Neuroscience, Mindreading, and the Courts: the Example of Pain Henry T

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Neuroscience, Mindreading, and the Courts: the Example of Pain Henry T Journal of Health Care Law and Policy Volume 18 Issue 2 Imaging the Brain, Changing Minds: Chronic Article 2 Pain Neuroimaging and the Law Neuroscience, Mindreading, and the Courts: The Example of Pain Henry T. Greely Follow this and additional works at: http://digitalcommons.law.umaryland.edu/jhclp Part of the Health Law Commons, and the Neurosciences Commons Recommended Citation Henry T. Greely, Neuroscience, Mindreading, and the Courts: The Example of Pain, 18 J. Health Care L. & Pol'y 171 (2015). Available at: http://digitalcommons.law.umaryland.edu/jhclp/vol18/iss2/2 This Symposium is brought to you for free and open access by the Academic Journals at DigitalCommons@UM Carey Law. It has been accepted for inclusion in Journal of Health Care Law and Policy by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@UM Carey Law. For more information, please contact [email protected]. NEUROSCIENCE, MINDREADING, AND THE COURTS: THE EXAMPLE OF PAIN HENRY T. GREELY* Our brains hold about 100 billion neurons.1 At the synapses where neurons connect, the neurons are constantly giving off and picking up chemicals called neurotransmitters, which in turn can cause those neurons to “fire”—to run cascading ions down the neurons’ “wires” or axons.2 And that process creates the Universe we live in.3 Not quite, literally. I do believe, though I cannot rigorously prove, that you exist, the Earth exists, and the Universe exists outside of our own brains, but our only interaction with that reality is through our brains and the physical events that happen there. Those objective physical events create a subjective and non-physical “thing” we call the mind. If you remember tomorrow that you read this Article (or this much of the Article), it is because this Article (and I) will have made physical changes to your brain. As we get better at looking at those physical changes in the brain through various new technologies, we can begin to correlate those objective physical brain states with subjective mental states.4 We can begin to say Copyright © 2015 by Henry Greely. * Henry Greely is the Deane F. and Kate Edelman Johnson Professor of Law and Professor, by courtesy, of Genetics at Stanford University, where he directs the Center for Law and the Biosciences, and the Stanford Program in Neuroscience and Society. This Article is adapted from the author's Stuart Rome Lecture, delivered at the University of Maryland Carey School of Law on April 24, 2014. 1. Suzana Herculano-Houzel, The Human Brain in Numbers: A Linearly Scaled-Up Primate Brain, FRONTIERS HUMAN NEUROSCIENCE (Nov. 9, 2009), http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/neuro.09.031.2009/full. 2. See HARVEY LODISH ET AL., MOLECULAR CELL BIOLOGY 935 (4th ed. 2000) (explaining the process by which a neurotransmitter, which originates at the presynaptic neuron, sends a signal when it is sent to the postsynaptic target cell). 3. See Robert Lanza, A New Theory of the Universe: Biocentrism Builds on Quantum Physics by Putting Life Into the Equation, AM. SCHOLAR (Mar. 1, 2007), https://theamericanscholar.org/a-new-theory-of-the-universe/#.VRIZjjTF-Hw. 4. See B. ALAN WALLACE, MIND IN THE BALANCE: MEDITATION IN SCIENCE, BUDDHISM, AND CHRISTIANITY 23 (2009) (noting the difference between objectively studying the brain and understanding the subjective mental state that is occurring simultaneously). 