Modeling the Dative Alternation with Automatically Extracted Features∗

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Modeling the Dative Alternation with Automatically Extracted Features∗ Modeling the dative alternation with automatically extracted features∗ Huayan Zhong and Amanda Stent Mary Swift Computer Science Department Computer Science Department Stony Brook University University of Rochester Stony Brook, NY 11794-4400 Rochester, NY 14627 huayan, [email protected] [email protected] Abstract tures. We compare the effect of different types of feature on this classification task, and show that the most predic- We show that generation of contextually appropriate syntactic tive features for text differ from those for dialog. Finally, variation can be improved using a model based on automati- cally extracted features. We adapt a model for predicting da- we show that modeling this type of syntactic variation im- tive alternation from (Bresnan et al. 2005); this model incor- proves the performance of statistical surface realization for porates lexical, syntactic, semantic and pragmatic features. both text and dialog. We evaluate the effect of using different types of feature on The rest of this paper is structured as follows: first, we this classification task and show that the most predictive fea- discuss previous approaches to predicting dative alternation, tures for text differ from those for dialog. Finally, we show and give a brief description of research on surface realiza- that modeling this type of syntactic variation improves the tion. Second, we describe our data. Third, we describe our performance of surface realization for both text and dialog. model for predicting dative alternation. Fourth, we describe our surface realization experiments. Finally, we summarize Introduction our results and describe future work. The ability to produce contextually appropriate syntactic variation contributes to the coherence and naturalness of nat- Related Work ural language generation (NLG). Factors that contribute to Predicting Dative Alternation such variation can be wide-ranging and complex and require models beyond corpus frequencies (e.g. (Creswell & Kaiser One analysis of the dative alternation assumes that the two 2004)). In this paper we examine modeling one type of vari- structures have different semantic representations (e.g, (Pe- ation that affects the placement of arguments within the VP, setsky 1995; Harley 2000)). A contrasting approach (e.g. the dative alternation. Dative alternation involves the varia- (Baker 1997)) argues that the two structures have the same tion between two different syntactic representations for the semantic representation, and surface variation is motivated arguments of a ditransitive verb, i) the dative NP: by the discourse theoretic status of the theme and recipi- Agent Verb Recipient Theme ent arguments. Previous research has explored the influence I gave the dog a bone of factors such as animacy, definiteness and length of the and ii) the dative PP: arguments (syntactic weight) on the surface realization of Agent Verb Theme [to Recipient] the dative construction (e.g. (Collins 1995; Halliday 1970; I gave a bone to the dog Thompson 1990)). (Bresnan et al. 2005) use three differ- In recent work, (Bresnan et al. 2005) describe a combina- ent models predicting from multiple variables to explore the tion of lexical, syntactic, semantic and pragmatic features problem of what really drives the dative alternation. Model that model the dative alternation with high accuracy. For A is a logistic regression model using 14 features (features their experiments, they use hand-annotated features. In this are described in Section ) and shows that certain proper- paper, we show that it is possible to achieve comparable ac- ties of recipient and theme (discourse accessibility, animacy, curacy using automatically extracted training data and fea- definiteness, pronominality and syntactic weight) are mutu- ∗ ally irreducible. Model B is a multilevel logistic regression We would like to thank the anonymous reviewers for their variant of Model A which shows that the features remain sig- helpful comments. This work was supported by the Defense Ad- nificant when conditioned on different verb senses. Model C vanced Research Projects Agency (DARPA), through the Depart- is a variant of Model B adapted for available features in the ment of the Interior, NBC, Acquisition Services Division, un- der Contract No. NBCHD030010, via subcontract to SRI #03- data and shows that the features are predictive when tested 000223). on different corpora (Switchboard and Wall Street Journal). Copyright c 2006, American Association for Artificial Intelli- Bresnan et al. report the performance of the models: Model gence (www.aaai.org). All rights reserved. A, 92%; Model B, 94% and Model C, 93%. In this paper we explore the utility of such models for statistical NLG. We Genre Corpus Dative examples adapt Model A using automatically extracted data and fea- V-NP-NP V-NP-PP tures, evaluate its performance on data from both text and Dialog ROCH 99 274 dialog corpora, and examine which features are most