CHARACTERIZATION OF LOCOMOTOR RESPONSE TO PSYCHOSTIMULANTS IN THE PARTHENOGENETIC MARBLED CRAYFISH (Procambarus fallax forma virginalis): A PROMISING MODEL FOR STUDYING THE OF

Cedric James Jackson

A Thesis

Submitted to the Graduate College of Bowling Green State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of

MASTER OF SCIENCE

August 2016

Committee:

Moira van Staaden, Advisor

Robert Huber

Vipaporn Phuntumart

© 2016

Cedric James Jackson

All Rights Reserved iii CHAPTER I. ABSTRACT

Moira van Staaden, Advisor

Epigenetic regulation has been implicated as an important factor in understanding the

molecular mechanisms of drug addiction. This is due to the life-long behavioral changes that commonly afflict addicted individuals long after drug exposure has been extinguished.

Invertebrates, such as crayfish, offer excellent model systems to study these molecular mechanisms because they retain the ancestral neural reward circuit that is evolutionarily conserved across taxa, possess relatively few, large , and have an accessible, modularly

organized nervous system. The marbled crayfish (Procambarus fallax forma virginalis), in

particular, has potential as a model for epigenetic studies because it is parthenogenetic, and thus

all individuals are genetic clones. To provide the foundation of this model system for parsing the

epigenetic mechanisms of drug addiction, here I characterize the locomotor response of juvenile

P. f. f. virginalis exposed to the psychostimulant, d- sulfate. Custom video-tracking

software was used to record the movement patterns of juveniles exposed to water infused with

varying concentrations of d-amphetamine sulfate. ANOVA demonstrated that crayfish

locomotion was significantly impacted by drug concentration. These psychostimulant effects,

along with the non-invasive mode of drug delivery, which avoids potential epigenetic changes

resulting from the of direct injection into the hemolymph, set the stage for using P. f. f.

virginalis as an model for epigenetic studies.

iv CHAPTER II. ABSTRACT

Moira van Staaden, Advisor

With the rising interest in using invertebrate models for behavioral epigenetic studies,

and the tissue-specific nature of epigenetic changes, there is a need for effective methods of

extracting genomic DNA from neural tissue. Invertebrates, such as crayfish, represent useful

models in which to parse out the basic underlying epigenetic changes because of the relatively

simplified anatomical structure of their nervous system. The parthenogenetic marbled crayfish

(Procambarus fallax forma virginalis) offers a particularly elegant system because it reproduces

asexually and produces large numbers of genetically identical offspring. However, the low DNA yield of fatty neural tissue combined with small sample size represent technical challenges in using invertebrate brains. The present study seeks to support the use of the marbled crayfish as a

model for studying the mechanisms of behavioral epigenetics by providing an optimized method

for genomic DNA extraction from invertebrate neural tissue. v

Dedicated to Dr. T. Edward Weiss Jr. (1951-2013), a caring and supportive professor who

always believed in and fostered the scientist within me. I owe a great deal of my success to the help and support Dr. Weiss gave as my Advisor during the academically formative years of my undergraduate education at Christopher Newport University, without which I would not be

the scientist I am today.

vi ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

I would like to express my appreciation for all of those who contributed to the success of my master’s project: to anyone who has taken the time to offer me advice, constructive criticism, technical help, manuscript review, or moral support.

I am deeply grateful to my advisor Moira van Staaden and my committee members

Robert Huber and Vipa Phuntumart for all of their support, guidance, and constructive feedback at every stage of my thesis. I would like to thank Moira for allowing me the freedom to pursue a project that incorporated many of my areas of interest and for her excellent and numerous revisions of my manuscript. I am grateful for Robert’s technological contribution of the custom video tracking software he developed and the lab space he provided me to establish a breeding colony of my research . And I would like to thank Vipa for the opportunity to work in her molecular lab and for including me in her lab meetings while my advisors were away on sabbatical.

I also want to recognize the support and friendship I have been given from the members of the van Staaden Lab: Kami Stamey and Jessica LaHurd; the Huber Lab: Sayali Gore; and the

Phuntumart Lab: Shannon Miller, Dilshan Beligala, Gayathri Beligala, Satyaki Ghosh, Alex

Howard. All of whom have offered me much help on my thesis, through both technical and moral support. Thank you all.

I have received help and friendship from many of the graduate students and faculty of the

Biology Department at BGSU, and, although too numerous to list individually, your efforts have been deeply appreciated.

I would like to offer my particular gratitude to Noah Ryan, who spent countless hours of his free time, after working eight to ten hour days as a computer scientist at NASA, to help me clean up my data, write my data analysis and statistical analysis programs, and countless hours vii de-bugging said programs. In addition to offering technical support, he has offered continual moral support, and friendship over the years and throughout the entire process of my thesis.

I would like to thank my family and friends for their unconditional support and their loving encouragement throughout the years.

Finally, my sincerest thanks to the marbled crayfish, without whom this research would not have been possible.

viii

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Page CHAPTER 1: LOCOMOTOR RESPONSE OF THE MARBLED CRAYFISH

(Procambarus fallax forma virginalis) TO D-AMPHETAMINE SULFATE

ADMINISTRATION ...... 1

INTRODUCTION ...... 1

METHODS ...... 5

Animal Care and Maintenance ...... 5

Behavioral Testing Apparatus ...... 6

Experimental Design and Procedure ...... 7

Behavioral Analysis ...... 9

Statistical Analysis ...... 10

Euthanasia, Dissection and Brain Extraction ...... 10

RESULTS ...... 11

Locomotion ...... 11

Space Use and Walking Pattern Behavioral Response ...... 14

DISCUSSION ...... 15

Crayfish Locomotion in Response to Novel Arena ...... 16

Effects of Initial Drug Exposure ...... 16

Effects of Continuous Chronic Exposure ...... 20

Conclusion ...... 22

REFERENCES ...... 23 ix

CHAPTER II: OPTIMIZATION OF GENOMIC DNA EXTRACTION FROM MARBLED

CRAYFISH (Procambarus fallax forma virginalis) BRAIN TISSUE ...... 33

INTRODUCTION ...... 33

METHODS ...... 35

Euthanasia, Dissection and Brian Extraction ...... 35

Preparation of Crayfish Brian and Abdominal Tissue Samples ...... 35

Genomic DNA Extraction...... 36

Methylation-sensitive Enzyme Digestion ...... 37

RESULTS ...... 38

Genomic DNA Extracted from Different Starting Amounts of Abdominal

Tissue ...... 38

Genomic DNA Extracted from Assorted Number of Pooled Whole Brains . 40

Methylation-Sensitive Enzyme Digests ...... 43

DISCUSSION ...... 44

Genomic DNA Extraction...... 45

Methylation-Sensitive Enzyme Digests ...... 46

REFERENCES ...... 47 x

LIST OF TABLES

Table ...... Page

2.1 Total genomic DNA extracted from various amounts of P. f. f. virginalis

abdominal tissue ...... 39

2.2 Total genomic DNA extracted from assorted number of pooled P. f. f.

virginalis brains during the early stage of DNA extraction optimization ...... 41

2.3 Total genomic DNA extracted from pooled P. f. f. virginalis brains after

optimization of the DNA extraction method ...... 43 xi

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure ...... Page

1.1 Juvenile crayfish housing unit ...... 6

1.2 Example of behavioral testing arena ...... 7

1.3 Schematic outlining the experimental design ...... 8

1.4 Mean distance travelled in the first 30 minutes of each recording period for

d-amphetamine sulfate concentrations from 0.0 to 50.0 µM ...... 13

1.5 Psychostimulant effects of d-amphetamine sulfate concentrations from 0.0 to

50.0 µM ...... 14

1.6 Example of crayfish movement patterns over a one-hour period in drug-free water 15

2.1 Genomic DNA extracted from varying amounts of P. f. f. virginalis abdominal

tissue ...... 39

2.3 Genomic DNA extracted from assorted number of pooled P. f. f. virginalis brains

during the early stage of DNA extraction optimization ...... 42

2.4 Methylation sensitive enzyme (Msp I and Hpa II) digests of low and high

concentrations of P. f. f. virginalis gDNA ...... 44 1

CHAPTER I: LOCOMOTOR RESPONSE OF THE MARBLED CRAYFISH

(Procambarus fallax forma virginalis) TO D-AMPHETAMINE SULFATE

ADMINISTRATION

INTRODUCTION

Drug addiction is a syndrome characterized by maladaptive plasticity of the neural reward circuit (reviewed in Hyman et al., 2006). Under normal circumstances, the neural reward circuit is triggered by natural rewards (food, sex, social interaction, etc.) to release signals from the ventral tegmental area to projections in the and the prefrontal cortex and from the substantia nigra to projections in the dorsal striatum (reviewed in;

Hyman et al., 2006; Spanagel and Weiss, 1999; Wise, 1996). Therefore, it is unsurprising that the monoamine dopamine is required for motivated and appetitive and for reward-

related learning (reviewed in Alcaro et al., 2007). However, drugs of abuse have the ability to

falsely trigger and overstimulate, or “hijack,” the natural reward circuit by increasing the magnitude and the duration of these dopamine signals (reviewed in Kelley and Berridge, 2002; but see Hagen et al., 2013). In this way drugs of abuse act as strong reinforcers and can, with repeated exposure in susceptible individuals, develop into the syndrome of addiction. The

addictive cycle is developed as drugs reinforce and strengthen drug-taking and

consumption, until the associated drug-seeking behaviors become compulsive despite negative repercussions (reviewed in Wise, 1998). Such behavioral abnormalities can persist even after long periods of abstinence, sometimes indefinitely, and are accompanied with a high susceptibility of (O’Brien et al., 1998; Wise and Bozarth, 1987; Wise, 2000). These 2

persisting lifelong symptoms present particularly recalcitrant clinical challenges in developing

effective treatments for addiction.

