I. Background II

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

I. Background II Report of the External Review Committee for the Department of Psychology Ohio State University November 22, 2010 Committee Members: Michael Domjan, University of Texas at Austin Richard Gonzalez, University of Michigan Lynn Hasher, University of Toronto E. Tory Higgins, Columbia University Members of the External Review Committee visited Ohio State University October 20 to October 22, 2010. During the visit they met with the Chair of the Department of Psychology (Richard Petty), and faculty in various areas of the Department (Social, Quantitative, Developmental, Cognition, Behavioral Neuroscience, and Clinical). They also met separately with all untenured tenure- track faculty, groups of graduate and undergraduate students, select staff members, members of the Departmental Graduate Studies Office, members of the Departmental Undergraduate Studies Office, Dean of Social and Behavioral Sciences Gifford Weary and her senior staff, Vice-Provost and Executive Dean of Arts and Sciences Joseph Steinmetz, Executive Vice President and Provost Joseph Alutto and his senior staff, Vice Provost and Dean of the Graduate School Patrick Osmer, and the Vice President for Research Caroline Whitacre. This report is based on those meetings as well as the extensive self-studies that were prepared by the current and previous chair of the Psychology Department (Richard Petty and Gifford Weary, respectively) with help and input from other psychology faculty. The report is organized in the following sections: I. Background II. Current Interdisciplinary Activities III. Strengthening Cross-Cutting Links within Psychology IV. Strengthening Links between Psychology and other Related Disciplines V. Role and Treatment of New Faculty VI. Minority Faculty Recruitment VII. Graduate Training in the Interdisciplinary Age VIII. 2010 NRC Rankings of Psychology at Ohio State I. Background The Department of Psychology at Ohio State University has a long and distinguished history. Numerous internationally prominent psychologists have been on the faculty of Ohio State or received their training at OSU, including Carl Rogers, Julian Rotter, Claude Steele, John Caccioppo, Walter Mischel, Tony Greenwald, and 2 George Kelly. The quest for excellence continues today. The Department has an outstanding record. The faculty are excited and ambitious. The proportion of faculty with funded research projects is outstanding. New faculty hires are all among the elite in their specialties. Given the size of the faculty, the Department teaches a remarkably large number of Ph.D. students (about 145) and has an excellent record of placing these students in good positions after graduation. The Department also carries a huge undergraduate teaching burden. Currently there are about 1800 students majoring in psychology, and numerous non-majors have contact with the Department when they take Psychology 100. Despite the large numbers of students served, the Department provides considerable personal attention to undergraduates. The Psychology 100 is taught in relatively small sections (80 students each). Numerous undergraduates also participate in the honors program, which provides research and practicum experiences. The faculty and staff should be commended on this impressive achievement. Ohio State achieved its eminence in psychology by focusing on a number of core areas of psychology (social, clinical, cognition, quantitative methods, behavioral neuroscience, and developmental), and building strong research and graduate training programs in these areas. This was the “standard” model in the field of psychology for much of the twentieth century, as research psychologists worked hard to establish psychology as a core science discipline. Through those efforts, psychology became the prominent fulcrum among the sciences, positioned between the social sciences on the one hand and the biological and physical sciences on the other. Having become a core science discipline, with the largest student enrollment among the sciences at many universities, psychology is now at a crossroad. There is much important work left to be done in the core areas of psychology. However, the position of the discipline at the interface between the social and biological sciences has also created numerous new and exciting opportunities for interdisciplinary research. Psychology is fundamentally concerned with human emotion, cognition and behavior. How we think and act is integrally involved in the solution of all major societal problems. As Steven Jobs well knows, modern technology is effective only if it is user friendly and people are wiling to put the technology into practice. Efforts to reduce waste or shift to new transportation strategies are successful only to the extent that they can change age-old behavioral habits. Public health and preventative medicine work only if they successfully interface with people’s behavior and encourage healthier life styles. The success of the most spectacular medical interventions or new pharmaceuticals is similarly limited by compliance with medical instructions and changes in behavior that promote recovery. Psychology is now well positioned to extend the frontiers of knowledge by joining with allied sciences to solve problems that no one discipline can solve alone. Rising rates of obesity and diabetes will not be arrested without involving psychologists. For example, what psychologists have discovered about learning and memory can help design new educational strategies. In addition to the medical sciences, nursing, public health, and education, psychologists can now work effectively with linguistics, communication sciences and disorders, human development and family sciences, integrative biology and anthropology, molecular 3 biology and neuroscience, pharmacology, computer science, and biomedical engineering. Such interdisciplinary collaborations promise to advance knowledge and solve intractable societal problems. Interdisciplinary activities are only successful if they are strongly rooted in the excellence of each of the contributing disciplines. Thus, a focus on interdisciplinary research requires nurturing each of the participating fields of inquiry. This becomes a balancing act. Core disciplines cannot be ignored in the pursuit of interdisciplinary work, but attention to each core should not stifle the interdisciplinary