predic- SWBD 401 147 tive. We then evaluate the impact of our model on surface Text PT 818 408 realization of the dative alternation. GW 4067 4117 Surface Realization Table 1: Data used in our experiments Surface realizers generate text from an input representa- tion that may be semantic or syntactic in nature. Tra- ditionally, surface realizers were built using hand-written of corpora of spoken dialogs collected at the University of grammars or templates (e.g. (Elhadad & Robin 1997; Rochester; and the treebanked portion of the Switchboard Baptist & Seneff 2000; Channarukul 1999)). However, most (SWBD) corpus of spoken dialogs (Marcus, Santorini, & recent work on surface realization has used a two-stage ap- Marcinkiewicz 1995). The Text data set comprises the fol- proach (e.g. (Langkilde 2002; Rambow & Bangalore 2000; lowing two corpora: the APW-Dec-99 portion of the English Ratnaparkhi 2000)). The first stage uses a simple hand- Gigaword (GW) corpus of raw text (Graff et al. 2005); and written grammar that overgenerates (i.e. produces syntac- the Penn Treebank (PT) corpus of parsed text, including the tically correct and incorrect outputs). The second stage uses Brown and Wall Street Journal subsets (Marcus, Santorini, a statistical language model to rank sentences output by the & Marcinkiewicz 1995). first stage. These surface realizers run quickly and pro- We used automatic methods to prepare each corpus for duce high-quality output. However, the interaction between our experiments as follows. First, we split each tree- grammar and language model, and the lack of modeling of banked sentence into independent clauses. We parsed non- semantics and context in the language model, can make it treebanked sentences into independent clauses using the hard for these surface realizers to produce valid syntactic Collins parser (Collins 1999). Then we automatically an- variants in a principled way. In previous work, we built a notated each clause with semantic information: statistical surface realizer in which word order and word fre- • Verb frames from VerbNet (Kipper, Dang, & Palmer quency probabilities are folded into the grammar rather than 2000). into a second-stage language model (Zhong & Stent 2005). • With this surface realizer, we can more effectively model Noun/verb hypernyms from WordNet (Fellbaum 1998). syntactically valid variation. As a backup, we used Comlex (Girshman, Macleod, & (Creswell & Kaiser 2004) argue that statistical NLG sys- Meyers 1994). tems based only on probabilities cannot capture fine distinc- We extracted from the treebanked corpora verbs that tions in meaning; discourse context and meaning must be might be examples of the dative: verbs appearing in taken into account when selecting a construction for NLG sentences of the form V-NP-PP:to with the prepositional purposes. They demonstrate their point by outlining an al- phrase’s role labeled as -DTV. We augmented this list with gorithm to determine surface form of dative constructions the verbs that are in classes with dative frames in VerbNet. based on manually annotated discourse status (hearer-old Then, from all four of our corpora we automatically ex- and hearer-new) and heaviness of arguments, which would tracted clauses containing any of these verbs in either form reduce the error rate by 25% compared to surface form as of the dative alternation. determined by corpus frequency. They argue that labeling Table 1 shows the number of clauses in each of our data data with pragmatic information such as discourse status is sets having either the V-NP-NP or the V-NP-PP construction crucial for statistical NLG. Bresnan et al. (Bresnan et al. and the number of examples of each form of the dative alter- 2005) provide evidence for this by showing that some prag- nation we were able to extract automatically. We included matic features are useful for predicting the dative. We adapt all examples of either form of the dative, even if in our data Bresnan’s model, but use only automatically extracted fea- a particular verb appeared with only one form. tures. Unlike (Creswell & Kaiser 2004), we incorporate this Because these data are automatically extracted, they in- model into a statistical NLG system and achieve improved clude sentences that are not examples of the dative. In par- performance on dative constructions. ticular, in our dialog corpora there are verbs, like ’take’, that (Stede 1998) proposed a general approach to handling take the dative alternation and also have a transitive form verb alternation in NLG for English and German. This ap- with a destination; some of these sentences show up in our proach was focused on representing possible alternations data if the semantic type of the destination is mislabeled or rather than on having a surface realizer determine which one ambiguous. Also, some incomplete sentences are included to use in a given set of circumstances. in our dialog data set because of sentence splitting errors. Data Modeling The Dative Alternation We used two data sets in these experiments; each data set (Bresnan et al. 2005) demonstrated that pragmatic features has two parts, one treebanked and one not.