The long-term behavioral abnormalities that characterize the addiction syndrome are

attributed to changes in neural expression. Epigenetics is the interaction of biochemical mechanisms that result in a tissue-specific change in gene expression, while the underlying DNA

sequence remains unaltered (Jaenisch and Bird, 2003). These biochemical changes can be induced by environmental conditions experienced during an individual’s lifetime. In this way, epigenetics can be conceptualized as a means through which the environment interacts with an organism’s genome. As such, a subset of stable epigenetic changes have been identified as important molecular mechanisms underlying the neural plasticity associated with the addiction syndrome (reviewed in Nestler, 2014). Drugs of abuse affect the molecular environment within the brain’s reward regions, which can induce three major modes of epigenetic regulation: modifications (acetylation and methylation), DNA methylation, and non-coding RNA (reviewed in Nestler, 2014). DNA methylation is one of the most studied epigenetic mechanisms because it is considered important in the mediation of stable, and possibly heritable, changes in transcription (reviewed in Bestor, 2000).

Molecular and neural mechanisms underlying drug-induced reward have primarily been studied with the use of mammalian models (Chao and Nestler, 2004). Research in mammalian systems has successfully identified molecular targets, neurochemical systems and brain regions that mediate drug-induced reward (reviewed in Berridge and Robinson, 1998; Ikemoto and

Panksepp, 1999; Kalivas and McFarland, 2003; Kalivas and Volkow, 2005). Although, drugs of abuse target homologous brain regions in and other mammalian models (reviewed in

Butler and Hodos, 2005; and Koob and Volkow, 2010), a similarity in the molecular neural 3

mechanisms for drug effects has been evolutionarily conserved across taxa (McClung and Hirsh,

1998; Andretic et al., 1999; Kaun et al., 2011).

Invertebrates possess a relatively simple, modularly organized nervous system. Unlike in mammalian models, the role of individual neurons and specific circuits can be determined in the invertebrate brain (Kaun et al., 2012). By taking advantage of these methods, invertebrate models have already greatly benefited our understanding of the basis of learning and memory,

and of the mechanisms involved in molecular and neurochemical signals (Carew and Sahley,

1986). Exploration into how drugs alter neural function and plasticity across many different

invertebrate taxa can help reveal the basic functional properties involved in the .

Among invertebrate models of drug addiction, decapod Crustacea, such as crayfish, offer

a particularly useful set of advantages. Crayfish possess relatively few, often large, easily

identifiable, and modularly organized neurons (reviewed in Clarac and Pearlstein, 2007).

Combined with a behavioral repertoire and relatively large size, this renders the crayfish particularly accessible to pharmo-behavioral manipulations (Panksepp and Huber, 2002;

Huber and Delago, 1998). Crayfish have been a longstanding animal model and, consequently, a great deal is known about their central nervous system structure and function: both crayfish and lobster brains have been mapped (Beltz, 1999; Polanska et al., 2007; Sullivan and Beltz, 2001; Tierney et al., 2003), the of crayfish behavior is well- understood (reviewed in Kravitz, 2000; Edwards et al., 2003), and the lobster transcriptome has recently been characterized (McGrath et al., 2016). This foundation of neuroethological research sets the stage for delving into the basic mechanisms of drug-induced neural plasticity and reward-related learning.

4

Psychostimulant behavioral effects have been demonstrated in the rusty crayfish

(Orconectus rusticus) in response to amphetamine (Alcaro et al., 2011), (Nathaniel et al.,

2012), and morphine (Nathaniel et al., 2010). This includes sensitization (Dziopa et al., 2011;

Nathaniel et al., 2010; Nathaniel et al., 2012), withdrawal (Huber et al., 2011; Nathaniel et al.,

2009), and reinstatement with a single priming dose (Nathaniel et al., 2009). Drug-induced reward in O. rusticus has been demonstrated through the use of conditioned place preference

(Panksepp and Huber, 2004; Nathaniel et al., 2009) and self-administration paradigms (Datta,

2015). Moreover, studies of drug-induced gene expression changes in the crayfish brain have shown changes of c-Fos mRNA expression in the accessory lobe (Nathaniel et al., 2012), which is hypothesized to be involved in high-order processing of environmental stimuli and an important region in the neural reward circuitry in crayfish (Nathaniel et al., 2012; Sullivan and

Beltz, 2005).

In the current study I examine the locomotor response of P. f. f. virginalis to the psychostimulant d-amphetamine sulfate. Using a non-invasive mode of drug administration over

24-hour exposure periods, I explored differential behavioral effects and characterized the locomotor responses of juvenile P. f. f. virginalis (1) during initial exposure, de-coupled from a novelty-induced behavioral response, and (2) after continuous chronic exposure. The results of this work provide the foundation for P. f. f. virginalis as a potential model system for studying

the epigenetic mechanisms underlying the maladaptive plasticity associated with drug addiction.

5

METHODS

Animal Care and Maintenance

Adult P. f. f. virginalis obtained from a commercial supplier were housed individually in

the lab in clear plastic containers (Snapware Total Solution, 232.4 mm L x 186.7 mm W x 76.2 mm H). These were modified so as to allow water flow-through (4 mm holes drilled into container: 34 on lid, 7 along each length, and 5 along each width) and were kept within a single circulating-water tank system, at ambient room temperature (~19-22ºC) and a 12:12 hour light/dark cycle. were fed rabbit pellets supplemented with frozen bloodworms and brine shrimp twice a week. Each crayfish had access to a shelter and were handled as little as possible to minimize stress. First generation offspring (n = 48) of these females were used in all subsequent experiments. Once they had detached from their mother’s tail, offspring were moved to individual compartments within multi-partitioned containers, which simultaneously ensured social isolation and macro-environmental standardization (Figure 1.1). Isolated offspring were exclusively fed a diet of two parts frozen brine shrimp and one part frozen emerald entrée (San

Francisco Bay Brand) until used in behavioral assays at 7-10 months of age (14-29 mm in length). 6

Figure 1.1: Juvenile crayfish housing unit (348 mm L x 218 mm W x 35 mm H). Holes

(2.5 mm) were drilled in the top bottom of each compartment (52 mm L x 42 mm W x 31

mm H) to allow for water circulation throughout the unit. Each unit housed 30

individuals in separately-labeled compartments, was labeled with the mother’s

identification number, and contained only juveniles from the same clutch.

Behavioral Testing Apparatus

A frosted plastic container (Figure 1.2) was made by bisecting a Whitmor stacking organizer box (492 mm L x 330 mm W x 91 mm H) to create six compartments, each measuring

91 mm L x 103 mm W x 61 mm H. A 64 mm diameter hole was drilled into the bottom center of each compartment and covered with a white plastic canvas mesh (Darice), which was adhered with VDR labeling tape. These compartmentalized containers were placed within white Sterilite

ID boxes (371 mm L x 279 mm W x 130 mm H; 9.7 Liters) and wedged into place with cut pieces of PVC pipe (60 mm D x 47 mm L) within 2.8 L of water. This apparatus allowed for individual crayfish, placed within each compartment, to be transferred between containers of 7 drug- free water and drug-infused water with minimal disturbance, and ensured simultaneous and identical exposure time for all animals.

Figure 1.2: Example of behavioral testing arena

Experimental Design and Procedure

Juvenile crayfish were randomly assigned to one of four treatment groups: 0.0 μM, 0.5

μM, 5.0 μM, or 50.0 μM d-amphetamine sulfate (FW: 368.5; Sigma, St. Louis: A 5880). Two trials of six crayfish were run for each treatment group (n = 12 per group). In order to minimize confounding variables, all water used during experimental procedures was taken from the circulating tank in which crayfish were housed on the day of testing. For each trial, six crayfish were placed in individual compartments within the compartmentalized testing arena. This compartmentalized arena was then placed in a container of drug-free water for an acclimation 8

period of one hour. Following acclimation, the compartmentalized container was lifted and transferred to a second container containing either drug-free water (d-amphetamine sulfate 0.0

μM) for control trials, or one of three concentrations of d-amphetamine sulfate (0.5 μM, 5.0 μM, or 50.0 μM) for test trials, for an exposure period of 24 hours (experimental design outlined in

Figure 1.3).

Figure 1.3: Schematic outlining the experimental design

During the one-hour acclimation period, the first two hours (early exposure) and last two hours (late exposure) of the 24-hour test period, crayfish movements were recorded at a rate of 4

frames s-1 with a Microsoft Lifecam Studio 1080p HD camera (1920 x 1080 pixel resolution)

mounted above the center of the testing arena. Custom video-tracking software (available at

) recorded into a text file for subsequent behavioral analysis, the time

stamps and x, y Cartesian coordinates of each individual’s position in real time (visualization of coordinate tracking shown in Figure 1.6). The experimental apparatus had diffuse light entering from above and was cordoned off with a black curtain to minimize light and other visual 9

disturbances to both the crayfish and the video tracker. After the exposure period, crayfish were

immediately removed from the experimental arena and anesthetized on ice before further

processing for molecular analysis. Between experimental trials, arenas and containers were

rinsed with alcohol, water, and then air-dried to remove all traces of drug and chemical cues.

Behavioral Analysis

Juvenile P. f. f. virginalis are relatively small and largely translucent, with the darkest

area generally being where consumed food in the stomach can be seen through the carapace.

Consequently, tracking software would occasionally have difficulty discerning the crayfish from

background shadows in the arena, which resulted in occasional dropped data frames. Because juvenile crayfish have a relatively slow walking speed, these dropped data frames had minimal impact on the final calculation of total distance travelled. Video tracking data was accumulated at an average rate of 254 frames per minute; minutes with no data totaled only 0.6%. The x, y

Cartesian coordinates recorded by the tracking software were converted from pixel units into real distances (mm). Euclidian distance between coordinates was calculated to obtain the total distance travelled for each individual. Coordinate data were plotted and visually checked for errors: large jumps in position were filtered from data before further analysis was performed. To remove flickering in recorded position of crayfish, a threshold of 1 mm was considered no movement and a moving average filter of 5 frames was applied to movement calculations.

