Ape Gestures and Language Evolution

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Ape Gestures and Language Evolution Ape gestures and language evolution Amy S. Pollick and Frans B. M. de Waal* Living Links, Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Emory University, 954 North Gatewood Road, Atlanta, GA 30329 Contributed by Frans B. M. de Waal, March 20, 2007 (sent for review January 28, 2007) The natural communication of apes may hold clues about language This difference in control was dramatically illustrated by the origins, especially because apes frequently gesture with limbs and failure of efforts to teach chimpanzees vocal modifications to hands, a mode of communication thought to have been the ‘‘speak’’ (21, 22), whereas this species had no trouble learning to starting point of human language evolution. The present study use the gestures of American sign language in a referential aimed to contrast brachiomanual gestures with orofacial move- manner (23). Greater control over natural gestures than facial ments and vocalizations in the natural communication of our expressions is also suggested by observations of (i) deception, in closest primate relatives, bonobos (Pan paniscus) and chimpanzees which apes may use a hand to modify a facial expression (24, 25), (Pan troglodytes). We tested whether gesture is the more flexible and (ii) cultural transmission, with gestures being far more form of communication by measuring the strength of association population-specific in both humans and apes than facial expres- between signals and specific behavioral contexts, comparing sions, which tend to be relatively invariant (8, 26–28). groups of both the same and different ape species. Subjects were All of these observations are in line with the gestural hypoth- two captive bonobo groups, a total of 13 individuals, and two esis of human language origins, which is further supported by captive chimpanzee groups, a total of 34 individuals. The study differential growth of the brain and vocal apparatuses, as seen in distinguished 31 manual gestures and 18 facial/vocal signals. It was paleoarchaeological remains (29), the appearance of gestural found that homologous facial/vocal displays were used very sim- communication in human infants before speech (30), and the ilarly by both ape species, yet the same did not apply to gestures. right-hand (hence left-brain) bias of both ape and human Both within and between species gesture usage varied enor- gestures (31–33). The ape homologue of Broca’s area (i.e., mously. Moreover, bonobos showed greater flexibility in this Brodmann’s area 44) is enlarged in the left hemisphere (34). In monkeys, this area is activated during both the production and regard than chimpanzees and were also the only species in which perception of gestures but not vocalizations (35). It has been multimodal communication (i.e., combinations of gestures and speculated, therefore, that the neural structures underlying facial/vocal signals) added to behavioral impact on the recipient. manual movements in the great apes, perhaps also including tool ͉ ͉ ͉ use (36), are homologous with the lateralized language areas in bonobo chimpanzee communication multimodal the human brain (37, 38). Gesture remains very much alive in human communication. he longstanding debate about the evolution of language Recent research demonstrates the universal importance of ges- Ttypically compares human speech with animal vocalizations ture to human cognitive functioning, such as enhanced infor- (1). The vocal modality offers obvious parallels, yet it has been mation transfer (39), lexical retrieval (40), and even the provi- proposed that our ancestors’ first linguistic utterances were not sion of a supplemental cognitive arena for thought (41). Gesture in the vocal but in the gestural domain (2–5). This proposal is all production in humans is so automatic that it is relatively immune the more intriguing because gestural communication is virtually to audience effects: blind subjects gesture at equal rates as limited to the Hominoidea (i.e., humans and apes). sighted subjects to a known blind audience (42). Gestures are narrowly defined here as communication by Gestures are rarely produced in the absence of other com- means of hands, feet, or limbs (Table 1). One reason to set municative signals, such as facial expressions and vocalizations. gestures apart from other bodily communication is that the two Multimodal communication has been appreciated in humans for are neurologically distinct in both production and perception by several decades (43) and is becoming increasingly important in others (6, 7). Whereas all primates regularly communicate by the study of animal communication (44). Multimodal signaling means of vocalizations, orofacial movements, body postures, and occurs across taxa, from snapping shrimps to spiders and birds, locomotion patterns, free brachiomanual gestures (i.e., manual and in all contexts, although those related to courtship and communication without touching another individual or a sub- mating are best documented (A.S.P., unpublished work). This strate) are