Recommended publications
  • Diachrony of Ergative Future
    • THE EVOLUTION OF THE TENSE-ASPECT SYSTEM IN HINDI/URDU: THE STATUS OF THE ERGATIVE ALGNMENT Annie Montaut INALCO, Paris Proceedings of the LFG06 Conference Universität Konstanz Miriam Butt and Tracy Holloway King (Editors) 2006 CSLI Publications http://csli-publications.stanford.edu/ Abstract The paper deals with the diachrony of the past and perfect system in Indo-Aryan with special reference to Hindi/Urdu. Starting from the acknowledgement of ergativity as a typologically atypical feature among the family of Indo-European languages and as specific to the Western group of Indo-Aryan dialects, I first show that such an evolution has been central to the Romance languages too and that non ergative Indo-Aryan languages have not ignored the structure but at a certain point went further along the same historical logic as have Roman languages. I will then propose an analysis of the structure as a predication of localization similar to other stative predications (mainly with “dative” subjects) in Indo-Aryan, supporting this claim by an attempt of etymologic inquiry into the markers for “ergative” case in Indo- Aryan. Introduction When George Grierson, in the full rise of language classification at the turn of the last century, 1 classified the languages of India, he defined for Indo-Aryan an inner circle supposedly closer to the original Aryan stock, characterized by the lack of conjugation in the past. This inner circle included Hindi/Urdu and Eastern Panjabi, which indeed exhibit no personal endings in the definite past, but only gender-number agreement, therefore pertaining more to the adjectival/nominal class for their morphology (calâ, go-MSG “went”, kiyâ, do- MSG “did”, bola, speak-MSG “spoke”).
    [Show full text]
  • Dative Constructions in Romance and Beyond
    Dative constructions in Romance and beyond Edited by Anna Pineda Jaume Mateu language Open Generative Syntax 7 science press Open Generative Syntax Editors: Elena Anagnostopoulou, Mark Baker, Roberta D’Alessandro, David Pesetsky, Susi Wurmbrand In this series: 1. Bailey, Laura R. & Michelle Sheehan (eds.). Order and structure in syntax I: Word order and syntactic structure. 2. Sheehan, Michelle & Laura R. Bailey (eds.). Order and structure in syntax II: Subjecthood and argument structure. 3. Bacskai­Atkari, Julia. Deletion phenomena in comparative constructions: English comparatives in a cross­linguistic perspective. 4. Franco, Ludovico, Mihaela Marchis Moreno & Matthew Reeve (eds.). Agreement, case and locality in the nominal and verbal domains. 5. Bross, Fabian. The clausal syntax of German Sign Language: A cartographic approach. 6. Smith, Peter W., Johannes Mursell & Katharina Hartmann (eds.). Agree to Agree: Agreement in the Minimalist Programme. 7. Pineda, Anna & Jaume Mateu (eds.). Dative constructions in Romance and beyond. ISSN: 2568­7336 Dative constructions in Romance and beyond Edited by Anna Pineda Jaume Mateu language science press Pineda, Anna & Jaume Mateu (eds.). 2020. Dative constructions in Romance and beyond (Open Generative Syntax 7). Berlin: Language Science Press. This title can be downloaded at: http://langsci-press.org/catalog/book/258 © 2020, the authors Published under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 Licence (CC BY 4.0): http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ ISBN: 978-3-96110-249-5 (Digital) 978-3-96110-250-1
    [Show full text]