171 Greely Final.docx 172 JOURNAL OF HEALTH CARE LAW & POLICY [VOL. 18:171 “any time you move the big toe on your left foot, these neurons fire,” or “every time you see a face, those neurons fire.”5 Consider, for example, some spectacular work by Professor Jack Gallant at the University of California at Berkeley.6 Gallant’s group showed thousands of hours of YouTube videos to some volunteers while they were in a magnetic resonance imaging (“MRI”) scanner.7 The MRI noted the changes at different times in the relative amounts of oxygenated and de- oxygenated hemoglobin in different parts of the volunteers’ brains, in a process called functional magnetic resonance imaging (“fMRI”).8 Computers analyzed the resulting data and found correlations between what the volunteers were seeing at any given time and the patterns of these hemodynamic changes.9 Gallant then took different volunteers, put them in the MRI scanner, and showed them trailers from movies.10 His team took the resulting brain scans and, using the correlations from the original work, “re-created” the scenes from the trailers as the volunteers saw them.11 The results are far from perfect—but still close to amazing. When, in a trailer, an elephant walks across a plain from left to right, the recreation of what the viewer sees from the viewer’s brain scan shows something that looks like an elephant-shaped haystack walking from left to right across a plain.12 The results come from correlating perceived physical states of the brain with 5. See Ferris Jabr, Know Your Neurons: How to Classify Different Types of Neurons in the Brain’s Forest, SCI. AM. (May 16, 2012), http://blogs.scientificamerican.com/brainwaves/2012/05/16/know-your-neurons-classifying-the- many-types-of-cells-in-the-neuron-forest/ (noting that neurons are classified by function because neurons that carry sensory information are not the same neurons that carry signals for motor function in the body). 6. See Yasmin Anwar, Scientists Use Brain Imaging to Reveal the Movies in Our Mind, UC BERKELEY NEWS CENTER (Sept. 22, 2011), http://newscenter.berkeley.edu/2011/09/22/brain- movies (citing the cutting-edge work by Prof. Jack Gallant and his lab, which have successfully reconstructed humans’ visual experiences through computer simulation as the participants watched Hollywood movie trailers); see also Shinji Nishimoto et al., Reconstructing Visual Experiences from Brain Activity Evoked by Natural Movies, GALLANT LAB @ UC BERKELEY http://gallantlab.org/publications/nishimoto-et-al-2011.html (last updated June 18, 2014) (explaining the use of an fMRI machine to measure brain activity during the experiment, and the computational models used to reconstruct what participants saw). 7. Anwar, supra note 6. 8. Id. 9. Id. 10. Id. 11. Id. 12. See Malcolm Ritter, Mind-Reading Technology Reconstructs Videos from Brain, SYDNEY MORNING HERALD (Sept. 23, 2011), http://www.smh.com.au/technology/sci-tech/mindreading- technology-reconstructs-videos-from-brain-20110923-1ko5s.html (noting that human forms were more recognizable in reconstructions, while figures such as elephants did not transition so clearly). 2015] NEUROSCIENCE, MINDREADING, AND THE COURTS 173 subjective mental states.13 It comes from, in some small way, reading minds. This Article is about mindreading and its applications to the law. We are beginning to be able to use neuroimaging and other techniques to read minds.14 Most of the attention in the burgeoning field of law and neuroscience has focused on issues of free will and criminal responsibility, but the most important contribution that neuroscience will make to the law will be through neuroscience-based mindreading. And I suspect its first important use will be in the area of detecting “pain,” on which this Article will focus.15 This Article makes that argument in four parts. First, it looks at what kind of evidence the law currently uses to read minds, and how neuroscience-based evidence would and would not be different.16 Second, it discusses some of the possible ways the law could use neuroscience-based mindreading.17 Third, in its most novel contribution, it analyzes what kind of proof the law should demand of the accuracy of such mindreading techniques—and what we would have to invest in developing these technologies to be confident in their use.18 Finally, it touches on one of the deepest problems that might be raised by the use of accurate mindreading evidence in the law.19 13. Anwar, supra note 6. 