typical of humans and apes (8). Gestures were first communication strategy can have a variety of functions, includ- described for chimpanzees (9, 10), followed by other anthropoid ing amplification and modulation of signal meaning. Combined apes: bonobos (11, 12), gorillas (Gorilla gorilla; refs. 13 and 14), with the graded facial/vocal signals typical of the apes (46), and orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus; ref. 15). gestural flexibility has the advantage over the more stereotyped The discontinuity between the Hominoidea and all other signaling by monkeys that it permits greater communicative primates regarding gestural modality suggests a relatively recent complexity. shift toward a more flexible and intentional communicative Despite prior work on ape gesture, no research has investi- gated how these signals combine with other forms of natural strategy in our prehominid ancestors (8). One mark of this shift communication and how variations in usage affect behavior. is contextually defined usage; that is, a single gesture may Here, we set out to test how the natural gestural modality communicate entirely different needs or intentions depending combines with and differs from the facial/vocal modality in the on the social context in which it is used. For example, a chimpanzee stretching out an open hand toward a third party during a fight signals a need for support, whereas the same Author contributions: A.S.P. and F.B.M.d.W. designed research, performed research, ana- gesture toward a possessor of food signals a desire for a share lyzed data, and wrote the paper. (refs. 16 and 17; Fig. 1). This is not to say that other forms of The authors declare no conflict of interest. communication cannot derive extra meaning from the context in *To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: [email protected]. which they occur (18), but gestures seem less closely tied to This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/cgi/content/full/ specific emotions, hence they permit greater cortical control 0702624104/DC1. than other forms of communication (19, 20). © 2007 by The National Academy of Sciences of the USA 8184–8189 ͉ PNAS ͉ May 8, 2007 ͉ vol. 104 ͉ no. 19 www.pnas.org͞cgi͞doi͞10.1073͞pnas.0702624104 Downloaded by guest on September 26, 2021 Table 1. Operational definition of gesture (1) Gesture refers to a nonlocomotory movement with communicative value of the forearm, hand, wrist, or fingers (cf. ref. 14). Leg and foot movements are included, because these movements have been observed to be communicative in bonobos (11). Excluded from this definition are any other movements that are sometimes labeled gestures in the literature (e.g., ref. 53), such as body postures, head movements, and locomotion patterns. (2) The movement must be directed at another individual. Directed means that the performer of the gesture must potentially be in the recipient’s view. This applies only to nontactile gestures; for a tactile gesture the recipient simply needs to be within touching proximity. (3) A tactile action is considered a gesture only if it lasts2sand visibly lacks the mechanical force to bring about the reaction shown by the recipient, and also does not include any attempt to grab or extensively hold a body part of the other. For example, the gesture hard touch applied to the leg neither moves the other’s leg in any observable direction nor does it visibly, forcibly change the other’s physical position (if a positional change does result, it must appear ЉvoluntaryЉ). (4) Play movements are repeated and exaggerated, usually leading to tickling, play hitting, or wrestling. Movements during play are not considered gestures in this study, except for those that initiate play. These gestures must occur at least 0.5 s before any play is observed between two individuals. communication of the two ape species genetically closest to humans: bonobos and chimpanzees. Given the above back- ground, we formulated a gestural flexibility hypothesis according to which the usage of gestures will be more flexible than that of facial/vocal displays, both within and between species. In testing this hypothesis, we will use context as a proxy for usage, meaning, PSYCHOLOGY and function; that is, we predict that gestures will be less context-bound than other types of signals. The Pan line, which includes both chimpanzees and bonobos, split off from the line that produced our species Ϸ6 million years ago (47), whereas the two ape species themselves split apart only Fig. 1. Meaning often needs to be extracted from the specific context in Ϸ2.5 million years ago (48). Chimpanzees and bonobos are, which a gesture is being used. (A) A juvenile chimpanzee tries to reclaim food therefore, genetically equidistant to us. Studying similar types of that a dominant has taken away by combining the reach out up begging gesture with a scream vocalization. (B) An adolescent bonobo male making communicative signals in closely related species allows one to sexual advances to a female adds the arm raise gesture. Photographs by Frans determine homologies, i.e., shared evolutionary ancestry (49).