  • From Latin to Romance: Case Loss and Preservation in Pronominal Systems
    FLORE Repository istituzionale dell'Università degli Studi di Firenze From Latin to Romance: case loss and preservation in pronominal systems Questa è la Versione finale referata (Post print/Accepted manuscript) della seguente pubblicazione: Original Citation: From Latin to Romance: case loss and preservation in pronominal systems / Manzini, MARIA RITA; Savoia, LEONARDO MARIA. - In: PROBUS. - ISSN 1613-4079. - STAMPA. - 26, 2(2014), pp. 217-248. Availability: This version is available at: 2158/891750 since: 2016-01-20T16:23:29Z Terms of use: Open Access La pubblicazione è resa disponibile sotto le norme e i termini della licenza di deposito, secondo quanto stabilito dalla Policy per l'accesso aperto dell'Università degli Studi di Firenze (https://www.sba.unifi.it/upload/policy-oa-2016-1.pdf) Publisher copyright claim: (Article begins on next page) 27 September 2021 Probus 2014; 26(2): 217 – 248 M. Rita Manzini* and Leonardo M. Savoia From Latin to Romance: case loss and preservation in pronominal systems Abstract: The evolution from Latin into Romance is marked by the loss of case in nominal declensions. In most Romance varieties, however, pronouns, specifi- cally in the 1st/2nd person singular, keep case differentiations. In some varieties 1st/2nd singular pronouns present a three-way case split, essentially the same re- constructed for proto-Romance (De Dardel and Gaeng 1992, Zamboni 1998). We document and analyze the current situation of Romance in the first part of the article (section 1). In the second part of the article we argue that the Dative Shifted distribution of loro in modern Italian, accounted for by means of the category of weak pronoun in Cardinaletti and Starke (1999), is best construed as a survival of oblique case in the 3rd person system (section 2).
    [Show full text]
  • TWO FORMS of "BE" in MALAYALAM1 Tara Mohanan and K.P
    TWO FORMS OF "BE" IN MALAYALAM1 Tara Mohanan and K.P. Mohanan National University of Singapore 1. INTRODUCTION Malayalam has two verbs, uNTE and aaNE, recognized in the literature as copulas (Asher 1968, Variar 1979, Asher and Kumari 1997, among others). There is among speakers of Malayalam a clear, intuitively perceived meaning difference between the verbs. One strong intuition is that uNTE and aaNE correspond to the English verbs "have" and "be"; another is that they should be viewed as the "existential" and "equative" copulas respectively.2 However, in a large number of contexts, these verbs appear to be interchangeable. This has thwarted the efforts of a clear characterization of the meanings of the two verbs. In this paper, we will explore a variety of syntactic and semantic environments that shed light on the differences between the constructions with aaNE and uNTE. On the basis of the asymmetries we lay out, we will re-affirm the intuition that aaNE and uNTE are indeed equative and existential copulas respectively, with aaNE signaling the meaning of “x is an element/subset of y" and uNTE signaling the meaning of existence of an abstract or concrete entity in the fields of location or possession. We will show that when aaNE is interchangable with uNTE in existential clauses, its function is that of a cleft marker, with the existential meaning expressed independently by the case markers on the nouns. Central to our exploration of the two copulas is the discovery of four types of existential clauses in Malayalam, namely: Neutral: with the existential verb uNTE.
