Distal Attribution in Sensory Substitution

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Distal Attribution in Sensory Substitution SPACING OUT: DISTAL ATTRIBUTION IN SENSORY SUBSTITUTION ____________________________________ A Thesis Presented to The Honors Tutorial College Ohio University _______________________________________ In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for Graduation from the Honors Tutorial College with the degree of Bachelor of Arts in Philosophy ______________________________________ by David E. Pence May 2013 Contents 1. Introduction: The ABCs of SSDs 1 1.1 More Background: Sensory Substitution and Enactivism 4 2. Why Action May not be Necessary 10 2.1 Seeing Without Structure 13 2.2 Seeing Without Corroboration? 18 2.3 A Thought Experiment 22 3. Alternative Frameworks for Sensory Substitution 25 3.1 The Prospects Considered 32 3.2. How it’s Done 32 3.3 Where it Happens 37 4. Crossmodal Plasticity 40 4.1 The Mechanisms of Plasticity 41 4.2 Where the Senses Trade 46 4.3 Acquired Synaesthesia? 48 5. The Multimodal Mind 52 5.1 Where it All Comes Together 53 5.2 Integration at Work 57 5.3 A Second Multimodal Proposal 67 6. Mental Imagery 71 6.1 The Step-by-Step 72 6.2 The Land of Imagination 77 6.3 Speaking For and Against 79 6.4 Does Reading Make a Better Metaphor? 87 7. How They All Fit Together 93 8. Conclusions: Lessons and Future Possibilities 97 9. References 100 List of Figures Figure 1: Retinal Disparity 9 Figure 2: Gibsonian Ecological Perception 9 Figure 3: High Contrast Image 14 Figure 4: Resolution of Early TVSS 16 Figure 5: Early TVSS Device 19 Figure 6: Working Memory Map 34 Figure 7: Map of Brain Regions Involved in Sensory Substitution 39 Figure 8: Multisensory Reverse Hierarchy Theory 69 Figure 9: Resolution of The vOICe 89 1 1. Introduction: The ABCs of SSDs One would be hard pressed to find a psychological result as surprising as sensory substitution. The basic idea is that, by translating sensory stimuli from one modality to another, one can regain lost or damaged perceptual capacities. Sensory substitution devices (SSDs) will turn images into vibrations on a tactile grid or variations in an auditory “soundscape,” and when suitably processed, these inputs are used for adaptive world-directed action, spatial navigation, and even object recognition. The blind, so many claim, can learn to see.1 Most significant among the gains shown by SSD users is the ability to perceive objects beyond their bodies, things “out there.” Sighted persons often take for granted their ability to sense and interact with the world of extrapersonal space, but for early and congenitally blind (i.e., blind from birth) users, the gain is massive. In the lab, users are often surprised to encounter depth and optical illusions (Bach-y-Rita, 1972); in everyday life, they might be surprised to “see” household objects long forgotten or distant trees on a walk outside.2 These perceptual capacities all fall under what psychologists have called “distal attribution” (Epstein et al., 1986; Auvray et al., 2003). Various definitions have been proposed, but it can be broadly understood as the ability to represent objects as occupying spatial locations beyond one’s own body. As surprising as the result itself, however, is the fact that despite over four decades of research, sensory substitution remains so mysterious. There is no orthodox explanation of what, exactly, is going on when subjects learn to use an SSD. The most 1 Sensory substitution is not limited to vision, but to date, the majority of research has centered on it. 2See (“what blind users say,” “www.seeingwithsound.com”). 2 influential account to date, and the one we seek to draw the sharpest contrast with, is that of enactivism (also known as the sensorimotor or sensorimotor contingency theory). For this radical way of modeling perception, our awareness of the world beyond is inextricably linked to our familiarity with movement and action. In particular, enactivism holds that perception is the recognition of sensorimotor contingencies, the law-like connections between our actions and resultant sensory input. To see a cup of tea, for example, it is necessary to understand how visual sensations caused by the object would change as a result of environmental exploration and movement. In sensory substitution, enactivists have found perhaps their most intuitively powerful source of evidence. First off, they can explain how one modality sitting in for another is even possible. Immediate sensation is unimportant so long as the sensorimotor contingencies that govern use of the device, that is, how movements affect video input, parallel those of vision. Second and still more impressively, enactivism seems to predict the reliance of distal attribution upon control over visual input from the camera. Passively trained subjects were