activity. Learning how to nurture each core while simultaneosly facilitating links between cores is a new challenge for faculty, departments, and universities. The Psychology Department at Ohio State University has begun the tackle these challenges. The faculty recognize and embrace the proposition that interdisciplinary research and teaching is important for the future success of psychology, and they have taken a number of major steps in that direction. However, it is our opinion that they have not yet achieved a comfortable balance between nurturing the core and reaching out to allied disciplines. In the following sections of this report, we will offer suggestions to achieve these long-term objectives in a manner that will result in a more comfortable and productive balance. II. Current Interdisciplinary Activities The review committee applauds the Department’s priority on interdisciplinary research. Throughout our visit we heard about the many examples of collaborative research between faculty across different areas of psychology, and heard about research projects involving units outside of psychology. The interdisciplinary emphasis was infused throughout the self-study report in both an organizing and generative manner. For example, interdisciplinarity provided an organizing framework for the new hiring initiatives in Neuroscience, Health, and Judgment and Decision Making discussed in the self-study report. Each of these initiatives builds on strengths within the department as well as across campus, providing an opportunity to take the department into new research areas. Given the department’s strength in these areas and the relevance to the national funding agenda, the interdisciplinary framework positions OSU’s Psychology Department to continue being a top department and to move to “eminence.” We encourage additional investment of resources in these areas. In the next section, we suggest several points that will help the department implement these interdisciplinary, cross-cutting links, to maximize chances for success. III. Strengthening Cross-Cutting Links within Psychology In recent years a consensus has emerged within the Department that it needs to move from being a collection of area silos (or even independent labs) to being a group of faculty and students who identify with the Department as a whole. Some 4 progress toward strengthening the cross-cutting links of the Department in this way has already been made: “…a major new development is the increasing number of faculty in the department who are working with fellow faculty in the department across area boundaries.” But more progress is needed. Our impression from our meetings with faculty and students is that the potential impediment to a faster rate of progress does not derive from active resistance from members of the Department but, rather, from long-term habits of working independently rather than as members of the Department as a whole. We have some suggestions for strengthening the Department as an integrated whole—a whole that is greater than the sum of its parts—that will not only lay a solid foundation for the emerging interdisciplinary Departmental initiatives (e.g., Neuroscience,
Recommended publications
  • Using Research to Inform Policies and Practices in Science Education: Conversations with Faculty and Administrators
    Using Research to Inform Policies and Practices in Science Education: Conversations With Faculty and Administrators Postsymposium Report This material is based upon work supported by the National Science Foundation under research grant HRD-1029477. Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Science Foundation. September 2014 Using Research to Inform Policies and Practices in Science Education: Conversations With Faculty and Administrators Postsymposium Report September 2014 1000 Thomas Jefferson Street NW Washington, DC 20007-3835 202.403.5000 | TTY 877.334.3499 www.air.org Copyright © 2014 American Institutes for Research. All rights reserved. 3254_09/14 Acknowledgments American Institutes for Research (AIR) extends its gratitude to the symposium speakers and participants who donated their time and participated in these important discussions. We also extend our appreciation to the National Science Foundation Program Directors and Project Officers in the Human Resources Division and the Education and Human Resources Directorate. We acknowledge the work on and dedication of the AIR staff who contributed so generously to organizing and facilitating this symposium, and the writing of this postsymposium report: Courtney Tanenbaum, M.A., Principal Investigator (current) and Project Director; Carlos Rodriguez, Ph.D., Principal Investigator (former); Rita Kirshstein, Ph.D., Senior Advisor; Andrea Berger, Ph.D., Project
    [Show full text]
  • Stereotype Threat: Contending and Coping with Unnerving Expectations
    CHAPTER Stereotype Threat: Contending and Coping with Unnerving Expectations JOSHUA ARONSON Department of Applied Psychology, New York University, New York When my friends and I entered junior high school in the mid~1970s, we quickly noticed something that our teachers seemed to miss: the black and Latino kids bused into our district were a lot smarter than the adults at the school seemed to think. It was not necessarily that our teachers held the prejudiced view that minorities were dumb. Rather, as students, we had the advantage of seeing these kids in a variety of classes and situations. And their behavior from one situation to the next was often remarkably different. For example, there were many kids like Ricky, the star pupil in my electronics class. Always the first to finish his project, Ricky would go from table to table, patiently helping the electronically challenged kids like me make sense of the complex diagrams for assembling the strobe light, radio, or whatever we happened to be struggling with. There were also kids like my friend Darryl, a black eighth grader, whom I hung around with at lunch and after school. Darryl was one of the most thoughtful, intelligent, and articulate kids I knew, and no one who knew him well thought otherwise. But in some of the classes I had with Ricky and Darryl, you would never get an inkling of how bright they were. In these more "aca- demic" classes like English, Math, and History, they simply ceased to be the same kids we knew. Like most of the minority students in the school, they fell into one of two camps.