    [Show full text]
  • MR Harley Miyagawa Syntax of Ditransitives
    Syntax of Ditransitives Heidi Harley and Shigeru Miyagawa (in press, Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics) July 2016 Summary Keywords 1. Structure for the Two Internal Arguments 2. Underlying Order 3. Meaning Differences 4. Case, Clitic 5. The Structure of Ditransitives 6. Nominalization Asymmetries 6.1. A Morphological Account of the Nominalization Asymmetry 6.2. –kata Nominalization in Japanese and Myer’s Generalization 6.3 Selectional Accounts of the Nominalization Asymmetry 6.4 Applicative vs Small Clause Approaches to the DOC. 7. Constraints on the Dative/DOC Alternation 7.1 Morphological Constraints 7.2 Lexical Semantic Constraints 7.3 Information-Structural and Sentential Prosody Constraints 8. Overview and Prospects Further Reading References Summary Ditransitive predicates select for two internal arguments, and hence minimally entail the participation of three entities in the event described by the verb. Canonical ditranstive verbs include give, show and teach; in each case, the verb requires an Agent (a giver, shower or teacher, respectively), a Theme (the thing given, shown or taught) and a Goal (the recipient, viewer, or student). The property of requiring two internal arguments makes ditransitive verbs syntactically unique. Selection in generative grammar is often modelled as syntactic sisterhood, so ditranstive verbs immediately raise the question of whether a verb might have two sisters, requiring a ternary-branching structure, or whether one of the two internal arguments is not in a sisterhood relation with the verb. Another important property of English ditransitive constructions is the two syntactic structures associated with them. In the so-called “Double Object Construction”, or DOC, the Goal and Theme both are simple NPs and appear following the verb in the order V-Goal-Theme.
    [Show full text]
  • The Special Datives
    The Special Datives To this point, the functions of the Dative Case have been 1. Indirect Object of give, tell, show verbs 2. Dative with Special Adjectives friendly to, unfriendly to, similar to, dissimilar to, equal to, suitable for, near to, dear to, pleasing to, etc. 3. Dative with Special Intransitive Verbs parco, mando, impero, noceo, resisto, studeo, etc. 4. Dative with Certain Compound Verbs praesum, praeficio, occurro, etc. (often verbs with prefixes of ob- and prae-) Two new Dative Case functions, sometimes called Special Datives, are explained in Unit XIII. These are the Dative of Purpose and the Dative of Reference. 1. Dative of Purpose. Sometimes, the idea of purpose can be stated in a single noun. In such a case, the Dative Case form of the noun is used. It answers the question, “For what purpose does something exist?” Note how we often ask, “What is that for?” The consul donated money for a reward. Consul pecuniam praemio donavit. Seven common Latin nouns are often used as (for?!) the Dative of Purpose: Principal Parts Dative Singular Form Meaning cura, -ae, f. curae for a concern auxilium, -I, n. auxilio for a help impedimentum, -I, n. impedimento for an obstacle praemium, -I, n. praemio for a reward praesidium, -I, n. praesidio for a guard subsidium, -I, n. subsidio for a support usus,, -us, m. usui for a use NOTA BENE: These are not the only nouns which may be used for purpose; they are only the most common. Sometimes a plural Dative Case form is used for purpose. Catullus poeta scripsit puellas esse curis.
    [Show full text]
  • The Old English To-Dative Construction1 Ghent
    1 The Old English to-dative construction1 LUDOVIC DE CUYPERE Ghent University 1 Acknowledgements: I wish to acknowledge helpful comments and suggestions by Cynthia Allen and Harold Koch. I am grateful to Daan Van den Nest for assisting me with the retrieval of the corpus data I also thank Joan Bresnan for providing me with an R script to perform the cross- validation test. Finally, I am grateful to the editor and the referees for several comments and suggestions that helped to improve the paper. 2 Abstract In Present Day English (PDE), the to-dative construction refers to clauses like John told/offered/mentioned/gave the books to Mary, in which a ditransitive verb takes a Recipient that is expressed as a to-Prepositional Phrase (to-PP). This study examines the to-dative construction in Old English (OE). I show, first of all, that this construction was not rare in OE, in contrast to what has been suggested in the literature. Second, I report on two corpus studies in which I examined the ordering behaviour of the NP and the to-PP. The results of the first study suggest that the same ordering tendencies already existed in OE as in PDE: both the NP-to-PP and the to- PP-NP orders were grammatical, but the NP-to-PP was the most frequently used one. However, in OE, the to-PP-NP was more common than in PDE, where its use is heavily restricted. My second corpus study is informed by the multifactorial approach to the English dative alternation and uses a mixed-effects logistic regression analysis to evaluate the effects of various linguistic (verbal semantics, pronominality, animacy, definiteness, number, person and length) and extra- linguistic variables (translation status, time of completion/manuscript) on the ordering of NP and to-PP.
