Cognitive Aging Is Associated with Redistribution of Synaptic Weights in the Hippocampus
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Cognitive aging is associated with redistribution of synaptic weights in the hippocampus Eric W. Bussa,1, Nicola J. Corbetta,1, Joshua G. Robertsa, Natividad Ybarraa, Timothy F. Musiala, Dina Simkinb, Elizabeth Molina-Camposa, Kwang-Jin Oha, Lauren L. Nielsena, Gelique D. Ayalaa, Sheila A. Mullena, Anise K. Farooqia, Gary X. D’Souzaa, Corinne L. Hilla, Linda A. Beana, Annalise E. Rogalskya, Matthew L. Russoa, Dani M. Curlikb, Marci D. Antionb, Craig Weissb, Dane M. Chetkovichc, M. Matthew Ohb, John F. Disterhoftb,2, and Daniel A. Nicholsona,2 aDepartment of Neurological Sciences, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, IL 60612; bDepartment of Physiology, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, IL 60611; and cDepartment of Neurology, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville, TN 37232 Edited by Carol A. Barnes, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, and approved January 19, 2021 (received for review December 6, 2019) Behaviors that rely on the hippocampus are particularly susceptible 11), cognitive aging within hippocampus-dependent forms of learn- to chronological aging, with many aged animals (including humans) ing and memory are relatively independent of those that engage the maintaining cognition at a young adult-like level, but many others prefrontal/orbitofrontal cortical neural systems (6–9, 12–15). the same age showing marked impairments. It is unclear whether Neither the mechanisms underlying the conservation of memory the ability to maintain cognition over time is attributable to brain function across chronological aging nor those contributing to the maintenance, sufficient cognitive reserve, compensatory changes in age-related emergence and exacerbation of memory impairments network function, or some combination thereof. While network dys- are clearly understood for either neurocognitive system. It is clear, function within the hippocampal circuit of aged, learning-impaired however, that neither frank neuronal loss (16, 17) nor overall animals is well-documented, its neurobiological substrates remain synapse loss (18) contributes to cognitive aging within the medial elusive. Here we show that the synaptic architecture of hippocampal temporal lobe/hippocampal memory system. Rather, the intrigu- regions CA1 and CA3 is maintained in a young adult-like state in ing idea that has emerged from work in both the hippocampal and aged rats that performed comparably to their young adult counter- the prefrontal/orbitofrontal cortical memory systems is that there parts in both trace eyeblink conditioning and Morris water maze are functional alterations in the synaptic connections in individual NEUROSCIENCE learning. In contrast, among learning-impaired, but equally aged microcircuits embedded within these larger neuroanatomical rats, we found that a redistribution of synaptic weights amplifies systems (6–10, 19–31). the influence of autoassociational connections among CA3 pyramidal Axospinous synapses (including those in hippocampal and neurons, yet reduces the synaptic input onto these same neurons cortical circuits) are characterized on the basis of the three- from the dentate gyrus. Notably, synapses within hippocampal re- dimensional morphology of their postsynaptic densities (PSDs) gion CA1 showed no group differences regardless of cognitive abil- (20, 32–34). The most-abundant axospinous synaptic subtype has a ity. Taking the data together, we find the imbalanced synaptic weights within hippocampal CA3 provide a substrate that can explain Significance the abnormal firing characteristics of both CA3 and CA1 pyramidal neurons in aged, learning-impaired rats. Furthermore, our work pro- A longstanding question in the field of the neurobiology of vides some clarity with regard to how some animals cognitively age learning and memory is why some aged individuals perform successfully, while others’ lifespans outlast their “mindspans.” comparably to young adults in learning and memory tasks, whereas others show impairments that resemble the perfor- synapse | aging | hippocampus | cognition mance of those with hippocampal damage. Answers to this question have been critical for advancing our understanding ging is the biggest risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease, but of, and our ability to treat, aging-related cognitive impair- Amany aged individuals nevertheless retain the ability to ments. Here, we have exploited the exquisite organization of perform cognitive tasks with young adult (YA)-like competency, the hippocampal circuit to reveal that “unsuccessful” cognitive and are thus resilient to age-related cognitive decline and de- aging is associated with a breakdown of young adult-like cir- mentias (1, 2). The