14. See Rob Hoskin, Can a Neuroscientist Read Your Mind?, SCIENCE BRAINWAVES (Apr. 30, 2012), http://www.sciencebrainwaves.com/uncategorized/can-a-neuroscientist-read-your- mind/ (highlighting the neural information that occurs even before a decision is made). 15. The implications for the legal system of neuroimaging of evidence of pain is the subject of a small, but growing literature. See Adam J. Kolber, The Experiential Future of the Law, 60 EMORY L.J. 585, 651 (2011) (identifying the need for more objective units to describe certain experiences, like pain); Adam J. Kolber, Pain Detection and the Privacy of Subjective Experience 33 AM. J.L. & MED. 433, 453–54 (2007) (noting the need for more privacy for records regarding pain); Amanda C. Pustilnik, Imaging Brains, Changing Minds: How Neuroimaging Can Transform the Law’s Approach to Pain, 66 ALA. L. REV. (forthcoming 2015); Amanda C. Pustilnik, Pain as Fact and Heuristic: How Pain Neuroimaging Illuminates Moral Dimensions of Law, 97 CORNELL L. REV. 801, 805 (2012) (highlighting that it is a major challenge to assign values to brain imaging, just as it is a challenge to do the same for pain when attempting to create law). 16. See infra Part I. 17. See infra Part II. 18. See infra Part III. 19. See infra Part IV. I have discussed some of these issues about mindreading twice before in some depth. See Henry T. Greely, Neuroscience, Mindreading and the Law, in A PRIMER ON CRIMINAL LAW AND NEUROSCIENCE 120–49 (Stephen J. Morse & Adina L. Roskies eds., 2013); Emily R. Murphy & Henry T. Greely, What Will Be the Limits of Neuroscience-Based Mindreading in the Law?, in THE OXFORD HANDBOOK OF NEUROETHICS (Judy Illes & Barbara Sahakian eds., 2011) (identifying the technical barriers to meaningful mindreading, including the likely impossibility of creating a complete model of the human brain).
Recommended publications
  • Call for Abstracts International Neuropsychological Society New York City | February 20-23, 2019 INS President: Keith Owen Yeates, Phd Program Chair: Michael W
    Call for Abstracts International Neuropsychological Society New York City | February 20-23, 2019 INS President: Keith Owen Yeates, PhD Program Chair: Michael W. Kirkwood, PhD Continuing Education Chair: Melissa Lamar, PhD Submission Opens: May 7, 2018 Submission Closes: August 10, 2018 Message from Michael Kirkwood, 2019 INS Program Committee Chair: Neuropsychology is a melting pot, at the crossroads of neuroscience and behavioral health. To fully understand brain health and illness, we need to consider both neurobiological and psychological processes. We also need to consider developmental factors and the various environmental contexts within which individuals function. Drawing on these ideas, the theme for the 2019 Annual Meeting in New York City is: Embracing the Biopsychosocial Melting Pot. We welcome abstract submissions that represent the interdisciplinary nature of neuropsychology, as well as those that further our understanding of the brain-context relationships that help determine cognitive, emotional, social, and moral development and functioning. As always, we encourage submissions that offer unique insights into cognitive and affective neuroscience of healthy and disease states, innovations in behavioral measurement, and state-of-the-art treatment and rehabilitation approaches. We couldn’t be more excited to be meeting in NYC, its own melting pot of people and cultures. We hope you’ll join us for a stellar scientific program and the virtually endless arts, cultural, and social opportunities that the Big Apple has to offer.