    [Show full text]
  • First Objects and Datives: Two of a Kind?
    BLS 32 February 2006 First Objects and Datives: Two of a Kind? Beth Levin Stanford University ([email protected]) 1 The big question There are two options for expressing recipients in English with dative verbs—verbs taking agent, recipient, and theme arguments, such as give and other verbs of causation of possession. (1) a. Terry gave Sam an apple. (double object construction) b. Terry gave an apple to Sam. (to construction) Many languages which lack a double object construction still have a core (i.e., nonadjunct) grammatical relation, distinct from subject and object, used to express recipient. Specifically, many languages have a dative case and use the dative (case marked) NP as the basic realization of possessors, including recipients of verbs of causation of possession. (2) Ja dal Ivanu knigu. I.NOM give.PST Ivan.DAT book.ACC ‘I gave Ivan a book.’ (RUSSIAN; dative construction) A PERENNIAL QUESTION: What is the status of the first object in the double object construction? — Is it comparable to the object of a transitive verb? AN ANSWER: YES is implicit in the label “first/primary/inner object” and is supported by its passivizability and postverbal position; cf. Dryer’s (1986) “primary object languages”. (3) Sam was given an apple. — Is it comparable to the dative NP of languages with a dative case? AN ANSWER: In the context of the answer to the previous question, NO is the usual answer given; it is the NP in the to phrase in the to construction that is equated with the dative NP. THE COMPLICATION: Repeated observations that despite surface similarities with direct objects, recipients in the double object construction do not show all direct object properties (e.g., Baker 1997; Hudson 1992; Maling 2001; Marantz 1993; Polinsky 1996; Ziv & Sheintuch 1979).
    [Show full text]
  • Title Serial Verb Construction in Cantonese-Speaking
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by HKU Scholars Hub Serial verb construction in Cantonese-speaking preschool Title children with and without language impairment Author(s) Chan, Ka-yee; 陳嘉儀 Chan, K. [陳嘉儀]. (2014). Serial verb construction in Cantonese- speaking preschool children with and without language Citation impairment. (Thesis). University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam, Hong Kong SAR. Issued Date 2014 URL http://hdl.handle.net/10722/238921 The author retains all proprietary rights, (such as patent rights) and the right to use in future works.; This work is licensed under Rights a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License. SERIAL VERB CONSTRUCTION IN CANTONESE PRESCHOOLER 1 Serial Verb Construction in Cantonese-Speaking Preschool Children With and Without Language Impairment CHAN Ka Yee A dissertation submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the Bachelor of Science (Speech and Hearing Sciences), The University of Hong Kong, June 30, 2014. SERIAL VERB CONSTRUCTION IN CANTONESE PRESCHOOLER 2 List of Abbreviations CL Noun Classifier LP Linking Particle PERT Perfective Aspect Marker V-PRT Verbal Particle SERIAL VERB CONSTRUCTION IN CANTONESE PRESCHOOLER 3 Serial Verb Construction in Cantonese-Speaking Preschool Children With and Without Language Impairment CHAN Ka Yee Abstract Serial verb construction (SVC) is very productive in Cantonese and it develops actively in the preschool years. Little is known about the development of SVC of children with language impairment (LI). Forty-four kindergarten children with and without LI, aged between 4 to 6 years, participated in the study. This study made use of a video description task to examine the developmental and error patterns of five subtypes of SVC, which were directional, instrumental, benefactive, purpose and dative SVC.