able to pick out the orientation of lines and even identify 2D shapes (granted high error rates; see White, 1970), but they never reached the astounding capacities of their actively trained counterparts. While active subjects moved freely into perception of external objects, passive subjects never described their experiences in anything but proximal, tactile terms— exactly what one would expect were understanding action a key component of perception. We should likewise note that this has been one of the most longstanding and popular morals drawn from experiments with SSDs. The enactivists are in good 3 company. Bach-y-Rita, for example, thought it a “plausible hypothesis” that self- guidance of input constitutes “the necessary and sufficient condition for the experienced phenomena to be attributed to a stable outside world” (1972, p. 99). The position seems to have been shared by his colleagues, as well, since nearly identical language is used in White et al. (1970). The hypothesis was discussed rather positively in Epstein et al.’s (1986) seminal paper on distal attribution, and as recently as ten years ago reviewers (Lenay et al., 2003) went so far as to claim “empirical proof” that “there is no perception without action” (p. 282). Nevertheless, we suspect such judgments are hasty. In the following, we shall make our case. First, we contend that the purported necessity of action is overstated and that the reports upon which enactivists hinge their account have not been adequately scrutinized. In particular, the quality of visual input available to subjects in the early experiments they cite was so low that action could plausibly be transformed from a mere aid to an essential means of disambiguation. Second, there are at least three alternative mechanisms that could explain the substitution process and the empirical observations associated with it: blindness induced neural plasticity, multimodal3 learning, and mental imagery. These three accounts, we shall argue, fit together in a clear and mutually beneficial way, making for a formidable, albeit speculative, answer to the problem of sensory substitution. 3 Throughout the paper, we shall use the terms multimodal and multisensory to mean more or less the same thing. 4 1.1 More Background: Sensory Substitution and Enactivism Although the idea of sensory substitution is an odd one, it is not entirely new. The basic idea has been around at least since Descartes, who in his Dioptrics hypothesized that the blind gain a kind of sight through use of a cane: No doubt you have had the experience of walking over rough ground without a light, and finding it necessary to use a stick in order to guide yourself. You may then have been able to notice that by means of this stick, you could feel the various objects situated around you, and that you could even tell whether there were trees or rocks, or sand, or water, or grass, or mud, or any other such thing. It is true that this kind of sensation is somewhat confused and obscure in those who do not have long practice with it. But consider it in those born blind, who have made use of it all their lives: with them, you will find it is so perfect and so exact, that one might almost say that they see with their hands, or that their stick is the organ of some sixth sense given to them in place of sight. (Descartes, 1637/1985, p. 153) He went on to add that, given two such rods, the blind may even be able to triangulate depth (for a recent take, see Cabe, Wright, & Wright, 2003). Another philosopher to consider the possibility was Rousseau. In Emile, he noted the possibility of auditory sensory substitution: As our sense of feeling, when properly exercised, becomes a supplement to sight, why may it not also substitute for hearing to a certain degree, since sounds excite in resonant bodies vibrations sensible to touch? Lay a hand on the body of the cello, and you will be able, without the assistance of either eyes or ears, to distinguish, merely by the way in which the wood vibrates and trembles, whether the sound it gives is deep or shrill, whether it comes from the treble or the bass. If one were to train the senses to these differences, I do not doubt that, in time, one could become so sensitive as to be able to distinguish a whole air by means of the fingers. Now, if we concede this, it is clear that we might easily talk to deaf people by means of music; for tone and measures are no less susceptible of regular combination than voice and articulation, so they may be made use of in the same way as the elements of speech. (Rousseau, 1762, 237-238, quoted in Geldard, 1966)4 4 Interestingly, both philosophers drew this conclusion from reflection on haptic touch. Some studies we shall discuss later might help explain this. 5 In order to reach something close to the contemporary SSDs, however, one must go to the 20th century.