    [Show full text]
  • Detecting Awareness After Severe Brain Injury
    PERSPECTIVES Recently, the results of some functional SCIENCE AND SOCIETY MRI (fMRI) and EEG studies have called into question the extent to which we can Detecting awareness after severe reliably consider a patient unaware simply because they exhibit no overt behavioural brain injury response to external stimulation8–11. Indeed, these studies have revealed a subset of patients who are aware but entirely physi- Davinia Fernández-Espejo and Adrian M. Owen cally unresponsive; thus, although they Abstract | Recent developments in functional neuroimaging have provided a fulfil all internationally agreed criteria for number of new tools for assessing patients who clinically appear to be in a the vegetative state, which are based on vegetative state. These techniques have been able to reveal awareness and even behavioural signs, clear signs of command- following can be demonstrated using fMRI allow rudimentary communication in some patients who remain entirely or EEG. In some cases, these developments behaviourally non-responsive. The implications of these results extend well beyond in functional neuroimaging technology have the immediate clinical and scientific findings to influencing legal proceedings, even allowed such patients to communicate raising new ethical questions about the withdrawal of nutrition and hydration and with the outside world for the first time since 10 providing new options for patients and families in that decision-making process. their brain injury . In this Perspective article, we begin by The findings have also motivated significant public discourse about the role of reviewing the ‘state‑of‑the-art’ of the two neuroscience research in society. methods — fMRI and EEG — that have been successfully used to detect covert aware- The definition and assessment of conscious- The vegetative state is a clinical condi- ness in the vegetative state, with a focus on ness is still one of the most challenging areas tion that is often described as ‘wakefulness the major developments that have emerged of contemporary neuroscience.
    [Show full text]
  • How Neuroscience Technology Is Changing Our Understanding of Brain Injury, Vegetative States and the Law
    NORTH CAROLINA JOURNAL OF LAW & TECHNOLOGY Volume 20 Issue 3 Article 2 3-1-2019 How Neuroscience Technology is Changing Our Understanding of Brain Injury, Vegetative States and the Law Glenn R. Butterton Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarship.law.unc.edu/ncjolt Part of the Law Commons Recommended Citation Glenn R. Butterton, How Neuroscience Technology is Changing Our Understanding of Brain Injury, Vegetative States and the Law, 20 N.C. J.L. & TECH. 331 (2019). Available at: https://scholarship.law.unc.edu/ncjolt/vol20/iss3/2 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by Carolina Law Scholarship Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in North Carolina Journal of Law & Technology by an authorized editor of Carolina Law Scholarship Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. NORTH CAROLINA JOURNAL OF LAW & TECHNOLOGY VOLUME 20, ISSUE 3: MARCH 2019 HOW NEUROSCIENCE TECHNOLOGY IS CHANGING OUR UNDERSTANDING OF BRAIN INJURY, VEGETATIVE STATES AND THE LAW Glenn R. Butterton* The author examines clinical studies that use neuroscience technology to study patients in Vegetative States. The studies indicate that some of the patients are, in fact, conscious. The author suggests that this finding is a matter of considerable practical importance for the drafting and execution of end-of-life protocols such as Advance Directives and Living Wills. He recommends that statutes, and other guidance used by patients, caregivers, medical institutions, family members and others to draft and interpret such Directives and Wills, be revised or amended to take account of these results. I. INTRODUCTION ........................................................................332 II.
    [Show full text]
  • Anesthesia and Neuroimaging: Investigating the Neural Correlates
    Review Anesthesia and neuroimaging: investigating the neural correlates of unconsciousness 1 1 2 2 Alex A. MacDonald , Lorina Naci , Penny A. MacDonald , and Adrian M. Owen 1 Undergraduate Medical Program, Faculty of Medicine, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada 2 The Brain and Mind Institute, The Natural Sciences Centre, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada In the past 15 years, rapid technological development in agents both complement and extend results from tradi- the field of neuroimaging has led to a resurgence of tional psychophysical studies. interest in the study of consciousness. However, the In the following we summarize findings from functional neural bases of consciousness and the boundaries of neuroimaging studies that have used anesthetic agents to unconscious processing remain poorly understood. An- study changes in brain activation at reduced levels of esthesia combined with functional neuroimaging pre- awareness, both with sensory stimulation and at rest. sents a unique approach for studying neural responses We then relate these results to those obtained with brain as a function of consciousness. In this review we sum- imaging (i) using traditional psychophysical manipula- marize findings from functional neuroimaging studies tions of awareness, and (ii) in behaviorally non-responsive that have used anesthetic drugs to study cognition at patients. In the latter case, we discuss how studies that different levels of conscious awareness. We relate the have used anesthetic drugs to modulate the level of aware- results to those of psychophysical studies of cognition ness might inform diagnoses in disorders of consciousness. and explore their potential usefulness in interpreting We focus on cognitive systems that have been studied clinical findings from studies of non-responsive patients.