    [Show full text]
  • Stanfordeducator Education Alumni Summer 2012 Newsletter
    Stanford University School of StanfordEducator Education Alumni Summer 2012 Newsletter Welcoming Our New Dean An Interview with Claude Steele Steve Castillo By Amy Yuen and Mindy Hollar laude Steele stepped into his new role as the I. James Quillen Dean for Stanford University School of Education on September 1, 2011. Steele, who served as the twenty-first provost of Columbia University, is Ca preeminent social psychologist. Prior to assuming the position of chief academic officer at Columbia, he was a member of the Stanford psychology faculty from 1991 to 2009. On The Farm, he held appointments as the Lucie Stern Professor in the Social A Return to the Farm: Claude Steele at the Cubberley Library. Sciences, chair of the Psychology Department, director of the Center for Comparative Studies in Race and Ethnicity, and director of the Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioral Sciences. His research focuses on the psychological experience of the individual, Table of Contents and particularly, on the experience of threats to the self and Inside Page the consequences of those threats. His book, Whistling Vivaldi: Welcoming Our New Dean 1 How Stereotypes Affect Us and What We Can Do, examines his 2 theory of stereotype threat, which has been the focus of much of School of Education Launches Lemann Center his research for the past 20 years. Steele earned his doctorate in End of the American Dream? 3 psychology from Ohio State University, and has received honorary School Reform Gets Real World 5 degrees from the University of Michigan, the University of Chicago, “Learning to Be Jewish” 8 Yale University, Princeton University, and the Faculty News 11 University of Maryland, Baltimore County.
    [Show full text]
  • Review: C. Steele. 2010. Whistling Vivaldi
    Jarrod Call University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, U.S. C. Steele. 2010. Whistling Vivaldi: And other clues to how stereotypes affect us. W. W. Norton & Company, Inc. 242 pp., (hardcover). ISBN: 978-0393-06249-6 Keywords: academic performance, stereotype threat, identity, social psy- chology, achievement gap With the ever increasing diversity job at the local Country Club as a of our societies, Whistling Vivaldi golf caddy. After being ignored all offers a perspective on one of the day standing outside the gated en- more relevant issues of our time: trance he began to question wheth- how stereotypes affect us. The au- er his identity limited him access to thor, Claude Steele, is a social psy- some opportunities. Claude is black, chologist with a doctorate from the and in Chicago during the 1950s, Ohio State University, and is cur- black children were relegated cer- rently provost at Columbia Univer- tain times they could visit swim- sity. Dr. Steele has received many ming pools and skating rinks; and academic and professional acco- they most certainly could not carry lades, most notably two awards for golf clubs at the Country Club. Dr. Distinguished Scientific Contribu- Steele’s experience led him to theo- tions from the American Psychology rize on how identity can alter one’s Association and American Psychol- success in a given society, not only ogy Society, respectively. Central to by limiting an individual’s access to his accomplishments is his quest certain opportunities, but also by af- to better understand the nature of fecting one’s performance. race, gender, and ethnicity and their Identity contingencies are, as impacts on the individual and soci- discussed by Dr.