Recommended publications
  • EAZA Best Practice Guidelines Bonobo (Pan Paniscus)
    EAZA Best Practice Guidelines Bonobo (Pan paniscus) Editors: Dr Jeroen Stevens Contact information: Royal Zoological Society of Antwerp – K. Astridplein 26 – B 2018 Antwerp, Belgium Email: [email protected] Name of TAG: Great Ape TAG TAG Chair: Dr. María Teresa Abelló Poveda – Barcelona Zoo [email protected] Edition: First edition - 2020 1 2 EAZA Best Practice Guidelines disclaimer Copyright (February 2020) by EAZA Executive Office, Amsterdam. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced in hard copy, machine-readable or other forms without advance written permission from the European Association of Zoos and Aquaria (EAZA). Members of the European Association of Zoos and Aquaria (EAZA) may copy this information for their own use as needed. The information contained in these EAZA Best Practice Guidelines has been obtained from numerous sources believed to be reliable. EAZA and the EAZA APE TAG make a diligent effort to provide a complete and accurate representation of the data in its reports, publications, and services. However, EAZA does not guarantee the accuracy, adequacy, or completeness of any information. EAZA disclaims all liability for errors or omissions that may exist and shall not be liable for any incidental, consequential, or other damages (whether resulting from negligence or otherwise) including, without limitation, exemplary damages or lost profits arising out of or in connection with the use of this publication. Because the technical information provided in the EAZA Best Practice Guidelines can easily be misread or misinterpreted unless properly analysed, EAZA strongly recommends that users of this information consult with the editors in all matters related to data analysis and interpretation.
    [Show full text]
  • Recent Developments in the Study of Wild Chimpanzee Behavior
    Evolutionary Anthropology 9 ARTICLES Recent Developments in the Study of Wild Chimpanzee Behavior JOHN C. MITANI, DAVID P. WATTS, AND MARTIN N. MULLER Chimpanzees have always been of special interest to anthropologists. As our organization, genetics and behavior, closest living relatives,1–3 they provide the standard against which to assess hunting and meat-eating, inter-group human uniqueness and information regarding the changes that must have oc- relationships, and behavioral endocri- curred during the course of human evolution. Given these circumstances, it is not nology. Our treatment is selective, and surprising that chimpanzees have been studied intensively in the wild. Jane Good- we explicitly avoid comment on inter- all4,5 initiated the first long-term field study of chimpanzee behavior at the Gombe population variation in behavior as it National Park, Tanzania. Her observations of tool manufacture and use, hunting, relates to the question of chimpanzee and meat-eating forever changed the way we define humans. Field research on cultures. Excellent reviews of this chimpanzee behavior by Toshisada Nishida and colleagues6 at the nearby Mahale topic, of central concern to anthropol- Mountains National Park has had an equally significant impact. It was Nishida7,8 ogists, can be found elsewhere.12–14 who first provided a comprehensive picture of the chimpanzee social system, including group structure and dispersal. SOCIAL ORGANIZATION No single issue in the study of wild Two generations of researchers erything about the behavior of these chimpanzee behavior has seen more have followed Goodall and Nishida apes in nature. But this is not the case. debate than the nature of their social into the field.
    [Show full text]
  • Conflict and Cooperation in Wild Chimpanzees
    ADVANCES IN THE STUDY OF BEHAVIOR VOL. 35 Conflict and Cooperation in Wild Chimpanzees MARTIN N. MULLER* and JOHN C. MITANIt *DEPARTMENT OF ANTHROPOLOGY BOSTON UNIVERSITY BOSTON, MASSACHUSETTS, 02215, USA tDEPARTMENT OF ANTHROPOLOGY UNIVERSITY OF MICHIGAN ANN ARBOR, MICHIGAN, 48109, USA 1. INTRODUCTION The twin themes of competition and cooperation have been the focus of many studies in animal behavior (Alcock, 2001; Dugatkin, 2004; Krebs and Davies, 1997). Competition receives prominent attention because it forms the basis for the unifying, organizing principle of biology. Darwin's (1859) theory of natural selection furnishes a powerful framework to understand the origin and maintenance of organic and behavioral diversity. Because the process of natural selection depends on reproductive competition, aggression, dominance, and competition for mates serve as important foci of ethological research. In contrast, cooperation in animals is less easily explained within a Darwinian framework. Why do animals cooperate and behave in ways that benefit others? Supplements to the theory of natural selection in the form of kin selection, reciprocal altruism, and mutualism provide mechanisms that transform the study of cooperative behavior in animals into a mode of inquiry compatible with our current understand- ing of the evolutionary process (Clutton-Brock, 2002; Hamilton, 1964; Trivers, 1971). If cooperation can be analyzed via natural selection operating on indivi- duals, a new way to conceptualize the process emerges. Instead of viewing cooperation as distinct from competition, it becomes productive to regard them together. Students of animal behavior have long recognized that an artificial dichotomy may exist insofar as animals frequently cooperate to compete with conspecifics.