    [Show full text]
  • Canadian Perspectives on the Clinical Actionability of Neuroimaging in Disorders of Consciousness
    ORIGINAL ARTICLE COPYRIGHT © 2015 THE CANADIAN JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGICAL SCIENCES INC. Canadian Perspectives on the Clinical Actionability of Neuroimaging in Disorders of Consciousness Grace Lee, Adrian C. Byram, Adrian M. Owen, Urs Ribary, A. Jon Stoessl, Andrea Townson, Christine Stables, Judy Illes ABSTRACT: Background: Acquired brain injury is a critical public health and socioeconomic problem in Canada, leaving many patients in vegetative, minimally conscious, or locked-in states, unresponsive and unable to communicate. Recent advances in neuroimaging research have demonstrated residual consciousness in a few exemplary patients with acquired brain injury, suggesting potential misdiagnosis and changes in prognosis. Such progress, in parallel with research using multimodal brain imaging technologies in recent years, has promising implications for clinical translation, notwithstanding the many challenges that impact health care and policy development. This study explored the perspectives of Canadian professionals with expertise either in neuroimaging research, disorders of consciousness, or both, on the potential clinical applications and implications of imaging technology. Methods: Twenty-two professionals from designated communities of neuroimaging researchers, ethicists, lawyers, and practitioners participated in semistructured interviews. Data were analyzed for emergent themes. Results: The five most dominant themes were: (1) validation and calibration of the methods; (2) informed consent; (3) burdens on the health care system; (4) implications for the Canadian health care system; and (5) possibilities for improved prognosis. Conclusions: Movement of neuroimaging from research into clinical care for acquired brain injury will require careful consideration of legal and ethical issues alongside research reliability, responsible distribution of health care resources, and the interaction of technological capabilities with patient outcome.
    [Show full text]
  • Anesthesia and Neuroimaging: Investigating the Neural
    Review Anesthesia and neuroimaging: investigating the neural correlates of unconsciousness 1 1 2 2 Alex A. MacDonald , Lorina Naci , Penny A. MacDonald , and Adrian M. Owen 1 Undergraduate Medical Program, Faculty of Medicine, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada 2 The Brain and Mind Institute, The Natural Sciences Centre, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada In the past 15 years, rapid technological development in agents both complement and extend results from tradi- the field of neuroimaging has led to a resurgence of tional psychophysical studies. interest in the study of consciousness. However, the In the following we summarize findings from functional neural bases of consciousness and the boundaries of neuroimaging studies that have used anesthetic agents to unconscious processing remain poorly understood. An- study changes in brain activation at reduced levels of esthesia combined with functional neuroimaging pre- awareness, both with sensory stimulation and at rest. sents a unique approach for studying neural responses We then relate these results to those obtained with brain as a function of consciousness. In this review we sum- imaging (i) using traditional psychophysical manipula- marize findings from functional neuroimaging studies tions of awareness, and (ii) in behaviorally non-responsive that have used anesthetic drugs to study cognition at patients. In the latter case, we discuss how studies that different levels of conscious awareness. We relate the have used anesthetic drugs to modulate the level of aware- results to those of psychophysical studies of cognition ness might inform diagnoses in disorders of consciousness. and explore their potential usefulness in interpreting We focus on cognitive systems that have been studied clinical findings from studies of non-responsive patients.
