“Why Are We Doing This?”: Clinician Helplessness in the Face of Suffering

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

“Why Are We Doing This?”: Clinician Helplessness in the Face of Suffering JOURNAL OF PALLIATIVE MEDICINE Volume 18, Number 1, 2015 ª Mary Ann Liebert, Inc. DOI: 10.1089/jpm.2014.0115 ‘‘Why Are We Doing This?’’: Clinician Helplessness in the Face of Suffering Anthony L. Back, MD,1 Cynda H. Rushton, PhD,2 Alfred W. Kaszniak, PhD,3 and Joan S. Halifax, PhD4 Abstract Background: When the brutality of illness outstrips the powers of medical technology, part of the fallout lands squarely on front-line clinicians. In our experience, this kind of helplessness has cognitive, emotional, and somatic components. Objectives: Could we approach our own experiences of helplessness differently? Here we draw on social psychology and neuroscience to define a new approach. Methods: First, we show how clinicians can reframe helplessness as a self-barometer indicating their level of engagement with a patient. Second, we discuss how to shift deliberately from hyper- or hypo-engagement toward a constructive zone of clinical work, using an approach summarized as ‘‘RENEW’’: recognizing, embracing, nourishing, embodying, and weaving—to enable clinicians from all professional disciplines to sustain their service to patients and families. Case this, maybe I should give up.’’ They might be feeling hopeless, angry, resigned, or even ashamed. They might notice sensa- Ms. R is a 40-year-old female with metastatic melanoma with tions of bodily heaviness, lethargy, or a sinking feeling. brain metastases that have caused new blindness. The mela- 1,2 noma has progressed despite first- and second-line targeted Helplessness is known to be an occupational hazard, and therapy, and she is despondent. When the inpatient attending probably contributes to burnout. But ‘‘low personal accom- physician discussed with her that she was unlikely to regain plishment,’’ a component of burnout, seems inadequate to her sight, and that her melanoma was very unlikely to respond describe a moment of hopelessness like the one above. to further anticancer therapy, she began to weep so intensely Is it time to rewrite our story of helplessness? Although our that further conversation was impossible. The physician and first reaction to helplessness is often to remain silent, to push it nurse looked at each other; the physician gave a tiny shrug away, to keep it at a distance—we think that another path is that no one but the nurse would catch, and the nurse looked up possible. Instead of seeing helplessness as something to avoid, as if to say, ‘‘This is a fine mess.’’ The patient’s husband, and if that’s not possible, as a pathology that must be extirpated, appearing stricken, looked at the team and said, ‘‘We have to do something—could she have some Ativan?’’ could we first reframe it as an indicator about our internal state, and second, reexperience it as a moment characterized by Introduction a particular state of relationality with our patient? We are hoping to stimulate more dialog, and more research hen the brutality of illness outstrips the powers about helplessness by drawing on developments in contem- Wof medical technology, part of the fallout lands plative practice, neuroscience, and psychological research on Downloaded by Univ Of Washington from online.liebertpub.com at 08/24/17. For personal use only. squarely on front-line clinicians. Being a nurse or physician for attention and affect. a patient like Ms. R is a difficult assignment—she’s despon- dent, her husband is desperate, and there isn’t anything in the Helplessness as a Self-barometer clinical toolkit that seems remotely up to the task. In our ex- perience, this kind of helplessness has cognitive, emotional, The first part of our rewrite, reframing, involves our un- and somatic components. The nurse and physician in the case intentional reaction to helplessness. Could we reframe our might be thinking: ‘‘Nothing will fix this, there’s no way out of unintentional reaction to helplessness as a self-barometer that 1Department of Medicine, University of Washington, Fred Hutchinson Cancer Research Center, Seattle, Washington. 2Berman Institute of Bioethics; School of Nursing & Medicine, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland. 3Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona. 4Upaya Institute, Santa Fe, New Mexico. Accepted October 5, 2014. 