Recommended publications
  • SENSORY SUBSTITUTION: LIMITS and PERSPECTIVES Charles Lenay, Olivier Gapenne, Sylvain Hanneton, Catherine Marque, Christelle Genouëlle
    SENSORY SUBSTITUTION: LIMITS AND PERSPECTIVES Charles Lenay, Olivier Gapenne, Sylvain Hanneton, Catherine Marque, Christelle Genouëlle To cite this version: Charles Lenay, Olivier Gapenne, Sylvain Hanneton, Catherine Marque, Christelle Genouëlle. SEN- SORY SUBSTITUTION: LIMITS AND PERSPECTIVES. Yvette Hatwell; Arlette Streri; Edouard Gentaz. Touching for Knowing, 53, John Benjamins Publishers, pp.275-292, 2004, Advances in Con- sciousness Research, 9789027251855. 10.1075/aicr.53.22len. hal-02434266 HAL Id: hal-02434266 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-02434266 Submitted on 9 Jan 2020 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. SENSORY SUBSTITUTION: LIMITS AND PERSPECTIVES Charles Lenay, Olivier Gapenne, Sylvain Hanneton1, Catherine Marque et Christelle Genouëlle 29/08/2003 Université de Technologie de Compiègne COSTECH - BIM Groupe Suppléance Perceptive2 A quarter of a century ago, in the preface to « Brain Mechanisms in Sensory Substitution », Paul Bach y Rita wrote: "This monograph thus risks becoming outdated in a very short time since the development of refined sensory substitution systems should allow many of the question raised here to be answered, and some of the conclusions may appear naive to future readers." (BACH Y RITA, 1972) As it turns out, this prediction is far from having been fulfilled: in spite of their scientific and social interest, their real effectiveness and a certain technological development, prosthetic devices employing the principle of "sensory substitution" are not widely used by the blind persons for whom they were originally destined.
    [Show full text]
  • Perceiving Invisible Light Through a Somatosensory Cortical Prosthesis
    ARTICLE Received 24 Aug 2012 | Accepted 15 Jan 2013 | Published 12 Feb 2013 DOI: 10.1038/ncomms2497 Perceiving invisible light through a somatosensory cortical prosthesis Eric E. Thomson1,2, Rafael Carra1,w & Miguel A.L. Nicolelis1,2,3,4,5 Sensory neuroprostheses show great potential for alleviating major sensory deficits. It is not known, however, whether such devices can augment the subject’s normal perceptual range. Here we show that adult rats can learn to perceive otherwise invisible infrared light through a neuroprosthesis that couples the output of a head-mounted infrared sensor to their soma- tosensory cortex (S1) via intracortical microstimulation. Rats readily learn to use this new information source, and generate active exploratory strategies to discriminate among infrared signals in their environment. S1 neurons in these infrared-perceiving rats respond to both whisker deflection and intracortical microstimulation, suggesting that the infrared repre- sentation does not displace the original tactile representation. Hence, sensory cortical prostheses, in addition to restoring normal neurological functions, may serve to expand natural perceptual capabilities in mammals. 1 Department of Neurobiology, Duke University, Box 3209, 311 Research Drive, Bryan Research, Durham, North Carolina 27710, USA. 2 Edmond and Lily Safra International Institute for Neuroscience of Natal (ELS-IINN), Natal 01257050, Brazil. 3 Department of Biomedical Engineering, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina 27710, USA. 4 Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina 27710, USA. 5 Center for Neuroengineering, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina 27710, USA. w Present address: University of Sao Paulo School of Medicine, Sao Paulo 01246-000, Brazil. Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to M.A.L.N.