    [Show full text]
  • Exploring the Mind What Our Brain Reveals About Our Thoughts
    11th Berlin Colloquium of the Gottlieb Daimler and Karl Benz Foundation in cooperation with the Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience Berlin and the Charité University Medicine Berlin on May 9th 2007 at the Academy of the Konrad Adenauer Foundation, Berlin Exploring the Mind What Our Brain Reveals about Our Thoughts Scientific Coordinators: Professor John-Dylan Haynes and Professor Gabriel Curio Statements of the Speakers Gisbert zu Putlitz Reading thoughts – Accessing our innermost lives 2 Cornelius Borck Making brains write: Electrical strategies in the 4 history of mind-reading Gabriel Curio Non-invasive brain-computer interfaces – 5 On thoughts that move Miguel A. L. Nicolelis Computing with neural ensembles 7 Rainer Goebel Real-time mind-reading and BOLD 8 brain interactions John-Dylan Haynes Decoding conscious and unconscious thoughts 10 from fMRI signals Daniel D. Langleben The study of deception: Functional MRI and lie 12 detection applications Adrian Owen Detecting residual cognitive function 14 in the vegetative state Henrik Walter From brain to bedside? Implications of brain 16 reading for psychiatry Thomas Metzinger Philosophical implications of brain-reading: 18 Ethical and anthropological consequences Christof Koch Decoding the contents of perception from single 20 neurons in the human brain Editing: Kristina Vaillant and Marlene Schoofs, Berlin 11th Berlin Colloquium “Exploring the Mind” 2 Professor Gisbert zu Putlitz President of the Board, Gottlieb Daimler and Karl Benz Foundation, Ladenburg Reading thoughts – Accessing our innermost lives Dr. Gisbert zu Putlitz is a physicist at the University of Heidelberg, specializing in elementary particle physics, nuclear physics and quantum fluids. When the Gottlieb Daimler and Karl Benz Foundation was founded in 1986, he was appointed Executive Director of the Board.
    [Show full text]
  • Working Memory Task Performance and Chunking in Early Alzheimer’S Disease Jonathan Huntley, Daniel Bor, Adam Hampshire, Adrian Owen and Robert Howard
    The British Journal of Psychiatry (2011) 198, 398–403. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.083857 Working memory task performance and chunking in early Alzheimer’s disease Jonathan Huntley, Daniel Bor, Adam Hampshire, Adrian Owen and Robert Howard Background Chunking is a powerful encoding strategy that significantly significantly better on structured trials of the digit working improves working memory performance in normal young memory tasks, indicating successful use of chunking people. strategies to improve verbal working memory performance. The control and very mild disease groups also performed Aims significantly better on structured trials of the spatial task, To investigate chunking in patients with mild Alzheimer’s whereas those with mild disease demonstrated no significant disease and in a control group of elderly people without difference between the structured and unstructured spatial cognitive impairment. conditions. Method Conclusions People with mild Alzheimer’s disease (n = 28) were recruited The ability to use chunking as an encoding strategy to and divided according to Mini-Mental State Examination improve verbal working memory performance is preserved at score into mild and very mild disease groups. A control the mild stage of Alzheimer’s disease, whereas use of group of 15 elderly individuals was also recruited. All chunking to improve spatial working memory is impaired by participants performed digit and spatial working memory this stage. Simple training in the use of chunking might be a tasks requiring either unstructured sequences or structured beneficial therapeutic strategy to prolong working memory sequences (which encourage chunking of information) to be functioning in patients at the earliest stage of Alzheimer’s recalled.
