostriatal projections in the monkey. An anterograde tracing Amsterdam: Elsevier. study. Brain Research, 329, 241-257. MOGENSON, G. I., lONES, D. L., & YIM, c. Y. (1980). From motivation SCHEEL-KRUGER, I., & WILLNER, P. (1991). The mesolimbic system: to action: Functional interface between the and the Principles of operation. In P. Willner & 1. Scheel-KrUger (Eds.), The motor system. Progress in Psychobiology, 14, 60-97. mesolimbic dopamine system: From motivation to action (pp. 559- MULDER, A. B., ARTS, M. P. M., & LOPES DA SILVA, F. H. (1997). Short­ 597). Chichester, U.K.: Wiley. and long-term plasticity of the hippocampus to nucleus accumbens SHINONAGA, Y., TAKADA, M., & MIZUNO, N. (1994). Topographic or­ and prefrontal cortex pathways in the rat, in vivo. European Journal ganization of collateral projections from the baso1ateral amygdaloid ofNeuroscience, 9,1603-1611. nucleus to both the prefrontal cortex and the nucleus accumbens. MULDER, A. B., GUSBERTI HODENPUL, M., & LOPES DA SILVA, F. H. Neuroscience, 58, 389-397. (1995). Electrophysiology ofthe hippocampal and basolateral amyg­ STRATFORD, T. R., & KELLEY, A. E. (1997). GABA in the nucleus ac­ daloid inputs to the nucleus accumbens of the rat: Patterns of conver­ cumbens shell participates in the central regulation of feeding behav­ gence and segregation. European Journal ofNeuroscience, 8, lSI. ior. Journal ofNeuroscience, 17,4434-4440. MULDER, A. B., GUSBERTI HODENPUL, M., & LOPES DA SILVA, F. H. TOTTERDELL, S., & MEREDITH, G. E. (1997). Topographical organiza­ (1998). Electrophysiology of the hippocampal and amygdaloid pro­ tion of projections from the to the striatum of the jections to the nucleus accumbens of the rat: Convergence, segregation rat. Neuroscience, 78, 715-729. and interaction of inputs. Journal ofNeuroscience, 18, 5095-5102. TOTTERDELL, S., & SMITH, A. D. (1989). Convergence of hippocampal MULDER, A. B., ZUIDERWIJK, M., & LOPES DA SILVA, F. H. (1995). En­ and dopaminergic input onto identified neurons in the nucleus ac­ hancement oflong-term potentiation in the nucleus accumbens by re­ cumbens of the rat. Journal ofChemical Neuroanatomy, 2, 285-298. moval of GABAergic inhibition. Pjliigers Archives, European Jour­ UCHIMURA, N., CHERUBINI, E., & NORTH, R. A. (1989). Inward rectifi­ nal of Physiology, 430, RI77. cation in rat nucleus accumbens neurons. Journal of Neurophysiol­ NAUTA, W. I. H., SMITH, G. p', FAULL, R. L., & DOMESICK, V. B. (1978). ogy,62,1280-1286. Efferent connections and nigral afferents of the nucleus accumbens VOORN, P., GERFEN, C. R., & GROENEWEGEN, H. I. (1989). Compart­ in the rat. Neuroscience, 3, 385-401. mental organization of the ventral striatum of the rat: Immunohisto­ NEWMAN, R., & WINANS, S. S. (1980). An experimental study of the chemical distribution of , substance P, dopamine and calcium ventral striatum of the golden hamster: I. Neuronal connections of binding protein. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 289, 189-201. the nucleus accumbens. Journal ofComparative Neurology, 191, 167- WEINER,I., GAL, G., RAWLINS, I. N., & FELDON, I. (1996). Differential 192. involvement of the shell and core subterritories of the nucleus ac­ NISHUO, H., ONO, T., & NISHI NO, H. (1988). Single neuron responses in cumbens in latent inhibition and amphetamine-induced activity. Be­ amygdala of alert monkey during complex sensory stimulation with havioural Brain Research, 81,123-134. affective significance. Journal ofNeuroscience, 8, 3570-3583. WIENER, S. I. (1996). Spatial, behavioral and sensory correlates of hippo- 164 GROENEWEGEN ET AL