Individuals that molted during testing, or for which more than 10% data was lost in any one recording session, were omitted from the data set entirely for statistical analysis (n = 5). The final

analysis sample size after data clean-up for each treatment group was as follows: 0.0 µM (n=11),

0.5 µM (n=10), 5.0 µM (n=11), and 50.0 µM (n=11). Behavioral analysis was conducted on the 10

first 30 minutes of data from each recording session; due to technical error, data for some

recording sessions was not recorded beyond this time frame.

Statistical Analysis

Custom scripts written in Python Version 3.4.2 (Python Software Foundation;

) were used to generate data sets used for descriptive and inferential statistics. Supporting packages/ functions installed were numpy, matplotlib, and pyplot.

Descriptive statistics tabulated total distance travelled over the first 30 minutes of each recording

session. Statistical analyses were conducted with R (version 3.2.3, )

with supporting installed packages R.utils, multcomp, AOV, boot, and car. In some datasets, treatment groups showed homoscedasticity, or equality of variance, (Levene’s Test) in the

acclimation period, but there was large individual variation in response to drug exposure and data in the early and late exposure periods were not homoscedastic. Therefore, an ANOVA was

run through bootstrap resampling (n=10,000) with a confidence interval of 95%; pairwise

contrasts were performed to discern differences between control and treatment groups. In datasets that showed homoscedasticity, significance was tested using a one-way ANOVA (R package AOV), with a Bonferroni correction applied, and the post-hoc Dunnett’s test was used to determine the differences between treatment groups to the control group with pairwise comparisons.

Euthanasia, Dissection and Brain Extraction

Experimental crayfish were euthanized and dissected immediately following the 24-hour exposure period. After being deeply anaesthetized in an ice-slurry for at least 20 minutes, the crayfish was decapitated, the antennae were removed, and the head was mounted and pinned onto a Sylgard dish anterior side down and covered in 125 mM saline solution. Under a 11

microscope, what remained of the stomach, antennal glands, and other tissue was removed to expose the brain. The lateral brain connections and the eyestalks were dissected and the brain

was removed from the head and placed on a wet slide where any non-neural tissue was removed.

The crayfish body and the brain were placed in separate, correspondingly labeled containers and stored at -80ºC for subsequent genetic analysis.

RESULTS

Locomotion

Crayfish of all treatment groups travelled the furthest distance during the acclimation

period when they were first introduced to, and were exploring, the novel environment of the test

arena: 0.0 µM (4584.7 mm: SE ± 266.5), 0.5 µM (4895.2 mm; SE ± 276.3), 5.0 µM (3566.4 mm;

SE ± 308.7), and 50.0 µM (4111.3 mm; SE ± 362.1) (Figure 1.4). There was no significant

difference in the distance travelled between treatment groups (p = 0.133). Over time, crayfish of

all treatment groups slowly reduced their locomotion, covering smaller distances in each

successive recording period.

During early exposure, when treatment groups were first exposed to varying

concentrations of d-amphetamine sulfate, there was a significant effect due to treatment (p =

0.006). The 50 µM treatment group travelled a significantly greater distance, averaging 2828.4

mm (SE ± 306.5; Pairwise contrast p < 0.001), compared to the control group which travelled an

average of 1514.4 mm (SE ± 205.6). There was no significant difference between the control

group and the 0.5 µM treatment group which travelled an average of 2171.6 mm (SE ± 343.7;

Pairwise contrast p = 0.201), nor with the 5.0 µM treatment group which travelled an average of

1083.3 mm (SE ± 343.7; Pairwise contrast p = 0.508). 12

During late exposure, after animals had remained in exposure tank overnight, there was a significant difference observed in distance travelled due to treatment (p = 0.028), with the control animals travelling further (1637.6 mm; SE ± 367.8) than any of the treatment groups: 0.5 µM

(776.8 mm; SE ± 194.2), 5.0 µM (567.4 mm; SE ± 148.6), or 50.0 µM (813.9 mm; SE ± 200.3)

(Figure 1.4).

A great deal of variability was observed amongst individual responses to first drug exposure. In order to better control for this large individual variation, the difference travelled between the early exposure and the acclimation period was calculated for each individual, then averaged together within each treatment group (Figure 1.5). This revealed a relationship resembling a dose-response curve in which the 0.0 µM control group exhibited the smallest reduction in distance travelled (2981.3 mm; SE ± 301.9), followed by 0.5 µM (2636.9 mm; SE ±

402.1), 5.0 µM (2369.3 mm; SE ± 302.3), and 50.0 µM (1216.6 mm; 320.6). A significant treatment effect was detected (F(3,39) = 4.924, p = 0.005). The Dunnett’s test revealed a

significant difference (p = 0.002) between the 50.0 µM and control groups, but no significant

difference between the control and either the 0.5 µM group (p = 0.831) or the 5.0 µM group (p =

0.458). 13

Figure 1.4: Mean distance travelled in the first 30 minutes of each recording period for d- amphetamine sulfate concentrations from 0.0 to 50.0 µM. As data does not show a normal distribution and does not pass the Levene’s test for homoscedasticity, an ANOVA was conducted with bootstrap resampling (n=10,000). No significant difference between treatment groups was observed in the acclimation period (p = 0.133). However, a significant difference was observed between treatment groups after crayfish were exposed to amphetamine (early exposure p = 0.006; late exposure p = 0.028). Bootstrapping pairwise contrasts revealed that, when compared to the 0.0 µM control group, the 50.0 µM treatment group was significantly different in the early exposure (p < 0.001) and all treatment groups showed a significant difference in the late exposure (0.5 µM, p < 0.001; 5.0 µM, p < 0.001;

50.0 µM, p < 0.001). 14

Figure 1.5: Psychostimulant effects of d-amphetamine sulfate concentrations from 0.0 to

50.0 µM: difference between mean distance travelled by individuals in first 30 minutes of

early exposure and acclimation period. There was a significant difference due to treatment (p

= 0.005), with Dunnett’s test differentiating the 50 µM treatment group from the control (p =

0.002).

Space Use and Walking Pattern Behavioral Responses

As crayfish generally show an edge-preference (Figure 1.6), each arena compartment was divided into space divisions of “edge” (inner 15 mm around compartment perimeter) and

“middle” (the remaining inner portion of arena). Percent time spent and distance travelled in each area was quantified for all treatment groups for each recording session. Crayfish of all treatment groups displayed a clear preference for the edge and there was no significant difference between treatment groups (data not shown). 15

Figure 1.6: Example of crayfish movement patterns over a one-hour period in drug-free

water. Tracking coordinates overlaid on picture of experimental arena.

Crayfish move with an intermittent walking pattern in which they take frequent pauses to sample with their antenna before continuing. In order to characterize whether amphetamine exposure affected this behavioral pattern, pause duration, number of pauses, and duration of walking between pauses were quantified, both for the entire arena and for each arena space (edge

and middle) separately. No significant differences were found between treatment groups for any

of these parameters (data not shown).

DISCUSSION

An ideal experimental system for studying the epigenetics of addiction would combine an animal model with little or no genetic variation, with a non-invasive mode of drug delivery. The 16

results of this study demonstrate that the parthenogenetic marbled crayfish, P. f. f. virginalis, is just such a system, displaying a significant, dose-dependent, psychostimulant-induced locomotor response when immersed in water containing d-amphetamine sulfate.

Crayfish Locomotion in Response to Novel Arena

The uniformly high levels of locomotion observed on initial introduction of crayfish into the drug-free environment are typical of animals exposed to novel stimuli. Under natural conditions, such behaviors facilitate exploration, the acquisition of information, and the procurement of other resources essential for survival (Di Chiara and Bassareo, 2007; Marinelli,

2005). Similarly, the reduction of locomotion as crayfish became accustomed to the test arena is concordant with prior observations in O. rusticus, where animals with initial high levels of exploration, gradually decrease movement over time as they habituate, until they eventually settle into one location along the edge of the arena (Panksepp and Huber, 2004; Alcaro et. al.,

2011).

Effects of Initial Drug Exposure

On initial exposure to d-amphetamine, the 50.0 µM treatment group of P. f. f. virginalis showed a significant increase in locomotion compared to that of control animals during the first

30 minutes of exposure. This result is congruent with the typical psychostimulant-induced effects noted in both a range of mammals (Fog, 1969; Schiorring, 1971; Segal and Mandell, 1974;

Hoebel et al., 1983), as well as in other invertebrate systems (reviewed in Wolf and Heberlein,

2003). In O. rusticus, d-amphetamine exposure stimulates enhanced locomotion, along with

other exploratory behaviors, including rearing and antennal sampling (Alcaro et al., 2011).

The behavioral locomotor variability between individuals appears to be reduced in P. f. f.

virginalis compared to that observed in O. rusticus. The degree of variation in locomotion 17

observed in the acclimation period in P. f. f. virginalis was about half that observed in O.

rusticus (Datta, 2015) and one sixth of what was observed in Bhimani and Huber (2015). The

greater locomotor variability observed in O. rusticus may be attributed to the fact that these were

wild-caught individuals of a sexually-reproducing species, with a (presumably) higher degree of

genetic and developmental variation compared to P. f. f. virginalis, which were lab-reared,

asexually reproducing genetic clones. It is notable that individual behavioral and morphological

variation was observed in P. f. f. virginalis regardless of genetic identity, likely as a consequence of uncontrolled microenvironmental influences on development.

Evidence suggests that exploratory behaviors are specifically mediated by the dopaminergic system in both mammals and invertebrates (Hills et al., 2004; reviewed in Alcaro,

et al., 2007). This dopaminergic system is thought to be a key component in the control of the

complex appetitive-motivational (Alcaro et al., 2007), as its primary function is to

sustain exploratory behaviors and reinforce adaptive behaviors that increase the acquisition of

rewards (food, shelter, sex, etc.). Therefore, activation of these neural systems by drugs

of abuse is perceived as rewarding (Di Chiara and Bassareo, 2007), and may result in an increase

of exploratory behaviors in crayfish.