    [Show full text]
  • Electrocorticography Evidence of Tactile Responses in Visual Cortices
    UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Electrocorticography Evidence of Tactile Responses in Visual Cortices Gaglianese, A.; Branco, M.P.; Groen, I.I.A.; Benson, N.C.; Vansteensel, M.J.; Murray, M.M.; Petridou, N.; Ramsey, N.F. DOI 10.1007/s10548-020-00783-4 Publication date 2020 Document Version Final published version Published in Brain Topography License CC BY Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Gaglianese, A., Branco, M. P., Groen, I. I. A., Benson, N. C., Vansteensel, M. J., Murray, M. M., Petridou, N., & Ramsey, N. F. (2020). Electrocorticography Evidence of Tactile Responses in Visual Cortices. Brain Topography, 33(5), 559–570. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10548-020-00783-4 General rights It is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). Disclaimer/Complaints regulations If you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, stating your reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Ask the Library: https://uba.uva.nl/en/contact, or a letter to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam, The Netherlands. You will be contacted as soon as possible. UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (https://dare.uva.nl) Download date:03 Oct 2021 Brain Topography (2020) 33:559–570 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10548-020-00783-4 ORIGINAL PAPER Electrocorticography Evidence of Tactile Responses in Visual Cortices Anna Gaglianese1,2,3 · Mariana P.
    [Show full text]
  • Sensory Substitution and the Human
    tavvJ TRENDSin CognitiveScierc€s Vot.7No.l2 D6csmbe.2OO3 Sensorysubstitution and the human- machineinterface Paul Baeh-y-Ritaland Stephen W. Kercelz l?e?.anmglE-9fonhopedics and RehabiritationMedicine, and BiomedicarEngineering, univorsity of wisconsin, MadisonWl 53706.USA 2Endogenous SystemsResearch Group, New EnglandInstitute, University ot New England,Biddeford ME 04005,USA Recent advances in the instrumentation technology ol advances have led to the possibility of new prosthetic sensory substitution presemed have new opportunities devices being potentially accessible at much lower cost to to dsv€lop systems for compsnsation of sansory loss, millions ofpatients. ThiB is Btimulating the interest ofboth ,n sensory substitution {€.g. ot sight or vestibular func. research groups and industry leading to the establish- tion), intormation trom an artificial roceptor is coupled ment of new research and development efrorts in many tho to brain via a human-machine interfaco. The brain countries. Thia recent exploeion of interest in sensorv is able to use this intormation in place of that usually substitution suggest€that now is a good time to review transmitted from an intaqt ssnss organ. Both auditory progress in the area. and tactilo systems show promiss for practical sensory substitution intartace sitos. This research provides Brain plasticity experimental tools tor examining brain plasticity and Brain plasticity can be defined as the adaptiee capacities has implications fot porceptual and cognition studios ofthe central nervous system - its ability to modify its own more gonerally. structural organization and fiuctioning, IlT. It permits atr adaptive (or a maladaptive) responseto functional demand, Persons who become blind do not lose tÄe capacity to Mechanisms of brain plasticity include neurochemical, see.
    [Show full text]
  • A Review and Selective Analysis of 3D Display Technologies for Anatomical Education
    University of Central Florida STARS Electronic Theses and Dissertations, 2004-2019 2018 A Review and Selective Analysis of 3D Display Technologies for Anatomical Education Matthew Hackett University of Central Florida Part of the Anatomy Commons Find similar works at: https://stars.library.ucf.edu/etd University of Central Florida Libraries http://library.ucf.edu This Doctoral Dissertation (Open Access) is brought to you for free and open access by STARS. It has been accepted for inclusion in Electronic Theses and Dissertations, 2004-2019 by an authorized administrator of STARS. For more information, please contact [email protected]. STARS Citation Hackett, Matthew, "A Review and Selective Analysis of 3D Display Technologies for Anatomical Education" (2018). Electronic Theses and Dissertations, 2004-2019. 6408. https://stars.library.ucf.edu/etd/6408 A REVIEW AND SELECTIVE ANALYSIS OF 3D DISPLAY TECHNOLOGIES FOR ANATOMICAL EDUCATION by: MATTHEW G. HACKETT BSE University of Central Florida 2007, MSE University of Florida 2009, MS University of Central Florida 2012 A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Modeling and Simulation program in the College of Engineering and Computer Science at the University of Central Florida Orlando, Florida Summer Term 2018 Major Professor: Michael Proctor ©2018 Matthew Hackett ii ABSTRACT The study of anatomy is complex and difficult for students in both graduate and undergraduate education. Researchers have attempted to improve anatomical education with the inclusion of three-dimensional visualization, with the prevailing finding that 3D is beneficial to students. However, there is limited research on the relative efficacy of different 3D modalities, including monoscopic, stereoscopic, and autostereoscopic displays.