    [Show full text]
  • Download Thesis
    This electronic thesis or dissertation has been downloaded from the King’s Research Portal at https://kclpure.kcl.ac.uk/portal/ The functional anatomy of working memory training using chunking in Alzheimer's disease Huntley, Jonathan David Awarding institution: King's College London The copyright of this thesis rests with the author and no quotation from it or information derived from it may be published without proper acknowledgement. END USER LICENCE AGREEMENT Unless another licence is stated on the immediately following page this work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International licence. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ You are free to copy, distribute and transmit the work Under the following conditions: Attribution: You must attribute the work in the manner specified by the author (but not in any way that suggests that they endorse you or your use of the work). Non Commercial: You may not use this work for commercial purposes. No Derivative Works - You may not alter, transform, or build upon this work. Any of these conditions can be waived if you receive permission from the author. Your fair dealings and other rights are in no way affected by the above. Take down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact [email protected] providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Download date: 04. Oct. 2021 THE FUNCTIONAL ANATOMY OF WORKING MEMORY TRAINING USING CHUNKING IN ALZHEIMER'S DISEASE Jonathan Huntley Thesis submitted for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy King’s College London University of London June 2014 1 ABSTRACT AIMS A randomised controlled trial of a novel cognitive training regime based on chunking was conducted in participants with early Alzheimer’s Disease.
    [Show full text]
  • Putting Brain Training to the Test
    Vol 465 | 10 June 2010 | doi:10.1038/nature09042 LETTERS Putting brain training to the test Adrian M. Owen1, Adam Hampshire1, Jessica A. Grahn1, Robert Stenton2, Said Dajani2, Alistair S. Burns3, Robert J. Howard2 & Clive G. Ballard2 ‘Brain training’, or the goal of improved cognitive function broader range of cognitive functions was trained using tests of short- through the regular use of computerized tests, is a multimillion- term memory, attention, visuospatial processing and mathematics pound industry1, yet in our view scientific evidence to support its similar to those commonly found in commercially available brain- efficacy is lacking. Modest effects have been reported in some training devices. The difficulty of the training tasks increased as the studies of older individuals2,3 and preschool children4, and participants improved to continuously challenge their cognitive per- video-game players outperform non-players on some tests of formance and maximize any benefits of training. The control group visual attention5. However, the widely held belief that commer- did not formally practise any specific cognitive tasks during their cially available computerized brain-training programs improve ‘training’ sessions, but answered obscure questions from six different general cognitive function in the wider population in our opinion categories using any available online resource. At six weeks, the bench- lacks empirical support. The central question is not whether per- marking assessment was repeated and the pre- and post-training scores formance on cognitive tests can be improved by training, but were compared. The difference in benchmarking scores provided the rather, whether those benefits transfer to other untrained tasks measure of generalized cognitive improvement resulting from train- or lead to any general improvement in the level of cognitive func- ing.
    [Show full text]
  • Working with the Science Media Centre
    With thanks to our sponsors Contents Editor’s foreword Geoff Watts, science writer and broadcaster The following organisations have contributed to the costs of the Science Media Centre’s 10th anniversary: aybe I’m entirely the wrong person to have publication of their own research through to distant BIVDA, BP plc, British Pharmacological Society, EUK Consulting, GlaxoSmithKline, Imperial College London, Editor’s foreword 1 collated these reflections on the Science Media events of which they have the specialised knowledge or Maudsley Charity, MSD, National Grid, Society for Applied Microbiology, Society for General Microbiology, MCentre (SMC). A decade ago, when I first heard understanding required to offer authoritative comment. Springer Science+Business Media, Wellcome Trust Chief Executive’s introduction 2 of the proposal to set up another body to help science Many of the issues tackled by the SMC are important not journalists do their job properly (which was how I then only for the science involved, but because that science Climate of crisis 4 perceived it), I really couldn’t see the point. Didn’t we all has an impact on society. This is clearly so of the topics GM on trial 6 have our own contact lists? Hadn’t every university and chosen for this booklet. every research institute got a press office eager to alert GM on trial again 8 us to their latest findings? Didn’t science and medical As many of the authors of the following accounts are journalists already get enough press information from keen to emphasise, even those among them who were Media meltdown 10 enough organisations without another body clamouring initially apprehensive about meeting the media have found the experience unthreatening and even enjoyable.
    [Show full text]