The appetitive-motivational neural circuit also appears to be stimulated by, in addition to more direct rewards, novel stimuli. In O. rusticus, the simultaneous presentation of a novel

environment with the onset of drug administration facilitated an increased psychostimulant behavioral response to d-amphetamine (Alcaro et al., 2011). In the current study, P. f. f.

virginalis were acclimatized for one hour in the test arena prior to drug exposure; an

experimental design which effectively decoupled the d-amphetamine-induced locomotor

response from the novel -induced locomotor response. This demonstrated that d- 18

amphetamine independently induces a psychostimulant response in P. f. f. virginalis. However,

this may also be one reason why a significant dose-response curve was not demonstrated. Had the test subjects been exposed to the novel arena and d-amphetamine simultaneously, individuals exposed to lower concentrations (0.5 and 5.0 µM) may have demonstrated a greater locomotor response, which may have exaggerated the data trend observed in the data that was controlled for individual-variation (Figure 1.5). In order to determine the effect that novel stimuli have on P. f. f. virginalis response to psychostimulants, future research could characterize the locomotor response elicited by the presentation of different concentrations of d-amphetamine in a novel environment.

The observed psychomotor effects establish immersion as an effective method of drug

delivery in juvenile crayfish, although knowledge of drug absorption and rates is

limited and requires more study. Possible pathways of drug intake include absorption through the

respiratory system via the gills and through the gastrointestinal tract. The method of drug

delivery will have a direct influence on crayfish locomotor response. According to the rate

hypothesis of action, the quicker a drug reaches the brain, the greater is both

the perceived reward and the resulting reinforcing effect (Nelson et al., 2006; de Wit et al., 1992;

Gorelick, 1998). Higher drug infusion rates generate greater subjective responses (Abreu et al.,

2001; Nelson et al., 2006), and when given a choice, rats have been shown to prefer higher

infusion rates of the same drug dose (Schindler et al., 2009). Furthermore, a rapid delivery time

frame results in an intensified motivation for obtaining the drug (Minogianis et al., 2013).

Consequently, the route of administration affects how reinforcing the drug is perceived to be, due

to varying pharmacokinetic effects in when the drug reaches the brain (Samaha and Robinson,

2005; Volkow et al., 2000). In crayfish, Datta (2015) demonstrated with O. rusticus that the 19

reinforcing properties of drug-reward were much more robust when psychostimulants were injected directly into the brain, compared to injection into the general hemolymph circulation. By extension then, simple immersion of the animals into drug-infused water is almost certain to have slowed the time course of drug delivery to the active sites in the nervous system, attenuated the behavioral responses to the drug, and diminished any resultant reinforcing effect. However, because route of administration can influence drug effect, it is important to have methods of drug delivery in animal models that are comparable to the methods humans use to self-administer drugs (Sovik, 2013). The route of administration the crayfish are experiencing, by absorbing drug through the respiratory system and the digestive tract simultaneously, may be likened to the human methods of inhalation and insufflation. Therefore, this mode of drug delivery in crayfish may offer a nice complement for studying the differential effects route of drug administration have on the molecular processes and behavioral responses of human drugs of abuse, although research is needed to determine the validity of this comparison.

Drug exposure via immersion may also directly affect a crayfish’s behavioral response, in ways other than locomotion. Crayfish exploration relies heavily upon mechanoreception from antennal sampling (Koch et al. 2006; Basil and Sandeman, 2000), with the antennae and

antennules conveying perceived sensory information to the olfactory lobe in the central nervous

system (Mellon, 2000; Sullivan and Beltz, 2005). The olfactory lobe is mediated by dopamine

and serotonin (Sandeman and Sandeman, 1987; Sandeman et al., 1995; Schmidt, 1997), and

therefore, because amphetamine produces psychostimulant effects by hijacking dopamine-

mediating cellular mechanisms, the olfactory lobe is likely a site of action for psychostimulants.

As such, increased dopamine signals in the olfactory lobe may induce crayfish to engage in

increased antennal sampling behavior. P. f. f. virginalis treated by direct immersion in d- 20

amphetamine were simultaneously being administered drug while also actively sampling a novel chemical in their environment. This may have induced crayfish to spend a greater proportion of their time in antennal sampling exploratory behaviors compared to O. rusticus, which were administered drug through direct injection to the brain and spent approximately equal time engaged in the exploratory behaviors of locomotion and antennal sampling (Alcaro et al., 2011;

Panksepp and Huber, 2004). In preliminary experiments, it was observed that crayfish did increase antennal sampling rate when exposed to amphetamine, however this was not measured in the current study. Quantifying this exploratory response of crayfish to a drug-infused water route of exposure is a possible avenue for future study.

Effects of Continuous Chronic Exposure

Following extended immersion (i.e. ‘late exposure’), all d-amphetamine treatment groups

displayed significantly reduced levels of locomotion compared to that observed in the control (no drug) group. At this point, test subjects had been exposed to d-amphetamine for a period of 22-

24 hours, a duration which is considered continuous chronic drug exposure (Robinson and

Becker, 1986). Amphetamine is relatively stable in urine samples with a pH of 6, demonstrating only a 1.3% degradation after three days at 37ºC (Jimenez et al., 2006). Therefore, it is likely that the d-amphetamine to which P. f. f. virginalis was exposed, at a pH of 6.8 and a temperature of

21ºC, was still biologically active throughout the entire 24-hour duration of the experiment. The

ability to test continuous chronic d-amphetamine exposure was made possible by the mode of

drug delivery employed in this study, by direct immersion in drug-infused water. When

psychostimulant effects and drug-induced reward to the administration of cocaine and

amphetamine were demonstrated in O. rusticus, drug delivery was achieved by direct injection

through a cannula surgically implanted in the crayfish carapace (Datta, 2015; Nathaniel et al., 21

2012; Alcaro et al., 2011; Nathaniel et al., 2009; Panksepp and Huber, 2004; Reviewed in Huber

et al., 2011). Such invasive procedures have limited utility in epigenetic studies where the stress

experienced by experimental animals can, itself, induce epigenetic changes in neural tissue

(Hunter et al., 2009). As such, this non-invasive method lends itself nicely for epigenetic studies.

Continuous chronic amphetamine exposure in mammals (humans and rats) results in hyperactivity, and increased locomotion, shortly after the onset of administration (reviewed in

Ellison and Eison, 1983). Rats are administered continuous chronic drug through the use of subcutaneously-implanted, slow-release pellets containing 50 mg amphetamine, which maintain brain drug levels for 10 days (Huberman et al., 1997). In rats however, a few hours after the onset of exposure, this hyperactivity and increased locomotion is gradually curtailed, over approximately an eight-hour period, until their behavior is consumed by motor stereotypies.

These stereotypies last for approximately two days and are then followed by a lengthy period of inactivity (reviewed in Ellison and Eison, 1983). This is similar to the time course of locomotor activity that was generally observed in P. f. f. virginalis: the highest d-amphetamine treatment group (50.0 µM) displayed a significantly higher level of locomotion during the early exposure, then in the late exposure all treatment groups (0.5, 5.0, and 50.0 µM) displayed reduced locomotion compared with controls. However, the increased locomotor response demonstrated in the 50.0 µM treatment group had a much faster onset (within 30 minutes) compared to that seen in rats, which only started to show increased locomotion after a few hours (Ellison and Eison,

1983). Therefore, it is possible that, compared to rats, the behavioral response time course of chronically administered d-amphetamine in P. f. f. Virginalis may be accelerated.

In mammals, amphetamine exposure induces these behavioral changes by stimulating the

release of dopamine and inhibiting the dopamine reuptake transporter (Hyman et al., 2006). This 22

results in prolonged and elevated levels of dopamine that can last for hours, disrupting typical

patterns of dopamine release and motor activity (Knutson et al., 2004). In preliminary

observations of exposure to a high d-amphetamine concentration (50.0 µM), crayfish started to

display motor stereotypies, such as leg twitching, within an hour from onset of continuous

administration. O. rusticus also display muscle tremors in walking legs when administered high levels of amphetamine (Panksepp et al., 2004). This behavioral response to amphetamine inevitably results in reduced locomotion. However, in mammals, the disruption of dopamine

systems that result from chronic exposure also eventually leads to a decrease of stimulation-

dependent dopamine release (Schmitz et al., 2001), which is associated with

(Johnels, 1982; Monti et al., 1990) and results in amphetamine tolerance (Shoblock et al., 2003).

This tolerance and hypoactivity results from a depletion of striatal dopamine and dopamine

receptors, which may be due to dopamine terminal degeneration (a.k.a. amphetamine

neurotoxicity) (reviewed in Robinson and Becker, 1986). Thus, by the late exposure period,

treatment animals may be demonstrating continuous chronic amphetamine exposure-induced

motor stereotypies or hypoactivity.

Conclusion

The present study has demonstrated (1) psychostimulant-induced locomotor effects in a

parthenogenetic crayfish, Procambarus fallax forma virginalis, (2) locomotor response after

continuous amphetamine administration, and (3) an effective non-invasive method of drug

delivery for crustaceans. With initial onset of d-amphetamine administration, P. f. f. virginalis

demonstrated behavioral and locomotor hyperactivity responses, expressed as an increase in

exploratory behaviors, which are comparable to those that have been previously observed in O.

rusticus (Alcaro et al., 2011; Panksepp and Huber, 2004). For the first time, however, the 23

locomotor response of a decapod crustacean has been characterized to continuous chronic d-

amphetamine exposure. After continuous chronic d-amphetamine administration, all treatment

crayfish demonstrated reduced locomotion, which offers a possible avenue for exploring the

molecular mechanisms involved in drug-tolerance.

