Qt8pg2k44f.Pdf
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
UC Irvine Structure and Dynamics Title CONCEPTUALIZING ‘FRONT’ AND ‘BACK’: FRAMES OF REFERENCE AND TAUMAKO REPRESENTATIONS OF SPACE Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/8pg2k44f Journal Structure and Dynamics, 9(1) Author Feinberg, Richard Publication Date 2016 DOI 10.5070/SD991031890 License https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/ 4.0 Peer reviewed eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California CONCEPTUALIZING ‘FRONT’ AND ‘BACK’ FRAMES OF REFERENCE AND TAUMAKO REPRESENTATIONS OF SPACE Richard Feinberg Department of Anthropology Kent State University [email protected] The study of navigation involves questions about the conceptualization of space and ways in which people share their spatial understandings with others. This article focus- es on one aspect of spatial cognition, a phenomenon commonly known as “frames of reference” (FoRs). It explores the myriad ways in which Taumako islanders in the southeastern Solomons talk about spatial relations that English speakers term ‘front’ and ‘back.’ I examine how Taumako notions of ‘front’ and ‘back’ articulate with FoRs that are well established in the anthropological literature, and I explore the challenge of applying commonly-accepted FoR typologies to actual Taumako usage. In some con- texts, there was little disagreement among my interlocutors as to proper use of the salient terms. In others, there was considerable divergence; and in certain instances even the same person appeared to be inconsistent from one occasion to the next. I will attempt to identify those areas in which I found widespread consensus as well as those in which disagreements were pervasive, and I will consider possible reasons for that difference. Key words: Polynesia, spatial cognition, frames of reference Introduction The present article, like others in this special issue, emerges from an interest in Pacific Island way-finding. In 2007-08, I spent nine months working with people from the Vaeakau-Taumako region of the southeastern Solomon Islands, studying canoe construc- tion, navigational techniques, and voyaging. Inter-island navigation, my initial focus, is an art that requires specialized skills shared only by a small proportion of the population (cf. Genz, this issue). Way-finding, however, is equally pertinent to ordinary people going about their daily business, whether walking to their gardens, visiting relatives, or locating productive fishing grounds (cf. Schneider and Van der Ryn, both this issue). In the end, such mundane activities loomed every bit as large in my investigation as did long dis- tance voyaging. The study of navigation involves questions about the conceptualization of space and ways in which people share their spatial understandings with others. This article fo- cuses on one aspect of spatial cognition, a phenomenon known in cognitive and linguistic anthropology as “frames of reference” (FoRs).1 More specifically, it explores the myriad !94 ways in which Taumako islanders speak of spatial relations termed ‘front’ and ‘back’ in English.2 It examines how Taumako notions of ‘front’ and ‘back’ articulate with FoRs that have been widely recognized (e.g., Báez 2011; O’Meara and Báez 2011; Bennardo 2002, 2009; Feinberg 2014; Levinson 1996; Palmer 2002). And it explores the challenge of applying commonly-accepted FoR typologies to actual Taumako usage. Like the Ton- gans studied by Bennardo, people on Taumako rely on at least two subtypes of the abso- lute FoR, described in the following section. Unlike Bennardo’s Tongans, however, in- trinsic and relative FoRs are also central. Moreover, it was sometimes difficult to ascer- tain which FoR was being invoked. In some contexts, there was little disagreement among my interlocutors as to the proper use of ‘front’ and ‘back.’ In others, there was considerable divergence; and in certain instances even the same person appeared to be inconsistent from one occasion to the next. I will attempt to identify those areas in which I found widespread consensus as well as those in which disagreements were pervasive, and I will consider possible reasons for that difference. Frames oF Reference: Types and Subtypes When conceptualizing an object’s location, one may pinpoint its position in relation to the speaker/cognizer (ego), a second object, or some fixed external axis. These types of FoR have been dubbed “relative,” “intrinsic,” and “absolute,” respectively. Following Palmer (2002:109-110), an intrinsic FoR is dyadic, as illustrated by the statement, “John is in front of the car,” where John is located in relation to an object that has a clearly-defined front and back. A relative FoR is triadic, as exemplified by the statement, “John is in front of the post.” A post does not have a front and back; rather, the statement means that John is between the post and the speaker. Absolute FoRs resemble the intrinsic type in that both are dyadic, but