26 CLINICIAN HELPLESSNESS 27 gives us a reading on the quality of our engagement with a attentional biases away from positive events.6,7 Interestingly, particular patient? The value of reframing our unintentional attentional biases appear to be modifiable over time, with reaction as a self-barometer is that it enables us to see how specific training.8 Extrapolating from this research, we think it challenges push us off balance constantly as a normal feature is likely that a clinician in the grip of helplessness is likely to of clinical work. We don’t expect the barometer readings to proceed with a constricted view of a clinical situation, meaning be unchanging, stable, and static—we expect them to fluc- that important facts will be missed, conclusions drawn pre- tuate with our circumstances, and to vary with our capacities maturely, and that the patient’s views, values, and stories will for ambiguity, uncertainty, empathy, and compassion on any be insufficiently seen2,9,10—distorting even palliative care into given day. With some patients, we find ourselves behaving in a series of medical treatments and procedures. a hyperactive mode. With other patients—even in the same As illustrated, these initial reactions of hypo-engagement day or hour—we find ourselves in a hypoactive mode. and hyper-engagement span professional roles. In the ex- ample above, we have illustrated hyper-engagement with a After the family meeting, the physician was talking to a col- league, and said, ‘‘I’m not even sure why we’re doing this— nurse, and hypo-engagement with a physician. However, it’s such a sad situation, it just seems futile. This sounds their responses could easily be reversed. A physician who terrible, but should we even bother with rehab?’’ Meanwhile, feels helpless might react with a hyper-engaged series of the inpatient nurse was telling her colleague, ‘‘She is suffering diagnostic tests (‘‘If we know as much as possible, we’ll find terribly. Why are we doing this—all these tests and treat- a treatment.’’). A nurse who feels helpless might react in a ments? I’m not sure that more tests are going to relieve her hypo-engaged way by spending less time at the patient’s suffering. I feel frantic: I called the psychologist; I called the beside, avoiding conversation (‘‘I’m so busy today.’’), and social worker; I talked to the occupational therapist—what requesting a different assignment. else should we do?’’ Both hypo- and hyper-engagement overlap with other is- The physician’s thoughts suggest a kind of engagement that sues in the clinical literature. For example, chronic, sustained we would call hypo-engagement, because it feels resigned, hypo-engagement may be related to empathic distress, 11,12 passive, and apathetic. In contrast, the nurse’s thoughts vicarious traumatization, or burnout. Alternatively, above suggest a kind of engagement that we would call hyper-engagement may be related to moral distress that is 13,14 hyper-engagement—because it feels anxious, pressured, characterized by ‘‘empathic over-arousal’’ in which the vigilant, even desperate. The physician’s and nurse’s reac- nervous system is stuck on high alert. tions both represent a common way of dealing with the feeling of helplessness, and a common assumption: Don’t Reexperiencing Helplessness as a Moment talk about it, and don’t allow yourself to experience it, be- of Relationality cause that will make the helplessness more real—and worse The second part of our rewrite involves using the reading than it is already. taken by the self-barometer, and reexploring our experience of helplessness. Could we change the experience of help- Hypo- or Hyper-Engagement Can Exacerbate Suffering lessness from one of being ‘‘at the end of the road’’ to being ‘‘in a moment of relationality with a patient’’? Reexperien- There are physiological reasons why clinicians might react cing helplessness as a moment in a clinician-patient rela- to a patient’s helplessness in this way: We unintentionally tionship can enable the clinician to shift to a middle place mirror the emotions of other people.3,4 But these mirrored between hypo- and hyper engagement. Between hypo- and emotions are then filtered through our own perspectives and hyper-engagement, we hypothesize that there is a middle prior experiences. So if we step back and reexamine these place, that we call the zone of constructive engagement. By initial reactions to helplessness, we can see how they can be constructive engagement, we mean a moment in which a read as a barometer of our engagement. In this case, the clinician is willing to take on a challenge, to bring her whole physician’s engagement with the clinical situation is de- self to the bedside, and to relate to the situation as it is—what creasing, whereas the nurse’s engagement is increasing. Both one colleague calls ‘‘being in the muck.’’ In more formal of their initial reactions to their own experiences of help- terms, constructive engagement is the willingness to put in lessness, however, draw them away from the person who is effort in the form of cognitive, emotional, and spiritual work suffering. The physician is backing away from a situation that goes beyond guidelines about professional competence— where medical expertise has little to offer, and the nurse is we are talking about clinical work performed in the service of intensifying her efforts to draw others into the problem- Downloaded by Univ Of Washington from online.liebertpub.com at 08/24/17. For personal use only. an intention to alleviate suffering that invokes deep personal solving process.