The non-invasive method of drug administration used in this study offers distinct

advantages and opportunities for future research: it is advantageous for epigenetic studies

because it minimizes potential stressors, it allows for the use of smaller animals with which

direct injection is not feasible, and it may be a method for drug-delivery in animal models that

may be comparable to the human method of inhalation or insufflation. Lastly, as a

parthenogenetic invertebrate, P. f. f. virginalis possesses a genetically identical, modularly organized and relatively simple nervous system with a conserved neural reward circuit.

Therefore, P. f. f. virginalis offers an elegant model system available for exploring many different avenues of research into the basic underlying behavioral, neural, and epigenetic mechanisms of drug-induced neural plasticity.

REFERENCES

Alcaro, A., J. Panksepp, and R. Huber. 2011. D-amphetamine stimulates unconditioned

exploration/approach behaviors in crayfish: Towards a conserved evolutionary function of

ancestral drug reward. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 99(1): 75-80.

Alcaro, A, R. Huber, and J. Panksepp. 2007. Behavioral functions of the mesolimbic

dopaminergic system: an affective neuroethological perspective. Brain Res. Rev., 56(2):

283-321. 24

Andretic, R., S. Chaney, and J. Hirsh. 1999. Requirement of circadian for cocaine

sensitization in Drosophila. Science, 285: 1066-1068.

Basil, J., and D. Sandeman. 2000. Crayfish (Cherax destructor) use tactile cues to detect and

learn topographical changes in their Environment. Ethology, 106: 247-259.

Beltz, B. S. 1999. Distribution and functional anatomy of amine-containing neurons in decapod

crustaceans. Microsc. Res. Tech., 44: 105-120.

Berridge, K. C., and T. E. Robinson. 1998. What is the role of dopamine in reward: hedonic

impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain Res. Rev., 28(3): 309-369.

Bestor, T. H., 2000. The DNA methyltransferases of mammals. Hum. Mol. Genet., 9: 2395-2402.

Bhimani, R., and R. Huber. 2015. Operant avoidance learning in crayfish, Orconectes rusticus:

Computational ethology and the development of an automated learning paradigm. Learning

and Behavior, 1: 1-11.

Butler, A. B., and W. Hodos. 2005. Comparative Vertebrate : Evolution and

Adaptation. Hoboken, John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Carew, T. J., and C. L. Sahley. 1986. Invertebrate learning and memory: from behavior to

molecules. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 9(1): 435-487.

Clarac, F., and E. Pearlstein. 2007. Invertebrate preparations and their contribution to

neurobiology in the second half of the 20th century. Brain Research Reviews, 54(1): 113-

161.

Chao, J., and E. J. Nestler. 2004. Molecular neurobiology of drug addiction. Annu. Rev. Med.,

55: 113-132. 25

Datta, U. 2015. Characterization of drug reward in an invertebrate model system using operant

conditioning paradigms. (unpublished Doctoral Dissertation). Bowling Green State

University, Bowling Green, OH.

de Wit, H., B. Bodker, and J. Ambre. 1992. Rate of increase of plasma drug level influences

subjective response in humans. , 107: 352-358.

Di Chiara, G. and V. Bassareo. 2007. Reward system and addiction: what dopamine does and

doesn’t do. Current Opinion Pharmacology, 7(1): 69-76.

Dziopa, L., A. Imeh-Nathaniel, D. Baier, M. Kiel, S. Sameera, A. Brager, et al., 2011. Morphine-

conditioned cue alters c-fos protein expression in the brain of crayfish. Brain Research

Bulletin, 85(6), 385-395.

Edwards, D. H., F. I. Issa, and J. Herberholz. 2003. The neural basis of dominance hierarchy

formation in crayfish. Microsci. Res. Tech. 60: 369-73.

Ellison, G. D. and M. S. Eison. 1983. Continuous amphetamine intoxication: an animal model of

the acute psychotic episode. Psychol. Med., 13: 751-762.

Fog, R. 1969. Stereotyped and non-stereotyped behaviour in rats induced by various stimulant

drugs. Psychopharmacology (Berlin, Germany), 14(4): 299-304.

Gorelick, D. A. 1998. The rate hypothesis and agonist substitution approaches to cocaine abuse

treatment. Adv. Pharmacol., 42: 995-997.

Hagen, E. H., C. J. Roulette, and R. J. Sullivan. 2013. Explaining human recreational use of

‘pesticides’: the regulation model of substance use vs. the hijack model and

implications for age and sex differences in drug consumption. Frontiers in ,

4(142): 1-21. 26

Hills, T., P. J. Brockie, and A. V. Maricq. 2004. Dopamine and glutamate control area-restricted

search behavior in Caenorhabditis elegans. J. Neurosci., 24(5): 1217-1225.

Hoebel, B., A. Monaco, L. Hernandez, E. Aulisi, B. G. Stanley, and L. Lenard. 1983. Self-

injection of amphetamine directly into the brain. Psychopharmacology, 81(2): 158-163.

Huber, R., J. B. Panksepp, T. Nathaniel, A. Alcaro, and J. Panksepp. 2011. Drug-sensitive

reward in crayfish: an invertebrate model system for the study of SEEKING, reward,

addiction, and withdrawal. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35:1847-1853.

Huber, R., and A. Delago. 1998. Serotonin alters decisions to withdraw in fighting crayfish,

Astacus astacus: the motivational concept revisited. J. Comp. Physiol., 182: 573-83.

Huberman, H. S., M. S. Eison, K. S. Bryan, and G. Ellison. 1997. A slow-release pellet for

chronic amphetamine administration. European Journal of Pharmacology, 45: 237.

Hunter, R. G., K. J. McCarthy, T. A. Milne, D. W. Pfaff, and B. S. McEwen. 2009. Regulation

of hippocampal H3 histone methylation by acute and chronic stress. PNAS, 106: 20912-

20917.

Hyman, S. E., C. M. Robert, and E. J. Nestler. 2006. Neural mechanisms of addiction: the role of

reward-related learning and memory. Annu. Rev. Neurosci., 29: 565-598.

Ikemoto, S., and J. Panksepp. 1999. The role of nucleus accumbens dopamine in motivated

behavior: A unifying interpretation with special reference to reward-seeking. Brain

Research Reviews, 31(1): 6-41.

Jaenisch, R., and A. Bird. 2003. Epigenetic regulation of gene expression: how the genome

integrates intrinsic and environmental signals. Nature Supplement, 33: 245-254. 27

Jimenez, C., R. de la Torre, M. Ventura, J. Segura, and R. Ventura. 2006. Stability studies of

amphetamine and ephedrine derivatives in urine. Journal of Chromatography B, 843: 84-

93.

Johnels, B. 1982. Locomotor hypokinesia in the reserpine-treated rat: drug effects from the

corpus striatum and nucleus accumbens. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 17:

283-289.

Kalivas, P. W., and K. McFarland. 2003. Brain circuitry and the reinstatement of cocaine seeking

behavior. Psychopharmacology, 168: 44-56.

Kalivas, P. W., and N. D. Volkow. 2005. The neural basis of addiction: A pathology of

motivation and choice. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(8): 1403-1413.

Kaun, K. R., R. Azanchi, Z. Maung, J. Hirsh, and U. Heberlein. 2011. Drosophila model for

alcohol reward. Nat. Neurosci., 14: 612-619.

Kaun, K. R., A. V. Devineni, and U. Heberlein. 2012. Drosophila melanogaster as a model to

study drug addiction. Human Genetics, 131(6): 959-975.

Kelley, A. E., and K. C. Berridge. 2002. The neuroscience of natural rewards: Relevance to

addictive drugs. The Journal of Neuroscience, 22(9): 3306-3311.

Knutson, B, J. M. Bjork, G. W. Fong, D. Hommer, V. S. Mattay, and D. R. Weinberger. 2004.

Amphetamine modulates human incentive processing. , 43: 261-69.

Koch, L. M., B. W. Patullo, and D. L. Macmillan. 2006. Exploring with damaged antennae: Do

crayfish compensate for injuries? The Journal of Experimental Biology, 209(16): 3226-

3233.

Koob, G. F., and N. D. Volkow. 2010. Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacol, 35:

217-238. 28

Kravitz, E. A., 2000. Serotonin and aggression: insights gained from a lobster model system and

speculations on the role of amine neurons in a complex behavior. J. Comp. Physiol., 186:

221-38.

Marinelli, M., 2005. The many facets of the locomotor response to a novel environment test:

Theoretical comment on Mitchell, Cunningham, and Mark. ,

119(4): 1144-1151.

McClung, C., and J. Hirsh. 1998. Stereotypical behavioral responses to free-base cocaine and the

development of behavioral sensitization in Drosophila. Curr. Biol., 8(2): 109-12.

McGrath, L. L., S. V. Vollmer, S. T. Kaluziak, and J. Ayers. 2016. De novo transcriptome

assembly for the lobster Homarus americanus and characterization of differential gene

expression across nervous system tissues. BMC Genomics, 17: 63.

Mellon Jr., D., 2000. Convergence of multimodal sensory input onto higher-level neurons of the

crayfish olfactory pathway. J. Neurophysiol., 84(6): 3043-3055.

Minogianis, E. A., D. Levesque, and A. N. Samaha. 2013. The speed of cocaine delivery

determines the subsequent motivation to self-administer the drug.

Neuropsychopharmacology, 38(13): 2644-2656.

Monti, J. M., M. Fernandez, and H. Jantos, 1990. during acute dopamine D1 agonist SKF

38393 or D1 antagonist SCH 23390 administration in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology,

3(3): 153-162.

Nathaniel, T. I., J. Panksepp, and R. Huber. 2012. Alteration of c-Fos mRNA in the accessory

lobe of crayfish is associated with a conditioned-cocaine induced reward. Neuroscience

Research, 72: 243-256. 29

Nathaniel, T. I., J. Panksepp, and R. Huber. 2010. Effects of a single and repeated morphine

treatment on conditioned and unconditioned behavioral sensitization in crayfish.