    [Show full text]
  • Plural Addressee Marker and Grammaticalization in Barayin
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by SOAS Research Online Published as: Lovestrand, Joseph. 2018. Plural addressee marker and Pre-publication version grammaticalization in Barayin. Journal of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 10(1) https://doi.org/10.1163/18776930-01001004 Accepted version downloaded from SOAS Research Online: http:// eprints.soas.ac.uk/32651 Plural addressee marker and grammaticalization in Barayin Joseph Lovestrand, University of Oxford Abstract This article describes two distinct but related grammaticalization paths in Barayin, an East Chadic lan- guage. One path is from a first-person plural pronoun to a first-person dual pronoun. Synchronically, the pronominal forms in Barayin with first-person dual number must now be combined with a plural addressee enclitic, nà, to create a first-person plural pronoun. This path is identical to what has been documented in Philippine-type languages. The other path is from a first-person dative suffix to a suffix dedicated to first-person hortative. This path of grammaticalization has not been discussed in the literature. It occurred in several related languages, and each in case results in a hortative form with a dual subject. Hortative forms with a plural subject are created by adding a plural addressee marker to the dual form. The plural addressee marker in Chadic languages is derived from a second-person pronominal. Keywords Chadic, Barayin, hortative, dual, pronouns, diachronic1 1 Introduction This paper describes two distinct but related grammaticalization paths in Barayin [bva] and other lan- guages of the Guera subbranch of East Chadic languages (East Chadic B).
    [Show full text]
  • CASE ALTERNATIONS in ANCIENT GREEK PASSIVES and the TYPOLOGY of CASE Elena Anagnostopoulou Christina Sevdali
    CASE ALTERNATIONS IN ANCIENT GREEK PASSIVES AND THE TYPOLOGY OF CASE Elena Anagnostopoulou Christina Sevdali University of Crete Ulster University This article presents and discusses evidence that genitive and dative objects regularly become nominative in Ancient Greek passives of monotransitives and ditransitives. This is a typologically and theoretically significant state of affairs for two reasons . (i) As is well known, nonaccusative objects are, in many languages, not allowed to enter into Case alternations, a fact that has been ac - counted for in the government-binding /principles-and-parameters literature on the basis of the as - sumption that nonaccusative objects —prototypically datives —bear inherent , lexical , or quirky Case. By this reasoning, Ancient Greek genitives and datives must be concluded to have structural Case. (ii) Even in languages where dative -nominative (DAT-NOM) alternations do obtain, they are often limited to ditransitives, a fact that can been taken to suggest that dative qual - ifies as structural Case only in ditransitives. A language like Ancient Greek , which allows genitive and dative objects to become nominative in all passives (monotransitives and ditransitives) , shows that it is, in principle, possible to have a linguistic system where genitive and dative qualify as structural Cases in both monotransitives and ditransitives. Case theories must be designed in such a way as to allow for this option. We argue for an analysis of Case alternations that combines the view that alternating datives and
    [Show full text]
  • The Biabsolutive Construction in Lak and Tsez
    Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Lingua 150 (2014) 137--170 www.elsevier.com/locate/lingua The biabsolutive construction in Lak and Tsez Annie Gagliardi a, Michael Goncalves a, Maria Polinsky a,*, Nina Radkevich b a Harvard University, USA b University of York, United Kingdom Received 12 November 2013; received in revised form 4 July 2014; accepted 8 July 2014 Available online Abstract In ergative constructions, the agent of a transitive verb is in the ergative case and the theme is in the absolutive case. By contrast, in biabsolutive constructions, both the agent and theme of a transitive verb appear in the absolutive case. This paper presents and analyzes the biabsolutive construction in two Nakh-Dagestanian languages, Lak and Tsez. Despite many surface similarities, the biabsolutive constructions in Lak and Tsez call for different syntactic analyses. We argue that the biabsolutive construction in Lak is an instance of restructuring in the presence of an aspectual head bearing a progressive (imperfective) feature. Tsez biabsolutive constructions, on the other hand, are biclausal; we argue that the theme and the lexical verb are contained in a PP complement selected by a light verb. Related languages may be classified as ‘‘Lak-type’’ or ‘‘Tsez-type’’ based on the behavior of their biabsolutives. The existence of two underlying structures for one surface pattern in Nakh-Dagestanian poses a learnability problem for a child acquiring a language with biabsolutive constructions. We outline a set of strategies used by a learner who must compare the available input data with a set of structural hypotheses. © 2014 Elsevier B.V.
    [Show full text]