Recommended publications
  • Why Feelings Stray: Sources of Affective Misforecasting in Consumer Behavior Vanessa M
    Why Feelings Stray: Sources of Affective Misforecasting in Consumer Behavior Vanessa M. Patrick, University of Georgia Deborah J. MacInnis, University of Southern California ABSTRACT drivers of AMF has considerable import for consumer behavior, Affective misforecasting (AMF) is defined as the gap between particularly in the area of consumer satisfaction, brand loyalty and predicted and experienced affect. Based on prior research that positive word-of-mouth. examines AMF, the current study uses qualitative and quantitative Figure 1 depicts the process by which affective misforecasting data to examine the sources of AMF (i.e., why it occurs) in the occurs (for greater detail see MacInnis, Patrick and Park 2005). As consumption domain. The authors find evidence supporting some Figure 1 suggests, affective forecasts are based on a representation sources of AMF identified in the psychology literature, develop a of a future event and an assessment of the possible affective fuller understanding of others, and, find evidence for novel sources reactions to this event. AMF occurs when experienced affect of AMF not previously explored. Importantly, they find consider- deviates from the forecasted affect on one or more of the following able differences in the sources of AMF depending on whether dimensions: valence, intensity and duration. feelings are worse than or better than forecast. Since forecasts can be made regarding the valence of the feelings, the specific emotions expected to be experienced, the INTRODUCTION intensity of feelings or the duration of a projected affective re- Before purchase: “I can’t wait to use this all the time, it is sponse, consequently affective misforecasting can occur along any going to be so much fun, I’m going to go out with my buddies of these dimensions.
    [Show full text]
  • Classification of Human Emotions from Electroencephalogram (EEG) Signal Using Deep Neural Network
    (IJACSA) International Journal of Advanced Computer Science and Applications, Vol. 8, No. 9, 2017 Classification of Human Emotions from Electroencephalogram (EEG) Signal using Deep Neural Network Abeer Al-Nafjan Areej Al-Wabil College of Computer and Information Sciences Center for Complex Engineering Systems Imam Muhammad bin Saud University King Abdulaziz City for Science and Technology Riyadh, Saudi Arabia Riyadh, Saudi Arabia Manar Hosny Yousef Al-Ohali College of Computer and Information Sciences College of Computer and Information Sciences King Saud University King Saud University Riyadh, Saudi Arabia Riyadh, Saudi Arabia Abstract—Estimation of human emotions from [1]. Recognizing a user‘s affective state can be used to Electroencephalogram (EEG) signals plays a vital role in optimize training and enhancement of the BCI operations [2]. developing robust Brain-Computer Interface (BCI) systems. In our research, we used Deep Neural Network (DNN) to address EEG is often used in BCI research experimentation because EEG-based emotion recognition. This was motivated by the the process is non-invasive to the research subject and minimal recent advances in accuracy and efficiency from applying deep risk is involved. The devices‘ usability, reliability, cost- learning techniques in pattern recognition and classification effectiveness, and the relative convenience of conducting applications. We adapted DNN to identify human emotions of a studies and recruiting participants due to their portability have given EEG signal (DEAP dataset) from power spectral density been cited as factors influencing the increased adoption of this (PSD) and frontal asymmetry features. The proposed approach is method in applied research contexts [3], [4]. These advantages compared to state-of-the-art emotion detection systems on the are often accompanied by challenges such as low spatial same dataset.