Behavioural Brain Research, 207(2): 310-320.

Nathaniel, T. I., J. Panksepp, and R. Huber. 2009. Drug-seeking behavior in an invertebrate

system: Evidence of morphine-induced reward, extinction and reinstatement in crayfish.

Behavioural Brain Research, 197(2), 331-338.

Nelson, R. A., S. J. Boyd, R. C. Ziegelstein, R. Herning, J. L. Cadet, J. E. Henningfield, et al.

2006. Effect of rate of administration on subjective and physiological effects of intravenous

cocaine in humans. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 82(1): 19-24.

Nestler, E. J., 2014. Epigenetic mechanisms of drug addiction. , 76: 259-

268.

O’Brien, C. P., A. R. Childress, R. Ehrman, and S. J. Robbins. 1998. Conditioning factors in

drug abuse: can they explain compulsion? Journal of Psychopharmacology, 12(1): 15-22.

Panksepp, J. B., and R. Huber. 2004. Ethological analyses of crayfish behavior: A new

invertebrate system for measuring the rewarding properties of psychostimulants.

Behavioural Brain Research, 153(1): 171-180.

Panksepp, J., B. Knutson, and J. Burgdorf. 2002. The role of brain emotional systems in

: A neuro-evolutionary perspective and new ‘self-report’ animal model.

Addiction, 97(4): 459-469.

Polanska, M. A., A. Yasuda, S. Harzsch. 2007. Immunolocalisation of crustacean-SIFamide in

the median brain and eyestalk neuropils of the marbled crayfish. Cell Tissue Res., 330: 331-

344. 30

Robinson, T. E., and J. B. Becker. 1986. Enduring changes in brain and behavior produced by

chronic amphetamine administration: a review and evaluation of animal models of

amphetamine . Brain Research Reviews, 11: 157-198.

Samaha, A-N., and T. E. Robinson. 2005. Why does the rapid delivery of drugs to the brain

promote addiction? Trends Pharmacol. Sci., 26: 82-87.

Sandeman, R. E., A. H. Watson, and D.C. Sandeman. 1995. Ultrastructure of the synaptic

terminals of the dorsal giant serotonin-IR neuron and deutocerebral commissure

interneurons in the accessory and olfactory lobes of the crayfish. J. Comp. Neurol., 361(4):

617-632.

Sandeman, R. E., and D. C. Sandeman. 1987. Serotonin-like immunoreactivity of giant olfactory

interneurons in the crayfish brain. Brain Res., 403(2): 371-374.

Schindler, C. W., L. V. Panlilio, and E. B. Thorndike. 2009. Effect of rate of delivery of

intravenous cocaine on self-administration in rats. Pharmacology Biochemistry and

Behavior, 93(4): 375-381.

Schiorring, E., 1971. Amphetamine induced selective stimulation of certain behaviour items with

concurrent inhibition of others in an open-field test with rats. Behaviour, 39(1): 1-17.

Schm., idt, M 1997. Distribution of centrifugal neurons targeting the soma clusters of the

olfactory midbrain among decapod crustaceans. Brain Res., 752(1-2): 15-25.

Schmitz, Y., C. J. Lee, C. Schmauss, F. Gonon, and D. Sulzer. 2001. Amphetamine distorts

stimulation-dependent dopamine overflow: effects on D2 autoreceptors, transporters, and

synaptic vesicle stores. The journal of Neuroscience, 21(16): 5916-5924. 31

Segal, D. S., and A. J. Mandell. 1974. Long-term administration of d-amphetamine: Progressive

augmentation of motor activity and stereotypy. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior,

2(2): 249-255.

Shoblock, J. R., I. M. Maisonneuve, and S. D. Glick. 2003. Differences between d-

and d-amphetamine in rats: , tolerance, and extinction.

Psychopharmacology, 170: 150-156.

Sovik, E., and A. B. Barron. 2013. Invertebrate Models in Addiction Research. Brain Behav.

Evol., 82: 153-165.

Spanagel, R., and F. Weiss. 1999. The dopamine hypothesis of reward: past and current status.

Trends Neurosci., 22: 521-527.

Sullivan, J. M., and B. S. Beltz. 2005. Integration and segregation of inputs to higher-order

neuropils of the crayfish brain. J. Comp. Neurol., 481(1): 118-126.

Sullivan, J. M., and B. S. Beltz. 2001. Development and connectivity of olfactory pathways in

the brain of the lobster Homarus americanus. J. Comp. Neurol., 441: 23-43.

Tierney, A. N., T. Kim, and R. Abrams. 2003. Dopamine in crayfish and other crustaceans:

distribution in the central nervous system and physiological functions. Microsc. Res.

Techn., 60: 325-335.

Volkow, N. D., G. J. Wang, M. W. Fischman, R. Foltin, J. S. Fowler, D. Franceschi, et al., 2000.

Effects of route of administration on cocaine induced dopamine transporter blockade in the

. Life Sciences, 67(12): 1507-1515.

Wise, R. A., 2000. Addiction becomes a brain disease. Neuron, 26: 27-33.

Wise, R. A., 1998. Drug-activation of brain reward pathways. Drug alcohol depend., 51: 13-22. 32

Wise, R. A., M. A. Bozarth. 1987. A psychomotor stimulant theory of addiction. Psychol. Rev.,

94(4): 469-492.

Wise, R. A. 1996. Neurobiology of addiction. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol., 6: 243-251.

Wolf, F. W., and U. Heberlein. 2003. Invertebrate models of drug abuse. Journal of

Neruobiology, 54(1): 161-178. 33

CHAPTER II: OPTIMIZATION OF GENOMIC DNA EXTRATION FROM MARBLED

CRAYFISH (Procambarus fallax forma virginalis) BRAIN TISSUE

INTRODUCTION

Epigenetic changes occurring in neural and brain tissue may affect an animal’s behavior via subsequent changes in gene expression levels (reviewed in Crews, 2010; Nestler, 2014).

Several studies, in humans and mammalian animal models, suggest that DNA methylation is one

of the critical epigenetic regulators of neural gene expression (Brooks et al., 1996; Endres

et al., 2000; Endres et al., 2001; Fan et al., 2001). For instance, rapid DNA methylation

regulation has been demonstrated in the hippocampus following fear conditioning (Miller and

Sweatt, 2007). DNA methylation changes in the nucleus accumbens may also be an important

mechanism underlying behavioral sensitization to repeated cocaine exposure (Anier et al., 2010).

Although DNA methylation seems to play a vital role in neural gene expression, its processes are

not well understood. Robust and effective methods for extracting genomic DNA (gDNA) from

target neural tissues are thus vital to studying the underlying epigenetic mechanisms of behavior.

Although global DNA methylation has been implicated as a vital mechanism regulating

neural gene expression and, by extension, behavior, the large genome and high (ca. 70%)

methylation rates found in mammals represent difficulties in parsing out the particularities of the

processes. Because invertebrates have smaller genomes (Adams, 2000; Weinstock et al., 2006),

global DNA methylation studies are more feasible. Unfortunately, many invertebrate models,

including C. elegans and Drosophila, do not have fully functional DNA methylation systems at

all life stages and are therefore unsuitable for these studies (reviewed in Lyko and Maleszka,

2011). 34

The marbled crayfish (P. f. f. virginalis), has significant advantages for research into the

epigenetic mechanisms of behavior. It demonstrates DNA methylation during all life stages

(Vogt et al., 2008), and exhibits relatively low levels of global DNA methylation (between 1-

2%) for the two tissues (abdominal musculature and hepatopancreas) that have been measured

thus far (Vogt et al, 2008; Schiewek et al., 2007). Because small changes in global DNA

methylation can have large downstream effects on phenotype (Hinendleder et al., 2006) this is

advantageous as it is easier to detect small changes in methylation when overall methylation rate

is low (reviewed in Lyko and Maleszka, 2011). Additionally, P. f. f. virginalis is the only

decapod of over 10,000, that reproduces obligatorily through apomictic parthenogenesis: all

individuals are genetically identical, asexually reproducing, females (Scholtz et al., 2003; Martin

et al., 2007; Vogt et al., 2008). Minimizing genetic variability amongst experimental animals removes a significant confounding factor. However, behavioral epigenetic changes have yet to be

studied in the crayfish.

Common problems in working with invertebrates derive from the related challenges

inherent in lab rearing requirements and small body size, which limits tissue sample sizes.

Consequently, tissue samples from multiple individuals often need to be pooled in order to

obtain sufficient DNA for genetic analyses (Wang et al., 2006; Lockett et al., 2012). Moreover, epigenetic modifications are tissue-specific (reviewed in Jaenisch and Bird, 2003), hence behavioral epigenetic studies require DNA extracted from neural tissue, a fatty material which yields lower concentrations of DNA. In order to fully exploit the potential of invertebrates as model systems for studying the epigenetics of behavior, it is essential to have effective methods

for DNA extraction from small sample sizes. 35

The present study seeks to provide the parthenogenetic P f. f. virginalis as a model organism for studying the molecular mechanisms underlying behavioral epigenetics. Modifying the procedure of the Qiagen Puregene Blood Core Kit B (Qiagen, MD) for small tissue samples,

I optimized the method for gDNA extraction from invertebrate brain tissue. The results of this work provide the methodological foundation for use of the parthenogenetic P f. f. virginalis to

study the epigenetic processes underlying behavior.