    [Show full text]
  • Robustness in Neurological Systems 13 - 15 November, 2015 Center for Philosophy of Science
    Robustness in Neurological Systems 13 - 15 November, 2015 Center for Philosophy of Science ABSTRACTS Alison BArth, Professor, DepArtment of Biology, AnD HeAD, BArth LAb, CArnegie Mellon University Functional Connectivity is Regulated by SOM Interneurons Spontaneous Activity UnDerstAnDing the DynAmic rAnge for synAptic trAnsmission is A criticAl component of builDing A functionAl circuit DiAgrAm for the mammaliAn brAin. We find that excitAtory synaptic strength between neocortical neurons is markeDly suppresseD During network Activity in mouse somatosensory cortex, with smaller EPSP AmplituDes AnD high fAilure rAtes thAn previously reporteD. This phenomenon is regulAteD by tonic ActivAtion of presynAptic GABAb receptors viA the Activity of somatostAtin-expressing interneurons. Optogenetic suppression of somatostAtin neurAl firing wAs sufficient to enhAnce EPSP AmplituDe AnD reDuce fAilure rAtes, effects thAt were fully reversible AnD Also occluDeD by GABAb AntAgonists. These DAtA inDicAte thAt somatostAtin-expressing interneurons can rapidly and reversibly rewire neocortical networks through synAptic silencing, And suggest A critical role for these neurons in gating perception And plAsticity. Emilio Bizzi, DepArtment of BrAin AnD Cognitive Sciences And McGovern Institute for Brain Research, MIT Muscle Synergies, Concept, Principles, and Potential use in Neurorehabilitation When the centrAl nervous system (CNS) generAtes voluntAry movement, many muscles, eAch comprising thousAnDs of motor units, Are simultAneously ActivAteD AnD coorDinAteD. ComputAtionAlly, this is A DAunting tAsk, AnD investigAtors hAve striveD to unDerstAnD whether AnD how the CNS’s burDen is reDuceD to A much smaller set of vAriAbles. In the lAst few yeArs, we and our collaborators have seArcheD for physiologicAl eviDence of simplifying strAtegies by exploring whether the motor system makes use of motor moDules, to construct A lArge set of movement.
    [Show full text]
  • Audioviewer: Learning to Visualize Sound
    AudioViewer: Learning to Visualize Sound by Yuchi Zhang A THESIS SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF Master of Science in THE FACULTY OF GRADUATE AND POSTDOCTORAL STUDIES (Computer Science) The University of British Columbia (Vancouver) December 2020 c Yuchi Zhang, 2020 The following individuals certify that they have read, and recommend to the Faculty of Graduate and Postdoctoral Studies for acceptance, the thesis entitled: AudioViewer: Learning to Visualize Sound submitted by Yuchi Zhang in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science in Computer Science. Examining Committee: Helge Rhodin, Computer Science Supervisor Kwang Moo Yi, Computer Science Examining Committee Member ii Abstract Sensory substitution can help persons with perceptual deficits. In this work, we attempt to visualize audio with video. Our long-term goal is to create sound perception for hearing impaired people, for instance, to facilitate feedback for training deaf speech. Different from existing models that translate between speech and text or text and images, we target an immediate and low-level translation that applies to generic environment sounds and human speech without delay. No canonical mapping is known for this artificial translation task. Our design is to translate from audio to video by compressing both into a common latent space with a shared structure. Our core contribution is the development and evaluation of learned mappings that respect human perception limits and maximize user comfort by enforcing priors and combining strategies from unpaired image translation and disentanglement. We demonstrate qualitatively and quantitatively that our AudioViewer model maintains important audio features in the generated video and that generated videos of faces and numbers are well suited for visualizing high- dimensional audio features since they can easily be parsed by humans to match and distinguish between sounds, words, and speakers.