    [Show full text]
  • What We Mean When We Talk About Suffering—And Why Eric Cassell Should Not Have the Last Word
    What We Mean When We Talk About Suffering—and Why Eric Cassell Should Not Have the Last Word Tyler Tate, Robert Pearlman Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, Volume 62, Number 1, Winter 2019, pp. 95-110 (Article) Published by Johns Hopkins University Press For additional information about this article https://muse.jhu.edu/article/722412 Access provided at 26 Apr 2019 00:52 GMT from University of Washington @ Seattle What We Mean When We Talk About Suffering—and Why Eric Cassell Should Not Have the Last Word Tyler Tate* and Robert Pearlman† ABSTRACT This paper analyzes the phenomenon of suffering and its relation- ship to medical practice by focusing on the paradigmatic work of Eric Cassell. First, it explains Cassell’s influential model of suffering. Second, it surveys various critiques of Cassell. Next it outlines the authors’ concerns with Cassell’s model: it is aggressive, obscure, and fails to capture important features of the suffering experience. Finally, the authors propose a conceptual framework to help clarify the distinctive nature of sub- jective patient suffering. This framework contains two necessary conditions: (1) a loss of a person’s sense of self, and (2) a negative affective experience. The authors suggest how this framework can be used in the medical encounter to promote clinician-patient communication and the relief of suffering. *Center for Ethics in Health Care and School of Medicine, Oregon Health and Science University, Portland. †National Center for Ethics in Health Care, Washington, DC, and School of Medicine, University of Washington, Seattle. Correspondence: Tyler Tate, Oregon Health and Science University, School of Medicine, Depart- ment of Pediatrics, 3181 SW Sam Jackson Park Road, Portland, OR 97239-3098.
    [Show full text]
  • About Emotions There Are 8 Primary Emotions. You Are Born with These
    About Emotions There are 8 primary emotions. You are born with these emotions wired into your brain. That wiring causes your body to react in certain ways and for you to have certain urges when the emotion arises. Here is a list of primary emotions: Eight Primary Emotions Anger: fury, outrage, wrath, irritability, hostility, resentment and violence. Sadness: grief, sorrow, gloom, melancholy, despair, loneliness, and depression. Fear: anxiety, apprehension, nervousness, dread, fright, and panic. Joy: enjoyment, happiness, relief, bliss, delight, pride, thrill, and ecstasy. Interest: acceptance, friendliness, trust, kindness, affection, love, and devotion. Surprise: shock, astonishment, amazement, astound, and wonder. Disgust: contempt, disdain, scorn, aversion, distaste, and revulsion. Shame: guilt, embarrassment, chagrin, remorse, regret, and contrition. All other emotions are made up by combining these basic 8 emotions. Sometimes we have secondary emotions, an emotional reaction to an emotion. We learn these. Some examples of these are: o Feeling shame when you get angry. o Feeling angry when you have a shame response (e.g., hurt feelings). o Feeling fear when you get angry (maybe you’ve been punished for anger). There are many more. These are NOT wired into our bodies and brains, but are learned from our families, our culture, and others. When you have a secondary emotion, the key is to figure out what the primary emotion, the feeling at the root of your reaction is, so that you can take an action that is most helpful. .
    [Show full text]
  • Emotion Classification Using Physiological Signals
    Emotion Classification Using Physiological Signals Byoung-Jun Park1, Eun-Hye Jang1, Myoung Ae Chung1, Sang-Hyeob Kim1*, Jin Hun Sohn2* 1IT Convergence Technology Research Laboratory, Electronics and Telecommunications Research Institute, Daejeon, 305-700 2Department of Psychology/Brain Research Institute, Chungnam National University, Daejeon, 305-765 ABSTRACT Objective: The aim of this study is to discriminate negative emotions, such as sadness, fear, surprise, and stress using physiological signals. Background: Recently, the main topic of emotion classification research is to recognize human’s feeling or emotion using various physiological signals. It is one of the core processes to implement emotional intelligence in human computer interaction (HCI) research. Method: Electrodermal activity (EDA), electrocardiogram (ECG), skin temperature (SKT), and photoplethysmography (PPG) are recorded and analyzed as physiological signals. And emotional stimuli are audio-visual film clips which have examined for their appropriateness and effectiveness through preliminary experiment. For classification of negative emotions, five machine learning algorithms, i.e., LDF, CART, SOM, and Naïve Bayes are used. Results: Result of emotion classification shows that an accuracy of emotion classification by CART (84.0%) was the highest and by LDA (50.7%) was the lowest. SOM showed emotion classification accuracy of 51.2% and Naïve Bayes was 76.2%. Conclusion: We could identify that CART was the optimal emotion classification algorithm for classifying 4 negative emotions (sadness, fear, surprise, and stress). Application: This result can be helpful to provide the basis for the emotion recognition technique in HCI. Keywords: Emotion classification, Negative emotion, Machine learning algorithm, Physiological signal 1. Introduction Nasoz, Alvarez, Lisetti, and Finkelstein, 2003).