METHODS

Euthanasia, Dissection and Brain Extraction

Juvenile P f. f. virginalis (15-19 mm; 86.2-136.0 mg) were dissected immediately after being deeply anaesthetized in an ice-slurry for at least 20 minutes. The crayfish was decapitated, the antennae were removed, and the head was mounted and pinned onto a Sylgard dish anterior side down and covered in 125 mM saline solution. Under a microscope, what remained of the stomach, antennal glands, and other tissue was removed to expose the brain. The lateral brain connections and eyestalks were dissected and the brain moved from the head to a wet slide where any remaining non-neural tissue was removed. The crayfish body and the brain were placed in separate, correspondingly labeled, screw-top 1.5 mL microcentrifuge tubes, flash frozen in liquid nitrogen, and stored at -20˚C to await subsequent processing.

Preparation of Crayfish Brain and Abdominal Tissue Samples

Due to the small size of the juvenile crayfish brain, samples were prepared with an

increasing number of brains pooled together in order to optimize the extraction of total amount of DNA necessary for subsequent genetic analysis, with minimal number of samples pooled. 36

For abdomen samples, frozen body samples (brains removed) were thawed on ice. The

abdomen was dissected from the body and the carapace removed. Abdominal tissue was then

dissected and weighed into desired quantities. Prepared samples were placed in separate

containers for subsequent analysis.

Genomic DNA Extraction

Genomic DNA was isolated from frozen samples of brain or abdomen tissue with Qiagen

Puregene Blood Core Kit B according to the ‘DNA purification from tissue’ protocol included in

the supplier’s instruction manual (Qiagen, MD). In order to accommodate small sample volume

and optimize DNA yield, the supplier’s standard protocol was modified as follows.

Procedure for cell lysis from small samples: Frozen samples (each in 1.5 mL centrifuge

tube) were placed in ice. Tissue cells were lysed by adding 300 µL Cell Lysis Solution together

with five 1 mm glass beads to each sample, then vortexed for 20 seconds. Proteinase K (30 mg) was added and then mixed by 25 gentle inversions. In order to gain maximum DNA yield, samples were incubated at 55ºC in an AccuBlock Digital Dry Bath (Labnet international, Inc.) overnight (~12 hr).

Procedure for gDNA extraction from small samples: Samples were removed from the dry bath and treated with RNase A solution (1.5 mg), gently inverted 25 times, and incubated at 37ºC for 60 minutes. Samples were cooled for 1 minute on ice. Proteins were then precipitated by

vortexing at high speed for 20 seconds with 300 µL Protein Precipitation Solution, followed by 5

minutes’ centrifugation at 11,000 rpm. The supernatant was transferred to new centrifuge tubes

containing 300 µL isopropanol, gently inverted 50 times, and then DNA was precipitated by

centrifugation for 5 minutes at 11,000 rpm. The isopropanol was discarded. The DNA pellet was then washed with 300 µL of 70% and centrifuged at 11,000 rpm for 5 minutes. Ethanol

37

supernatant was removed and DNA pellet was allowed to air dry at 55ºC for 5 minutes, until no trace of ethanol remained. DNA was then resuspended by vortexing at medium speed for 5 seconds with 20 µL distilled water. Samples were then incubated for 1 hour at 65ºC to resuspend

DNA, after which they were incubated at 21ºC with gentle shaking (35 rpm) for 18 hours. The next day, samples were briefly centrifuged and analyzed for DNA purity and concentration using

a Thermoscientific NanoDrop 2000 Spectrophotomer. Final gDNA preparations were either run on a gel as an additional quality check, or stored at -20ºC for subsequent molecular analysis.

Methylation-sensitive Enzyme Digestion

Methylation sensitive enzyme digestion was performed with the procedure described in

Singh (2014). Each gDNA sample was separately digested with Msp I and Hpa II (New England

BioLabs Inc.) which both recognize the sequence (5’-CCGG-3’). The presence or absence of a

methyl group attached to the internal cytosine determines whether either enzyme will cut or not.

Although Msp I cuts this sequence regardless of methylation, Hpa II will only do so if the

internal cytosine is unmethylated (McCelland et al., 1994). Samples were digested at 37ºC

overnight and then, to confirm digestion, run on a 1.5 % agarose gel.

An additional digestion of the samples was performed with the enzyme CpG

Methyltransferasease M.SssI and then with the enzyme McrBC (New England BioLabs Inc.).

CpG Methyltransferase M.SssI adds a methyl group to the 5-carbon of CpG islands, recognizing the sequence (5’-CG-3’). McrBC cleaves these methylated CpG sites, but it does not recognize the methylation of the internal cytosine of (5’-CCGG-3’), which is the sequence recognized by

Msp I and Hpa II. Genomic DNA samples digested with CpG Methyltransferase M.SssI were prepared with 1.4 µL dH2O, 5 µL 10x NEB buffer 2, 5 µL of SAM (1600 µM), 36.1 µL gDNA

(1000 ng), and 2.5 µL CpG Methyltransferase M.SssI (4 U/µL). Genomic DNA samples to be 38

digested with McrBC were prepared with 0.4 µL BSA (100x), 0.4 µL GTP (100x), 4 µL buffer

(10x), 1 µL McrBC, and 14.2 µL gDNA (393 ng). Genomic DNA samples digested with CpG

Methyltransferase M.SssI were digested at 37ºC for one hour, and then 65ºC for twenty minutes.

Both gDNA samples that had and had not been digested with CpG Methyltransferase M.SssI were then digested with McrBC at 37ºC for three hours and then at 65ºC for twenty minutes.

RESULTS

Genomic DNA Extracted from Different Starting Amounts of Abdominal Tissue

When DNA extraction from P f. f. virginalis abdominal tail tissue was conducted following the unmodified protocol for tissue samples from Qiagen Puregene Blood Core Kit B,

the procedure resulted in no obtained product (data not shown). After procedure modification

(detailed in methods above), each successively larger starting tissue sample yielded higher

concentrations and total amounts of extracted gDNA (Table 2.1). The smallest starting tissue

sample (1 mg) yielded a concentration of 8.5 ng/µL gDNA, and a total of 42.5 ng gDNA run on

the gel is visible as a very faint band (Figure 2.1). Each larger starting tissue sample resulted in

higher gDNA concentrations. Quality of DNA was visualized by agarose gel electrophoresis. 39

Table 2.1: Total genomic DNA extracted from various amounts of P. f. f. virginalis abdominal tissue.

Starting tissue Extracted gDNA Total gDNA yield quantity (mg) concentration (ng/ (ng) µL)

1 8.5 42.5

2 15.6 78.0 5 27.4 137.0 10 33.7 168.5

Figure 2.1: Genomic DNA extracted from varying amounts of P. f. f. virginalis abdominal tissue. Five µL of each extract was loaded on the gel. Amount of starting tissue and total DNA added to gel reported for each sample respectively: lane (1) 1 mg,

42.5 ng; (2) 2 mg, 78.0 ng; (3) 5 mg, 137.0 ng; (4) 10 mg, 168.5 ng. L = 1 kilobase pair

DNA ladder (300 ng). 40

Genomic DNA Extracted from Assorted Number of Pooled Whole Brains

When first extracting DNA from crayfish brain tissue, all modified procedures detailed in methods for tissue samples from Qiagen Puregene Blood Core Kit B were followed apart from the cell lysis procedure, which was achieved by physical homogenization in liquid nitrogen.

These early DNA extractions demonstrated that samples comprising a larger number of crayfish brains generally yielded higher concentrations of extracted gDNA (Table 2.2; Figure 2.2). The tissue samples shown in lanes 3 (homogenized) and 4 (not homogenized, Figure 2.2) were differentially treated in order to examine the effect of physical lysis on very small tissue samples. gDNA yields were comparable in both treatments. Most of the tissue from the sample initially containing 3 brains (lane 5, Figure 2.2) was lost during the cell lysis procedure due to human error, and therefore resulted in lower concentration of extracted gDNA. Overall, samples containing more than 1 brain demonstrated visible bands when run on agarose gel electrophoresis (with the exception of that in lane 5 which was lost due to a processing error). 41

Table 2.2: Total genomic DNA extracted from assorted number of pooled P. f. f. virginalis brains during the early stage of DNA extraction optimization. Much tissue from sample containing starting tissue quantity of three brains was lost due to an error in the cell lysis procedure, resulting in low total DNA extracted.

Starting quantity Extracted gDNA Total gDNA yield of brains concentration (ng/ (ng) µL)

1 7.0 35.0

1 4.7 23.5 2 5.6 28.0 2 5.9 29.5 3 4.5 22.5 6 25.3 126.5 42

Figure 2.2: Genomic DNA extracted from assorted number of pooled P. f. f. virginalis brains during the early stage of DNA extraction optimization. Number of brains per sample in lanes 1 through 6 are 1, 1, 2, 2, 3, and 6 respectively. Five µL of each extract was loaded on the gel. Total gDNA added to gel reported for each sample respectively:

(1) 35 ng; (2) 23.5 ng; (3) 28.0 ng; (4) 29.5 ng; (5) 22.5 ng; (6) 126.5 ng. Much tissue from sample 5 was lost due to an error in the cell lysis procedure, resulting in low total

DNA extracted. L = 1 kilobase pair DNA ladder (300 ng). 43

All subsequent gDNA extractions were conducted with the fully optimized method detailed in methods, with cell lysis conducted through physical homogenization with glass beads.

Pools of three brains each were utilized and resulted in a mean concentration of 19.06 ng/µL ±

24.49 SD; mean purity = 1.83 ± 0.09, n = 10 (Table 3).

Table 2.3: Total genomic DNA extracted from pooled P. f. f. virginalis brains after

optimization of the DNA extraction method.

Starting number Purity (260/280) Extracted gDNA Total gDNA yield of brains concentration (ng/ (ng) µL)

3 1.75 8.7 174

3 1.82 9.8 196 3 1.80 90.9 1,818 3 1.82 11.6 232 3 1.80 9.3 186 3 2.06 4.6 92 3 1.77 6.8 136 3 1.95 8.8 176 3 1.75 23.8 476 3 1.77 16.3 326

Methylation-Sensitive Enzyme Digests

Controlled enzymatic digestion of both low (8.4 ng/µL) and high (21.2 ng/µL)

concentrations of gDNA with Msp I and Hpa II separately resulted in smearing patterns (Figure

2.3). Both enzymes produced distinctly stronger gel banding patterns in the higher concentration 44 digests. Results from CpG Methyltransferase M.SssI and McrBC digests were unable to be interpreted due to technical error (data not shown).