    [Show full text]
  • Brain Uploading-Related Group Mind Scenarios
    MIRI MACHINE INTELLIGENCE RESEARCH INSTITUTE Coalescing Minds: Brain Uploading-Related Group Mind Scenarios Kaj Sotala University of Helsinki, MIRI Research Associate Harri Valpola Aalto University Abstract We present a hypothetical process of mind coalescence, where artificial connections are created between two or more brains. This might simply allow for an improved formof communication. At the other extreme, it might merge the minds into one in a process that can be thought of as a reverse split-brain operation. We propose that one way mind coalescence might happen is via an exocortex, a prosthetic extension of the biological brain which integrates with the brain as seamlessly as parts of the biological brain in- tegrate with each other. An exocortex may also prove to be the easiest route for mind uploading, as a person’s personality gradually moves away from the aging biological brain and onto the exocortex. Memories might also be copied and shared even without minds being permanently merged. Over time, the borders of personal identity may become loose or even unnecessary. Sotala, Kaj, and Harri Valpola. 2012. “Coalescing Minds: Brain Uploading-Related Group Mind Scenarios.” International Journal of Machine Consciousness 4 (1): 293–312. doi:10.1142/S1793843012400173. This version contains minor changes. Kaj Sotala, Harri Valpola 1. Introduction Mind uploads, or “uploads” for short (also known as brain uploads, whole brain emu- lations, emulations or ems) are hypothetical human minds that have been moved into a digital format and run as software programs on computers. One recent roadmap chart- ing the technological requirements for creating uploads suggests that they may be fea- sible by mid-century (Sandberg and Bostrom 2008).
    [Show full text]
  • The Sound of Vision Project: on the Feasibility of an Audio-Haptic Representation of the Environment, for the Visually Impaired
    brain sciences Review The Sound of Vision Project: On the Feasibility of an Audio-Haptic Representation of the Environment, for the Visually Impaired Ómar I. Jóhannesson 1,*, Oana Balan 2, Runar Unnthorsson 3, Alin Moldoveanu 2 and Árni Kristjánsson 1 1 Laboratory of Visual Perception and Visuo-motor control, Faculty of Psychology, School of Health Sciences, University of Iceland, Reykjavik 101, Iceland; [email protected] 2 Faculty of Automatic Control and Computers, Computer Science and Engineering Department, University Politehnica of Bucharest, Bucharest 060042, Romania; [email protected] (O.B.); [email protected] (A.M.) 3 Faculty of Industrial Engineering, Mechanical Engineering and Computer Science, School of Engineering and Natural Sciences, University of Iceland, Reykjavik 101, Iceland; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +354-868-1407 Academic Editor: Bernadette Murphy Received: 28 April 2016; Accepted: 23 June 2016; Published: 27 June 2016 Abstract: The Sound of Vision project involves developing a sensory substitution device that is aimed at creating and conveying a rich auditory representation of the surrounding environment to the visually impaired. However, the feasibility of such an approach is strongly constrained by neural flexibility, possibilities of sensory substitution and adaptation to changed sensory input. We review evidence for such flexibility from various perspectives. We discuss neuroplasticity of the adult brain with an emphasis on functional changes in the visually impaired compared to sighted people. We discuss effects of adaptation on brain activity, in particular short-term and long-term effects of repeated exposure to particular stimuli. We then discuss evidence for sensory substitution such as Sound of Vision involves, while finally discussing evidence for adaptation to changes in the auditory environment.