    [Show full text]
  • OWNING YOUR FEELINGS Tips for Success
    OWNING YOUR FEELINGS It can be easy to get caught up in your emotions as you’re feeling them. Most people don’t think about what emotions they are dealing with, but taking the time to really identify what you’re feeling can help you to better cope with challenging situations. The English language has over 3,000 words for Tips for success emotions. Allow yourself to feel. Sometimes there are societal pressures that encourage people to shut down their emotions, often expressed through People who are good at statements like, “Big girls don’t cry,” or “Man up.” These outdated ideas are being specific about harmful, not helpful. Everyone has emotionsthey are part of the human identifying and labeling experienceand you have every right to feel them, regardless of gender, their emotions are less sexual orientation, ethnicity, socio-economic status, race, political likely to binge drink, be affiliation or religion. physically aggressive, or selfinjure when Don’t ignore how you’re feeling. Most of us have heard the term “bottling up distressed. your feelings” before. When we try to push feelings aside without addressing them, they build strength and make us more likely to “explode” at some point in When schoolaged kids are the future. It may not always be appropriate to process your emotions at the taught about emotions for very moment you are feeling them, but try to do so as soon as you can. 20-30 minutes per week their social behavior and Talk it out. Find someone you trust that you can talk to about how you’re school performance feeling.
    [Show full text]
  • Emotion Classification Based on Biophysical Signals and Machine Learning Techniques
    S S symmetry Article Emotion Classification Based on Biophysical Signals and Machine Learning Techniques Oana Bălan 1,* , Gabriela Moise 2 , Livia Petrescu 3 , Alin Moldoveanu 1 , Marius Leordeanu 1 and Florica Moldoveanu 1 1 Faculty of Automatic Control and Computers, University POLITEHNICA of Bucharest, Bucharest 060042, Romania; [email protected] (A.M.); [email protected] (M.L.); fl[email protected] (F.M.) 2 Department of Computer Science, Information Technology, Mathematics and Physics (ITIMF), Petroleum-Gas University of Ploiesti, Ploiesti 100680, Romania; [email protected] 3 Faculty of Biology, University of Bucharest, Bucharest 030014, Romania; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +40722276571 Received: 12 November 2019; Accepted: 18 December 2019; Published: 20 December 2019 Abstract: Emotions constitute an indispensable component of our everyday life. They consist of conscious mental reactions towards objects or situations and are associated with various physiological, behavioral, and cognitive changes. In this paper, we propose a comparative analysis between different machine learning and deep learning techniques, with and without feature selection, for binarily classifying the six basic emotions, namely anger, disgust, fear, joy, sadness, and surprise, into two symmetrical categorical classes (emotion and no emotion), using the physiological recordings and subjective ratings of valence, arousal, and dominance from the DEAP (Dataset for Emotion Analysis using EEG, Physiological and Video Signals) database. The results showed that the maximum classification accuracies for each emotion were: anger: 98.02%, joy:100%, surprise: 96%, disgust: 95%, fear: 90.75%, and sadness: 90.08%. In the case of four emotions (anger, disgust, fear, and sadness), the classification accuracies were higher without feature selection.