mechanisms of such resilience are unknown, cuit organization, and provide a remarkably straightforward but are thought to involve neural or cognitive reserve, brain net- potential cellular substrate for the well-documented dysfunc- work adaptations, or simply the ability to maintain cognitive brain tion in neuronal encoding of hippocampal CA1 and CA3 pyra- circuits in a YA-like state (3–5). Much of the cellular and func- midal neurons in aged learning-impaired rats. tional insight into the concept or risk of/resilience against age- related cognitive impairments has come from animal models of Author contributions: J.F.D. and D.A.N. designed research; E.W.B., N.J.C., J.G.R., N.Y., normal/nonpathological aging (6–10). Many of these studies have T.F.M., D.S., E.M.-C., K.-J.O., L.L.N., G.D.A., S.A.M., A.K.F., G.X.D., C.L.H., L.A.B., A.E.R., M.L.R., D.M. Curlik, M.D.A., C.W., M.M.O., and D.A.N. performed research; D.M. Chetko- shown that circuit function abnormalities are associated with be- vich and D.A.N. contributed new reagents/analytic tools; E.W.B., N.J.C., J.G.R., N.Y., T.F.M., havioral impairments. The cellular and structural bases for such D.S., E.M.-C., K.-J.O., L.L.N., G.D.A., S.A.M., A.K.F., G.X.D., C.L.H., L.A.B., A.E.R., M.L.R., D.M. functional aberrations, however, remain largely unknown. Curlik, M.D.A., C.W., D.M. Chetkovich, M.M.O., J.F.D., and D.A.N. analyzed data; and Two of the most well-studied cognitive domains that show E.W.B., N.J.C., M.L.R., M.M.O., J.F.D., and D.A.N. wrote the paper. susceptibility to chronological aging in both rodents and nonhu- The authors declare no competing interest. man primates are working memory and spatial/temporal memory This article is a PNAS Direct Submission. (6–10). Importantly, these cognitive domains engage anatomically Published under the PNAS license. distinct neurocognitive systems, with the former relying on pre- 1E.W.B. and N.J.C. contributed equally to this work. frontal/orbitofrontal cortical circuits and the latter relying on 2To whom correspondence may be addressed. Email: [email protected] or hippocampal circuitry. Interestingly, although behavioral deficits [email protected]. in these two domains (in the case of rat models of cognitive aging) This article contains supporting information online at https://www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/ begin to emerge, worsen, and become increasingly prevalent be- doi:10.1073/pnas.1921481118/-/DCSupplemental. tween 12 and 18 mo of age in most strains (reviewed in refs. 9 and Published February 15, 2021. PNAS 2021 Vol. 118 No. 8 e1921481118 https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1921481118 | 1of10 Downloaded by guest on September 29, 2021 continuous, macular, disk-shaped PSD, as compared to the less- only AMPARs, only perforated axospinous synapses, and only abundant perforated synaptic subtype, which has at least one dis- hippocampal region CA3, which together shift the balance of continuity in its PSD (34). In addition to differing substantially with synaptic weights that drive action potential output in CA3 py- regard to relative frequency, perforated and nonperforated synap- ramidal neurons maladaptively toward an overemphasis of the ses also harbor major differences in size and synaptic AMPA-type autoassociational synapses that interconnect CA3 pyramidal and NMDA-type receptor expression levels (AMPAR and neurons. NMDAR, respectively) (34–38). There is also evidence that per- forated and nonperforated synapses are differentially involved in Results synaptic plasticity (39–44) and in preservation of—or reduc- Spontaneous Excitatory Postsynaptic Potential Amplitude Is Elevated tions in—memory function during chronological aging (6, 20, in Aged CA3, but Not CA1, Pyramidal Neurons. We recorded spon- 45). Layered onto these general distinctions between perfo- taneous excitatory postsynaptic potentials (sEPSPs) from hippo- rated and nonperforated synapses are more specific differences campal pyramidal neurons of YA (6- to 8-mo old) and aged (28- in their characteristics when considered within neural circuits. to 31-mo old) rats from regions CA1 and CA3, which many studies For example, perforated synapses have a stronger and more have implicated in age-related learning and memory impairments consistent influence on neuronal computation within hippo- (6–10) (Fig. 1A). These rats, unlike all others studied here, were campal region CA1 than their nonperforated counterparts, which not behaviorally characterized. Although sEPSP amplitude was nevertheless outnumber the former by a roughly 9-to-1 ratio (34, similar in pyramidal neurons from YA and aged rats in region 46, 47). CA1 (Fig. 1A), CA3 pyramidal neurons from aged rats had sig- These and other circuit-specific differences necessitate a nificantly higher sEPSP amplitudes as compared to those from circuit-based approach to understanding the synaptic