    [Show full text]
  • Discussion Questions for Whistling Vivaldi I. Steele Writes That
    Discussion Questions for Whistling Vivaldi I. Steele writes that stereotype threat can often cause people to feel intense levels of anxiety that, through no fault of their own, can restrict a person from reaching his/her potential. In turn, feeling that you do not belong in a certain space can make it difficult for to remain in that setting, and to sustain your motivation to stay there. (p.111) • Have you or someone close to you ever felt that you do not belong somewhere? • What impact did that have and what would have helped to make you feel that you belong? • How does segregation relate to underrepresentation? • In hindsight, when could you have felt this way but didn’t – and what was the difference that made you buck the trend? II. Claude Steele calls out segregation in U.S. society saying: “Segregation remains a major feature of American life. The 2000 census shows that the average White American lives in a neighborhood that is 80 percent white and 7 percent black while the average black American lives in a neighborhood that is 33 percent white and 51 percent black. This holds for suburbs as much as cities. What are the implications for students in our present day schools? People, though capable of making decisions and choices, do have a location in society; their lives are located somewhere in its social, economic, and cultural structures and in the networks of relationships that make up society. Being born into a low-income Appalachian family in the hills of Eastern Kentucky is to take life on from a different location in society’s opportunity structure than being born into a high-income family in the northern suburbs of Chicago.
    [Show full text]
  • Foundations of an Inclusive Mindset
    Beyond Recruitment: Inclusion, Equity, and Belonging in Externships INCLUSION DESIGN GROUP DR. ERIN CONNER Inclusive Mindset Be Brave. Be Humble. Be Dedicated. Safe and Brave Space Comfort Zone Constructive Discomfort •Adapted from University of Michigan’s Intergroup Dialogue (IDI) Institute Diversity Inclusion Belonging Equity Diversity • Representation Inclusion • Opportunity & Access Belonging • Authenticity & Culture Equity • Internal & External Systems DIBE SUPERVISORS MATTER MOST 1 Success! 3 2 6 Diversity Social Identity A person’s sense of who they are based on their group membership. Which three words best describe your social identity? Social Identity Groups Age Physical Race Religious Gender Ability Beliefs Marital/ Nationality Ethnicity Parenting Body Type Status Sexual Orientation Citizenship Education Class Privilege Understand your position in relationship to others’ experiences Identify opportunities to leverage your privilege on behalf of others Intersectionality Intersectionality is a framework for thinking about power and identity. -- Kimberle Crenshaw Bias Prejudice Discrimination Oppression Bias Prejudice Discrimination Oppression Consciousness Bias Prejudice Power Discrimination Oppression Inclusion Unrelated to Passive, business indirect Low Quality outcomes Feedback Focused on style over Delayed substance Unactionable Absent 21 Tied to Specific, business concrete High Quality outcomes Feedback Tied to career Timely aspirations Not about Actionable personality 22 Disproportionate Feedback ▪Women have been found less likely to receive specific feedback tied to outcomes ▪Only 12% of women report being satisfied with the quality of the feedback they receive. ▪Professionals of color are much less likely to receive feedback than their Caucasian counterparts—and even when they do, they’re unclear as to how to act on it, particularly if they were born outside the U.S.
    [Show full text]
  • Our Unconscious Mind
    PSYCHOLOGY OurUnconscıous Unconscious impulses and desires impel Mindwhat we think and do in ways Freud never dreamed of By John A. Bargh 32 Scientific American, January 2014 Illustrations by Tim Bower Photograph by Tktk Tktk Illustration by Artist Name January 2014, ScientificAmerican.com 33 John A. Bargh is a professor of psychology at Yale University. His Automaticity in Cognition, Motivation, and Evaluation Lab at Yale researches unconscious influences on behavior and questions such as the extent to which free will really exists. hen psychologists try to understand the way our mind works, they frequently come to a conclusion that may seem startling: people W often make decisions without having given them much thought—or, more precisely, before they have thought about them consciously. When we decide how to vote, what to buy, where to go on vacation and myriad other things, unconscious thoughts that we are not even aware of typically play a big role. Research has recently brought to light just how profoundly our unconscious mind shapes our day-to-day interactions. One of the best-known studies to illustrate the power of the For more than 100 years the role of unconscious influences on unconscious focused on the process of deciding whether a candi- our thoughts and actions has preoccupied scientists who study date was fit to hold public office. A group of mock voters were giv- the mind. Sigmund Freud’s massive body of work emphasized en a split second to inspect portrait photographs from the Inter- the conscious as the locus of rational thought and emotion and net of U.S.