    [Show full text]
  • On Emotion Specificity in Decision Making
    Judgment and Decision Making, Vol. 3, No. 1, January 2008, pp. 18–27. On emotion specificity in decision making: Why feeling is for doing Marcel Zeelenberg∗1, Rob M. A. Nelissen1, Seger M. Breugelmans2, & Rik Pieters3 1 Department of Social Psychology and TIBER, Tilburg University 2 Department of Developmental, Clinical and Cross-cultural Psychology, Tilburg University 3 Department of Marketing and TIBER, Tilburg University Abstract We present a motivational account of the impact of emotion on decision making, termed the feeling-is-for-doing approach. We first describe the psychology of emotion and argue for a need to be specific when studying emotion’s impact on decision making. Next we describe what our approach entails and how it relates emotion, via motivation to behavior. Then we offer two illustrations of our own research that provide support for two important elements in our reasoning. We end with specifying four criteria that we consider to be important when studying how feeling guides our everyday doing. Keywords: emotion, decision making, motivation, action. 1 Introduction 1988). Bounded rationality may be helped by the exis- tence of emotion, because emotions restrict the size of the Most theories of rational decision making are descrip- consideration set and focus the decision maker on certain, tively implausible, especially if taken as process models. relevant aspects of the options (Hanoch, 2001). Emotions The idea that we take the time and invest the effort to pro- assign value to objects, aid the learning of how to ob- duce a list of advantages and disadvantages, or costs and tain those objects, and provide the motivation for doing benefits for all alternatives in each single decision and so (Gifford, 2002).
    [Show full text]
  • The Thinking-Feeling Connection
    Back from the Back from the Bluez Module 3 The Thinking-Feeling Connection The Thinking-Feeling Connection 2 Making the Connection 4 Module Summary 6 About the Modules 7 Centre for Clinical Module 3: The Thinking-Feeling Connection Page 1 nterventions •Psychotherapy•ReI search•Training Back from the The Thinking-Feeling Connection People often believe that the feelings and emotions they experience are determined by external events, situations, and the behaviour of others. For example, we may hear ourselves say, “My boss made me so nervous,” “My partner made me so angry,” “This trip down south made me feel so relaxed,” or “I’m depressed because I didn’t get the job I wanted.” What is the assumption underlying these statements? That someone or something other than ourselves was directly determining the feelings we experienced. We come to these conclusions automatically without asking ourselves if this assumption is true. However, if we stop to analyse the process that links an external situation to our emotional responses, we will find that there is a step in between. How Our Thoughts Influence Our Feelings What really makes us feel and respond the way we do, is often not the situation or the words and actions of another person, but how we perceive that situation or that person’s actions. It is how we see something or someone and what we think about it or them that really influences how we feel. It is our thoughts and beliefs about an event that significantly influences our emotions and actions. Here’s an example. Suppose you went to a party and your host introduces you to Mike.
    [Show full text]
  • How Emotion Enhances the Feeling of Remembering
    ARTICLES How emotion enhances the feeling of remembering Tali Sharot, Mauricio R Delgado & Elizabeth A Phelps Studies examining memories of arousing ‘real-life’ events show that emotion heightens the feeling of remembering, without necessarily enhancing the objective accuracy of the memories. We measured brain activity associated with the feeling of remembering emotional and neutral photos. Subjects indicated whether recognition was accompanied by a recollection of details about the study episode (‘remember’) or not (‘know’). ‘Remember’ judgments were boosted for emotional photos, but accuracy did not differ. For neutral photos, ‘remember’ judgments were related to enhanced activity in the parahippocampal cortex, previously related to recognition of visual details, which one might expect to supply the retrieval clues for a ‘remember’ judgment. In contrast, ‘remember’ judgments for emotional photos were associated with enhanced activity in the amygdala, suggesting that subjects rely on arousal and perceptual fluency to evaluate these memories. For the first time, we identify the neural mechanisms underlying the enhanced feeling of remembering for emotional events. http://www.nature.com/natureneuroscience Highly emotional events are often reported to be remembered clearly, studies9–11 support the notion that ‘remembering’ engages neural sub- vividly and with great detail. For this reason it has been suggested that, strates that are similar to those underlying associative retrieval12,13, such similar to a camera’s flashbulb, emotion triggers a mechanism that con- as the hippocampus and parahippocampal cortex, and are different serves what occurred at that instant, so that the memory stays true to the from those underlying ‘knowing’. original incident1. However, an emerging body of evidence2,3 indicates Findings of a recent behavioral study14 suggest that emotion affects that the feeling of remembering is heightened with emotion, so that the ‘remember’ and ‘know’ judgments differently.