Figure 2.3: Methylation sensitive enzyme (Msp I and Hpa II) digests of low and high

concentrations of P. f. f. virginalis gDNA. Samples were as follows: (1) low

concentration undigested gDNA (84.0 ng); (2) low concentration (84.0 ng) gDNA

digested with Msp I; (3) low concentration (84.0 ng) gDNA digested with Hpa II; (4)

high concentration undigested gDNA (212.0 ng); (5) high concentration (212.0 ng)

gDNA digested with Msp I; (6) high concentration (212.0 ng) gDNA digested with Hpa

II. L = 1 kilobase pair DNA ladder (300 ng).

DISCUSSION

The tissue-specific nature of epigenetic changes (reviewed in Jaenisch and Bird, 2003) necessitates the use of DNA extracted from neural tissue if one is interested in studying 45

epigenetic mechanisms that influence behavior. Using invertebrate brains represents technical

challenges in small sample sizes and fatty tissues with low DNA yield. Developing an effective gDNA extraction method for such tissues will enable much research to be conducted using potentially useful model organisms, such as the parthenogenetic marbled crayfish, to parse out the underlying mechanisms involved in epigenetically-mediated behaviors.

Genomic DNA Extraction

Using samples of abdominal muscle tissue (ranging from 1 mg to 10 mg) from juvenile

P. f. f. virginalis, first I confirmed that the Qiagen Puregene Blood Core Kit B (which is produced for the extraction of DNA from mammalian blood cells), with some modifications, could be used to extract high-quality and high quantities of gDNA from invertebrate crayfish neural tissue (mean purity = 1.83 ± 0.09, n = 10). As expected, larger quantities of starting material provided concomitantly larger gDNA yields.

Results of early attempts showed similarly effective isolation of gDNA from neural brain tissue (Figure 2.2) with a concentration of 5.85 ± 1.15 ng/µL and a mean purity of 2.17 ± 0.26 from starting tissue of single brain. Following a series of optimization procedures (detailed below), later attempts provided much higher yields of gDNA, on the order of 19.06 ng/µL ±

24.49 SD from samples of three pooled brains. Different amounts of total gDNA extracted from starting tissue of three pooled brains may be due to different donor specimen size and/or human error. These three pooled brain samples were preserved for subsequent genetic analysis and were hence not run through gel electrophoresis nor illustrated here.

Similar to initial tests using muscle tissue, larger amounts of starting brain tissue generally yielded higher gDNA concentrations and total amounts. Tests in which two equal samples of starting neural tissue were subjected to different physical treatments yielded

46

interesting results. Whether samples underwent additional physical disruption during the cell

lysis procedure or not, both treatments yielded approximately equivalent amounts of high quality

gDNA. This is comparable to what was found by Wang et al. (2006), who reported that delicate

tissues, such as the honey bee brain, dissolve sufficiently well in NTE buffer and Proteinase K as

to render physical disruption superfluous. This suggests that the physical homogenization step

may be omitted in the case of crayfish brain tissue, although it may be of benefit in processing

larger volume samples. Physical homogenization of small invertebrate neural tissue samples in

liquid nitrogen represented significant difficulties. When frozen in liquid nitrogen, the neural tissue hardened and would often stick to the pestle, preventing it from being broken up or pulverized and would result in much of the tissue sample being lost. Homogenization via glass beads was a much more effective cell lysis method and resulted in fewer complications.

Methylation-Sensitive Enzyme Digests

Methylation-sensitive enzymes Msp I and Hpa II, both resulted in smears on digesting P.

f. f. virginalis gDNA. Since both of these enzymes recognize the identical sequence (5’-CCGG-

3 ’: Msp I cuts regardless of methylation and Hpa II cuts only when the sequence is

unmethylated), this demonstrates that P. f. f. virginalis possesses a substantial number of this

motif in its genome. Previous studies have demonstrated that the P. f. f. virginalis

hepatopancreas and abdominal musculature exhibits low levels of methylation (Vogt et al., 2008;

Schiewek et al., 2007). Quantification of methylation levels in the brain of P. f. f. virginalis has

not previously been reported. In addition to using Msp I and Hpa II, the use of the enzymes CpG

Methyltransferase M.SssI, which adds methyl groups to the 5-carbon of cytosines located in CpG

islands in the sequence (5’-CG-3’), and McrBC, which cleaves at said methylated locations, may

provide a useful accompaniment in quantifying methylation levels in the P. f. f. virginalis neural 47

genome. Although I was unable to reach this endpoint, the present study does provide the basis on which such measures may be determined in the future.

This optimized method for extracting genomic DNA from crayfish brain tissue will significantly enhance the utility of the parthenogenetic marbled crayfish as an animal model for use in research studying the molecular epigenetic processes underlying behavior.

REFERENCES

Anier, K., K. Malinovskaja, A. Aonurm-Helm, A. Zharkovsky, and A. Kalda. 2010. DNA

methylation regulates cocaine-induced behavioral sensitization in mice.

Neuropsychopharmacology, 35: 2450-2461.

Adams, M. D., S. E. Celniker, R. A. Holt, C. A. Evans, J. D. Gocayne, P. G. Amanatides, et al.,

2000. The genome sequence of Drosophila melanogaster. Science, 287: 2185-2195.

Brooks, P. J., C. Marietta, D. Goldman. 1996. DNA mismatch repair and DNA methylation in

adult brain neurons. J. Neurosci., 16: 939-3181.

Crews, D., 2010. Epigenetic modifications of brain and behavior: theory and practice. Hormones

and Behavior, 59: 393-398.

Endres, M., G. Fan, A. Meisel, U. Dirnagl, R. Jaenisch. 2001. Effects of cerebral ischemia in

mice lacking DNA methyltransferase 1 in post-miotic neurons. NeruoReport, 12: 3763-

3766.

Endres, M., A. Meisel, D. Biniszkiewicz, S. Namura, K. Prass, K. Ruscher, et al., 2000. DNA

methyltransferase contributes to delayed ischemic brain injury. J Neurosci., 20: 3175-3181. 48

Fan, G., C. Beard, R. Z. Chen, G. Csankovszki, Y. Sun, M. Siniaia, et al., 2001. DNA

hypomethylation perturbs the function and survival of CNS neurons in postnatal animals. J.

Neurosci., 21: 788-797.

Hiendleder, S., M. Wirtz, C. Mund, M. Klempt, H. D. Reichenbach, M. Stojkovic, M. Weppert,

H. Wenigerkind, M. Elmlinger, F. Lyko et al., 2006. Tissuespecific effects of in vitro

fertilization procedures on genomic cytosine methylation levels in overgrown and normal

sized bovine fetuses. Biol. Reprod., 75: 17-23.

Jaenisch, R., and A. Bird. 2003. Epigenetic regulation of gene expression: how the genome

integrates intrinsic and environmental signals. Nature Genetics Supplement, 33: 245-254.

Lockett, G. A., R. Kucharski, and R. Maleszka. 2012. DNA methylation changes elicited by

social stimuli in the brains of worker honey bees. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 11: 235-242.

Lyko, F., and R. Maleszka. 2011. Insects as innovative models for functional studies of DNA

methylation. Trends Genet., 27: 127-131.

Martin, P., K. Kohlmann, and G. Scholtz. 2007. The parthenogenetic Marmorkrebs (marbled

crayfish) produces genetically uniform offspring. Naturwissenschaften, 94: 843-846.

McClelland, M., M. Nelson, and E. Raschke. 1994. Effect of site-specific modification on

restriction endonucleases and DNA modification methyltransferases. Nucleic Acids

Research, 22(17): 3640-3659.

Miller, C. A., and J. D. Sweatt. 2007. Covalent modification of DNA regulates memory

formation. Neuron, 53: 857-869.

Nestler, E. J. 2014. Epigenetic mechanisms of drug addiction. Neuropharmacology, 76: 259-268. 49

Schiewek, R., M. Wirtz, M. Thiemann, K. Plitt, G. Vogt, and O. J. Schmitz. 2007. Determination

of the DNA methylation level of the marbled crayfish: An increase in sample throughput

by an optimized sample preparation. Journal of Chromatography B, 850: 548-552.

Scholtz, G., A. Braband, L. Tolley, A. Reimann, B. Mittmann, C. Lukhaup, F. Steuerwald, and

G. Vogt. 2003. Parthenogenesis in an outsider crayfish. Nature, 421: 806.

Singh, K. P., 2014. Screening of DNA Methylation Changes by Methylation-Sensitive Random

Amplified Polymorphic DNA-Polymerase Chain Reaction (MS-RAPD-PCR). In

Keohavong, P., and Grant, S. G. (Ed.), Molecular Toxicology Protocols (pp. 71-81). New

York, NY: Spring Science and Business Media.

Vogt, G., M. Huber, M. Thiemann, G. van den Boogaart, O. J. Schmitz, and C. D. Schubart.

2008. Production of different phenotypes from the same genotype in the same environment

by developmental variation. The Journal of Experimental Biology, 211: 510-523.

Wang, Y., M. Jorda, P. L. Jones, R. Maleszka, X. Ling., H. M. Robertson, C. A. Mizzen, M. A.

Peinado, and G. E. Robinson. 2006. Functional CpG Methylation System in a Social Insect.

Science, 314: 645-647.

Weinstock, G. M., G. E. Robinson, R. A. Gibbs, K. C. Worley, J. D. Evans, R. Maleszka, et al.,

2006: Insights into social insects from the genome of the honeybee Apismellifera. Nature,

443: 931-949.