    [Show full text]
  • Conference Program Weary Symposium on Diversity and Social Identity the Ohio State University June 18-19, 2019 Columbus, OH
    Conference Program Weary Symposium on Diversity and Social Identity The Ohio State University June 18-19, 2019 Columbus, OH Tuesday June 18, 2019 Wednesday June 19, 2019 9:30-10:00 Breakfast 9:00-9:30 Breakfast 9:55-10:00 Opening Remarks 9:25-9:30 Opening Remarks 10:00-11:00 Dr. Mary Murphy 9:30-10:30 Dr. Sylvia Perry 11:00-12:00 Dr. Jason Okonofua 10:30-11:30 Dr. Hilary Bergsieker 12:00-1:00 Lunch 11:30-12:00 Data Blitz #2 (pg. 9) 12:30-1:30 Poster Session #1 (pg. 3) 12:00-1:00 Lunch 1:30-2:00 Data Blitz #1 (pg. 5) 12:30-1:30 Poster Session #2 (pg. 10) 2:00-3:00 Dr. Denise Sekaquaptewa 1:30-2:30 Early Career Talks #2 (pg. 12) 3:00-3:15 Break 2:30-3:30 Dr. Claude Steele 3:15-4:15 Early Career Talks #1 (pg. 6) 3:30-3:45 Wrap-Up 4:15-5:15 Dr. Jennifer Richeson 5:15-5:30 Wrap-Up Social Hour/Networking 6:00-7:30 The Eagle 790 N High St. Columbus, OH Conference Dinner 7:30-9:30 Hubbard Grille 793 N High St. Columbus, OH 1 Tuesday June 18, 2019 Distinguished Speaker Tuesday 10:00AM-11:00AM Dr. Mary C. Murphy, Indiana University - Bloomington Dr. Mary Murphy is the H.B. Wells Professor in the Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences and Associate Vice Provost for Diversity and Inclusion at Indiana University. Her research focuses on understanding how people’s social identities and group memberships, such as their gender, race, and socio-economic status, interact with the contexts they encounter to affect people’s thoughts, feelings, motivation, and performance.
    [Show full text]
  • Empirically Validated Strategies to Reduce Stereotype Threat Below Is a Brief List of Empirically Validated Strategies to Reduce Stereotype Threat
    Empirically Validated Strategies to Reduce Stereotype Threat Below is a brief list of empirically validated strategies to reduce stereotype threat. For reviews of these strategies, see: -Cohen, G. L., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Garcia, J. (2012). An identity threat perspective on intervention. In Stereotype threat (Inzlicht, Schmader, Eds.). (downloadable here: http://ed.stanford.edu/faculty/glc ) -Garcia, J., & Cohen, G. L. (in press). A social-psychological approach to educational intervention. In Behavioral foundations of policy (Shafir, Ed.) (downloadable here: http://ed.stanford.edu/faculty/glc -Steele, C. M. (2010). Whistling Vivaldi: And other clues how stereotypes affect us. New York: Norton & Company Inc. -Steele, D. M. (2012). Creating identity safe classrooms. In J. A. Banks (Ed.) Encyclopedia of Diversity in Education. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. -Walton, G. M., Spencer, S. J., & Erman, S. (in press). Affirmative meritocracy. Social Issues and Policy Review. (downloadable here: http://www.stanford.edu/~gwalton/home/Publications.html). -Yeager, D. S., & Walton, G. M. (2011). Social-psychological interventions in education: They’re not magic. Review of Educational Research, 81, 267-301. (downloadable here: http://www.stanford.edu/~gwalton/home/Publications.html). 1. Remove Cues That Trigger Worries About Stereotypes • Reduce prejudice (Logel et al., 2009); remove physical cues that make it seem that a school setting is defined by the majority group (Cheryan et al, 2009); don’t ask people to report a negatively stereotyped group identity immediately before taking a test (Steele & Aronson, 1995; Danaher & Crandall, 2008) 2. Convey That Diversity is Valued • For instance, communicate a multicultural ideology that explicitly values diversity (Purdie-Vaughns et al., 2008) 3.