    [Show full text]
  • Emotion in Schizophrenia: Where Feeling Meets Thinking
    Current Directions in Psychological Science Emotion in Schizophrenia: Where 19(4) 255-259 ª The Author(s) 2010 Reprints and permission: Feeling Meets Thinking sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0963721410377599 http://cdps.sagepub.com Ann M. Kring and Janelle M. Caponigro University of California, Berkeley Abstract Our understanding of the nature of emotional difficulties in schizophrenia has been greatly enhanced by translational research over the past two decades. By incorporating methods and theories from affective science, researchers have been able to discover that people with schizophrenia exhibit very few outward displays of emotion but report experiencing strong feelings in the presence of emotionally evocative stimuli or events. Recent behavioral, psychophysiological, and brain imaging research has pointed to the importance of considering the time course of emotion in schizophrenia. This work has shown that people with schizophrenia have the ability to experience emotion in the moment; however, they appear to have difficulties when anticipating future pleasurable experiences, and this perhaps affects their motivation to have such experiences. While advancements in our understanding of emotional experience and expression in individuals with schizophrenia have been made, these developments have led to a new collection of research questions directed at understanding the time course of emotion in schizophrenia, including the role of memory and anticipation in motivated behavior, translating laboratory findings to the development of new assessment tools and new treatments targeting emotional impairments in people with this disorder. Keywords schizophrenia, emotion, anticipation, memory Schizophrenia is a disorder that impacts many domains. Some new avenues for the assessment and treatment of emotional of its more recognizable symptoms involve difficulties in difficulties in schizophrenia are now under way.
    [Show full text]
  • Feeling Pain and Being in Pain
    Hanse-Studien / Hanse Studies Hanse-Wissenschaftskolleg Delmenhorst Hanse Institute for Advanced Study Band 1 / Volume 1 Nikola Grahek Feeling Pain and Being in Pain bis Bibliotheks- und Informationssystem der Universität Oldenburg 2001 Hanse-Studien / Hanse Studies Hanse-Wissenschaftskolleg Delmenhorst Hanse Institute for Advanced Study herausgegeben von Prof. Dr. Dr. Gerhard Roth und Uwe Opolka In der Reihe Hanse-Studien / Hanse Studies erscheinen – in deutscher oder englischer Sprache – unveröffentlichte Forschungsarbeiten, die am Hanse-Wissenschaftskolleg (HWK) in Delmenhorst entstanden sind, sowie Berichte über vom HWK durchgeführte Konferenzen. Das Hanse-Wissenschaftskolleg ist eine gemeinnützige Stiftung privaten Rechts der Länder Bremen und Niedersachsen sowie der Stadt Delmenhorst. Es wurde 1995 gegründet und nahm 1997 seine Arbeit auf. Seine Hauptaufgabe besteht in der Stärkung des überregional und international anerkannten Forschungspotentials der umliegenden Universitäten und Forschungseinrichtungen, insbesondere der Universitäten Oldenburg und Bremen. Seine derzeitigen Arbeitsschwerpunkte liegen auf den Gebieten Meeres- und Klimaforschung, Neuro- und Kognitionswissenschaften, Sozialwissenschaften / Sozialpolitik sowie auf interdisziplinären Projekten. In diesen Bereichen beruft es Fellows und führt Tagungen durch. Anschriften der Herausgeber: Prof. Dr. Dr. Gerhard Roth Uwe Opolka Hanse-Wissenschaftskolleg Hanse-Wissenschaftskolleg Lehmkuhlenbusch 4 Lehmkuhlenbusch 4 27753 Delmenhorst 27753 Delmenhorst Telefon: 0 42 21/91 60-108 Telefon: 0 42 21/91 60-109 Telefax: 0 42 21/91 60-199 Telefax: 0 42 21/91 60-199 e-mail: [email protected] e-mail: [email protected] Verlag/Druck/ Bibliotheks- und Informationssystem der Vertrieb Carl von Ossietzky Universität Oldenburg (BIS) – Verlag Postfach 2541, 26015 Oldenburg Telefon: +49-4 41-7 98-22 61 Telefax: +49-4 41-7 98-40 40 e-mail: [email protected] ISBN 3-8142-0780-7 3 Contents Acknowledgments 1.
    [Show full text]