campal CAl complex spike cell activity: Implications for informa­ YIM, C. Y., & MOGENSON, G. J. (1988). Neuromodulatory action of do­ tion processing functions. Progress in Neurobiology, 49, 335-361. pamine in the nucleus accumbens: An in vivo intracellular study. WILSON, C. J., & KAWAGUCHI, Y. (1996). The origins of two-state spon­ Neuroscience, 26, 403-415. taneous fluctuations of neostriatal spiny neurons. YIM, C. Y., & MOGENSON, G. J. (1989). Low doses of nucleus accum­ Journalo/Neuroscience, 16,2397-2410. bens dopamine modulate amygdala suppression of spontaneous ex­ WILSON, M. A., & McNAUGHTON, B. L. (1993). Dynamics of the hippo­ ploratory activity in rats. Brain Research, 477, 202-210. campal ensemble code for space. Science, 261,1055-1058. ZABORSZKY, L., ALHEID, G. F., BEINFELD, M. C., EIDEN, L. E., WRIGHT, C. I., BEIJER, A. V. J., & GROENEWEGEN, H. J. (1996). Basal HEIMER, L., & PALKOVITS, M. (1985). innervation of amygadaloid complex afferents to the rat nucleus accumbens are com­ the ventral striatum: A morphological and radioimmunological study. partmentally organized. Journal o/Neuroscience, 16, 1877-1893. Neuroscience, 14,427-453. WRIGHT, C.I., & GROENEWEGEN, H. 1. (1995). Patterns of convergence ZAHM, D. S., & BROG, 1. S. (1992). On the significance of subterritories and segregation in the medial nucleus accumbens ofthe rat: Relation­ in the "accumbens" part ofthe rat ventral striatum. Neuroscience, 50, ships of prefrontal cortical, midline thalamic and basal amygdaloid 751-767. afferents. Journal o/Comparative Neurology, 361, 383-403. ZAHM, D. S., & HEIMER, L. (1993). Specificity in the efferent projections WRIGHT, C. I., & GROENEWEGEN, H. 1. (1996). Patterns of overlap and of the nucleus accumbens in the rat: Comparison of the rostral pole segregation between insular cortical, intermediodorsal thalamic and projection patterns with those of the core and shell. Journal o/Com­ basal amygdaloid afferents in the nucleus accumbens of the rat. Neu­ parative Neurology, 327, 220-232. roscience, 73, 359-373. ZOLA-MoRGAN, S., SQUIRE, L. R, ALVAREZ-Royo, P., & CLOWER, R P. YANG, C. R, & MOGENSON, G. J. (1984). Eiectrophysiological responses (1991). Independence of memory functions and emotional behav­ of neurons in the nucleus accumbens to hippocampal stimulation and iour: Separate contributions of the hippocampal formation and the the attenuation of the excitatory responses by the meso limbic dopa­ amygdala. Hippocampus, 1,207-220. minergic system. Brain Research, 324, 69-84. ZOLA-MoRGAN, S., SQUIRE, L. R., & AMARAL, D. G. (1986). Human YANG, C. R, & MOGENSON, G. J. (1985). An electrophysiological study and the medial temporal region: Enduring memory impair­ ofthe neural projections from the hippocampus to the ventral pallidum ment following a bilateral lesion limited to field CAl of the hippo­ and the subpallidal areas by way of the nucleus accumbens. Neuro­ campus. Journal o/Neuroscience, 6, 2950-2967. science, 15, 1015-1024. ZOLA-MoRGAN, S., SQUIRE, L. R. & AMARAL, D. G. (1989). Lesions of YIM, C. Y., & MOGENSON, G. J. (1982). Response of nucleus accum­ the hippocampal formation but not lesions ofthe fornix or the mam­ bens neurons to amygdala stimulation and its modification by dopa­ millary nuclei produce long-lasting memory impairments in monkeys. mine. Brain Research, 239, 401-415. Journal o/Neuroscience, 9,898-913. YIM, C. Y., & MOGENSON, G. J. (1986). Mesolimbic dopamine projec­ tion modulates amygdala-evoked EPSP in nucleus accumbens neu­ (Manuscript received November 30, 1998; rons: An in vivo study. Brain Research, 369, 347-352. revision accepted for publication March 13, 1998.)