    [Show full text]
  • Claude Steele Lucie Stern Professor in the Social Sciences, Emeritus Psychology
    Claude Steele Lucie Stern Professor in the Social Sciences, Emeritus Psychology Bio BIO Claude M. Steele is an American social psychologist and a Professor of Psychology at Stanford University. He is best known for his work on stereotype threat and its application to minority student academic performance. His earlier work dealt with research on the self (e.g., self-image, self- affirmation) as well as the role of self-regulation in addictive behaviors. In 2010, he released his book, Whistling Vivaldi and Other Clues to How Stereotypes Affect Us, summarizing years of research on stereotype threat and the underperformance of minority students in higher education. He holds B.A. in Psychology from Hiram College, an M.A. in Social Psychology from Ohio State University, and a Ph.D. in Social Psychology and Statistical Psychology from Ohio State University. He is elected to the American Academy of Arts and Sciences, the National Academy of Sciences, the National Science Board, the National Academy of Education, and the American Philosophical Society. He currently serves as a trustee of the Russell Sage Foundation and the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation, and as a Fellow for both the American Institutes for Research and the American Academy of Political and Social Science. He has served in several major academic leadership positions as the Executive Vice Chancellor and Provost at UC Berkeley, the I. James Quillen Dean for the School of Education at Stanford University, and as the 21st Provost of Columbia University. Past roles also include serving as the President of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, as the President of the Western Psychological Association, and as a member of the Board of Directors of the American Psychological Society.
    [Show full text]
  • Unmasking Stereotype Threat & Impostor Syndrome
    Unmasking Stereotype Threat & Impostor Syndrome Facilitators: Pamela Nolan Young - [email protected] ​ Dominique Vargas - [email protected] ​ Goals Upon successful completion of this workshop, you will: ● Learn the definitions of Stereotype Threat and Impostor Syndrome ● Identify the signs and symptoms of Stereotype Threat and Impostor Syndrome in students and in ourselves ● Build a repertoire of strategies to help mitigate Stereotype Threat and combat Impostor Syndrome for students and ourselves Exercise 1: What does a competent, successful undergraduate student in your discipline look and act ​ ​ like? A graduate student? A faculty member? Stereotype Threat According to Claude Steele and Jeffrey Aronson (1995), stereotype threat is a situational predicament in which people are or feel themselves to be at risk of confirming, as a 1 self-characteristic, a negative stereotype about one’s social group/identity. Stereotype threat can be a contributing factor to long-standing racial and gender gaps in academic performance. It is important to note that anyone can be vulnerable to stereotype threat in specific circumstances What leads to Stereotype Threat? ● Group identity salience: When one’s stereotyped group status is made relevant or ​ conspicuous by situational features, stereotype threat and performance decrements are more likely. Because stereotype threat arises from negative performance expectations in a specific domain, any group can show evidence of underperformance if the situation brings attention to the threatened identity. In other words, although stereotype threat tends to be experienced by members of some groups more than others, it would be inappropriate to conclude that it is only experienced by members of traditionally stigmatized or stereotyped groups.
    [Show full text]
  • Curriculum Vitae CLAUDE MASON STEELE Professor Stanford University Department of Psychology 450 Serra Mall, Bldg
    Curriculum Vitae CLAUDE MASON STEELE Professor Stanford University Department of Psychology 450 Serra Mall, Bldg. 420; Stanford, CA 94305-2130 EDUCATION: B.A. Hiram College, Hiram, Ohio, June, 1967 Major in Psychology M.A. Ohio State University, June, 1969 Major in Social Psychology Ph.D. Ohio State University, September, 1971 Major in Social Psychology Minor in Statistical Psychology AWARDS: 1994-95 Cattell Faculty Fellowship, The James McKeen Cattell Fund 1995 Dean’s Teaching Award, Stanford University 1996 Elected to The American Academy of Arts and Sciences 1997 Gordon Allport Prize in Social Psychology, Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues 1998 Elected to The National Academy of Education 2000 Honorary Doctorate, University of Chicago 2000 William James Fellow Award for Distinguished Scientific Career Contribution, American Psychological Society 2001 Donald Campbell Award, Society for Personality and Social Psychology 2002 Honorary Doctorate, Yale University 2002 Kurt Lewin Memorial Award, Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues 2002 Senior Award for Distinguished Contributions to Psychology in the Public Interest, American Psychological Association 2003 Distinguished Scientific Contribution Award, American Psychological Assoc. 2003 Elected to The National Academy of Sciences 2004 Columbia Teachers College Medal for Distinguished Service 2006 Honorary Doctorate, University of Michigan 2007 Distinguished Scientific Impact Award, The Society of Experimental Social Psychology. (For “Threat in the Air”) 2007
    [Show full text]