an absolute FoR locates objects in relation to “pre-established arbi- trary fixed bearings.” This would be illustrated by the statement, “John is north of the house,” indicating that he is located on an axis running between the North and South Poles, and that he is positioned along that axis between the house and the North Pole. Bennardo (2002; 2009) describes these concepts in somewhat different terms but in a manner that is generally consistent with Palmer. For Bennardo (2002:161), a relative frame of reference is “a system of coordinates centred on the speaker/viewer/cogniser”; an intrinsic frame involves coordinates centered on an object other than the speaker; and an absolute FoR is centered neither on the object nor the speaker. Scholars have divided relative and absolute FoRs into a number of subtypes. Sub- types of the relative FoR that appear in Bennardo’s discussion (2009) include “basic,” “translation,” and “reflection.” Subtypes of the absolute FoR include what he calls “car- dinal point,” “single-axis,” “radial,” and “ad hoc/landmark.” The basic subtype of the relative FoR involves three axes—vertical (up/down), transverse (left/right), and sagittal (front/back)—which intersect at the speaker, and ob- jects are located with reference to these three axes. If the speaker moves, the axes also move, and the point of origin remains the speaker. The translation subtype of the relative FoR involves a second object, the “ground,” conceived as facing in the same direction as the speaker. The ground becomes the intersection point of the three axes. Thus, an object !95 on the opposite side of the ground from ego is described as being “in front” of that object. In the reflection subtype, the ground is conceived as facing toward the speaker, and an object located between ego and the ground is described as “in front of” it. The cardinal point subtype of the absolute FoR is typified by the Western division of geographical space in terms of points labeled (in English), north, east, south, and west. Other languages may use different cardinal points, and the numbers of such points may vary. In Vaeakau-Taumako, the ‘wind compass’ (Figure 1; cf. Pyrek and Feinberg, this issue) is conceived as a system of eight cardinal points. The single-axis subtype of the absolute FoR creates an axis between two points of special significance. A common version of this subtype in Oceania involves an imaginary line that runs from the center of an island seaward toward the horizon. Others may in- volve an axis characterized as running upward and downward (which may or may not be a matter of literal altitude), or between two culturally-salient landmarks, such as a town and a village. The radial subtype of the absolute FoR involves a fixed point of reference, and movement is either centripetal (toward) or centrifugal (away) with respect to that central point (Bennardo 2009:61). N Hokohua loa te angeho Tokelau Hakahiu Palapu W Tokelau Tū E Alunga Laki Tonga Ulu Hokohua loa te ngatae S Figure 1. Vaeakau-Taumako ‘wind compass’ and dominant swells as represented by Clement Teniau, November 2007; courtesy of Ghassan Rafeedie. Several authors (e.g., Bennardo 2002, 2009; Levinson 1996; Palmer 2002) sug- gest that Austronesian speakers privilege absolute frames of reference.3 Bennardo adds that in Tonga the predominant FoR is “the radial subtype of the absolute FoR.” Elsewhere (Feinberg 2014), I have described in some detail the conceptualization of space on Tau- mako and the variety of models that the islanders employ. Like Bennardo’s Tongans— and, perhaps, most Polynesians—the Taumako rely heavily on several subtypes of the !96 Figure 2. Map showing Santa Cruz Group in relation to other islands of the central and western Pacific. (Adapted from Yen and Gordon 1973:3.) absolute FoR, but relative and intrinsic FoRs are also important—and sometimes deci- sive. Taumako: The Island and Its People Taumako, also known as the Duff Islands, is a Polynesian outlier in the Solomon Islands’ Temotu Province (see Figures 2 and 3). It has gained prominence among students of Pa- cific navigation thanks to the writings of Davenport (1962, 1964, 1968), Koch (1971), Lewis (1972), and the Vaka Taumako Project (n.d.). It was once part of an elaborate trade network that Davenport (1962) described in his well-known article on “red feather mon- ey.” Until a few decades ago, the system involved production of elaborate voyaging ca- noes (known as puke and alo lili) on Taumako, a high island with dense vegetation and large Callophyllum trees. The Taumako then sold the canoes to voyagers from the Poly- nesian Outer Reef Islands (known locally as Vaeakau), who would sail to the main Reefs and the large Melanesian islands of Vanikoro, Utupua, and Ndeni. They would take pigs, and often women, to exchange for nuts and feather money. Those were then used to pay Taumako craftsmen for additional canoes. The Taumako, in turn, used the feather money as a central element in bridewealth payments.