    [Show full text]
  • Electronic Approaches to Restoration of Sight
    Home Search Collections Journals About Contact us My IOPscience Electronic approaches to restoration of sight This content has been downloaded from IOPscience. Please scroll down to see the full text. 2016 Rep. Prog. Phys. 79 096701 (http://iopscience.iop.org/0034-4885/79/9/096701) View the table of contents for this issue, or go to the journal homepage for more Download details: IP Address: 171.64.108.170 This content was downloaded on 09/08/2016 at 17:30 Please note that terms and conditions apply. IOP Reports on Progress in Physics Reports on Progress in Physics Rep. Prog. Phys. Rep. Prog. Phys. 79 (2016) 096701 (29pp) doi:10.1088/0034-4885/79/9/096701 79 Review 2016 Electronic approaches to restoration of sight © 2016 IOP Publishing Ltd G A Goetz1,2 and D V Palanker1,3 RPPHAG 1 Hansen Experimental Physics Laboratory, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA 2 Neurosurgery, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA 096701 3 Ophthalmology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA E-mail: [email protected] and [email protected] G A Goetz and D V Palanker Received 11 November 2014, revised 11 April 2016 Accepted for publication 23 May 2016 Electronic approaches to restoration of sight Published 9 August 2016 Abstract Printed in the UK Retinal prostheses are a promising means for restoring sight to patients blinded by the gradual atrophy of photoreceptors due to retinal degeneration. They are designed to reintroduce ROP information into the visual system by electrically stimulating surviving neurons in the retina. This review outlines the concepts and technologies behind two major approaches to retinal prosthetics: epiretinal and subretinal.
    [Show full text]
  • Neural Representation of Force, Grasp, and Volitional State in Intracortical Brain-Computer Interface Uers with Tetraplegia
    NEURAL REPRESENTATION OF FORCE, GRASP, AND VOLITIONAL STATE IN INTRACORTICAL BRAIN-COMPUTER INTERFACE UERS WITH TETRAPLEGIA BY ANISHA RASTOGI Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements For the degree of doctor of philosophy Dissertation Advisor: Dr. A. Bolu Ajiboye Department of Biomedical Engineering CASE WESTERN RESERVE UNIVERSITY January, 2021 CASE WESTERN RESERVE UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF GRADUATE STUDIES We hereby approve the thesis/dissertation of Anisha Rastogi candidate for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy* Committee Chair Dr. Dominique Durand Committee Member Dr. A. Bolu Ajiboye Committee Member Dr. Dawn M. Taylor Committee Member Dr. Jonathan P. Miller Date of Defense 22 June 2020 *We also certify that written approval has been obtained for any proprietary material contained therein Dedication To Bill Kochevar, for dedicating his last days to bettering the world for people with disabilities. This research is a way to honor your faith in a brighter future. i Table of Contents Dedication ........................................................................................................................ i Table of Contents ............................................................................................................ ii List of Figures ................................................................................................................. vi List of Tables ................................................................................................................ viii List of Abbreviations ......................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Symmetric Networks with Geometric Constraints As Models of Visual Illusions
    S S symmetry Article Symmetric Networks with Geometric Constraints as Models of Visual Illusions Ian Stewart 1,*,† and Martin Golubitsky 2,† 1 Mathematics Institute, University of Warwick, Coventry CV4 7AL, UK 2 Department of Mathematics, Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210, USA; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected] † These authors contributed equally to this work. Received: 17 May 2019; Accepted: 13 June 2019; Published: 16 June 2019 Abstract: Multistable illusions occur when the visual system interprets the same image in two different ways. We model illusions using dynamic systems based on Wilson networks, which detect combinations of levels of attributes of the image. In most examples presented here, the network has symmetry, which is vital to the analysis of the dynamics. We assume that the visual system has previously learned that certain combinations are geometrically consistent or inconsistent, and model this knowledge by adding suitable excitatory and inhibitory connections between attribute levels. We first discuss 4-node networks for the Necker cube and the rabbit/duck illusion. The main results analyze a more elaborate model for the Necker cube, a 16-node Wilson network whose nodes represent alternative orientations of specific segments of the image. Symmetric Hopf bifurcation is used to show that a small list of natural local geometric consistency conditions leads to alternation between two global percepts: cubes in two different orientations. The model also predicts brief transitional states in which the percept involves impossible rectangles analogous to the Penrose triangle. A tristable illusion generalizing the Necker cube is modelled in a similar manner.
    [Show full text]