    [Show full text]
  • The Mini Lesson
    BreakingNewsEnglish - The Mini Lesson U.S. police to look into True / False shooting of zoo gorilla a) The gorilla that was shot was 7 years old. T / F 2nd June, 2016 b) The little boy jumped into the gorilla's Police in the US enclosure. T / F state of Ohio are c) The water the gorilla dragged the boy through going to look into was not deep. T / F the shooting of a 17-year gorilla at d) The gorilla was upset by the noises of the Cincinnati Zoo. crowd. T / F They will investigate e) Zookeepers said a tranquilizer gun would the facts around the never sedate a big gorilla. T / F killing of Harambe the gorilla after a f) An online petition blaming the mother was four-year-old boy signed by 300,000 people. T / F fell into the ape's g) The mother said some parents can watch their enclosure on Saturday. Video footage showed the children every second. T / F gorilla dragging the boy through shallow water in his cage for up to ten minutes. Some people who h) The mother said society is quick to judge were at the zoo said they were worried about the parents. T / F little boy's safety. Other people said the gorilla was trying to protect the child and that the animal was Synonym Match becoming upset by the noise from the crowds of people looking at the event. Zookeepers made a 1. look into a. pulling decision to shoot the gorilla because they feared for 2. enclosure b.
    [Show full text]
  • Animal Representations, Anthropomorphism, and Några Tillfällen – Kommer Frågan Om Subjektivi- Interspecies Relations in the Little Golden Books
    Samlaren Tidskrift för forskning om svensk och annan nordisk litteratur Årgång 139 2018 I distribution: Eddy.se Svenska Litteratursällskapet REDAKTIONSKOMMITTÉ: Berkeley: Linda Rugg Göteborg: Lisbeth Larsson Köpenhamn: Johnny Kondrup Lund: Erik Hedling, Eva Hættner Aurelius München: Annegret Heitmann Oslo: Elisabeth Oxfeldt Stockholm: Anders Cullhed, Anders Olsson, Boel Westin Tartu: Daniel Sävborg Uppsala: Torsten Pettersson, Johan Svedjedal Zürich: Klaus Müller-Wille Åbo: Claes Ahlund Redaktörer: Jon Viklund (uppsatser) och Sigrid Schottenius Cullhed (recensioner) Biträdande redaktör: Niclas Johansson och Camilla Wallin Bergström Inlagans typografi: Anders Svedin Utgiven med stöd av Vetenskapsrådet Bidrag till Samlaren insändes digitalt i ordbehandlingsprogrammet Word till [email protected]. Konsultera skribentinstruktionerna på sällskapets hemsida innan du skickar in. Sista inläm- ningsdatum för uppsatser till nästa årgång av Samlaren är 15 juni 2019 och för recensioner 1 sep- tember 2019. Samlaren publiceras även digitalt, varför den som sänder in material till Samlaren därmed anses medge digital publicering. Den digitala utgåvan nås på: http://www.svelitt.se/ samlaren/index.html. Sällskapet avser att kontinuerligt tillgängliggöra även äldre årgångar av tidskriften. Svenska Litteratursällskapet tackar de personer som under det senaste året ställt sig till för- fogande som bedömare av inkomna manuskript. Svenska Litteratursällskapet PG: 5367–8. Svenska Litteratursällskapets hemsida kan nås via adressen www.svelitt.se. isbn 978–91–87666–38–4 issn 0348–6133 Printed in Lithuania by Balto print, Vilnius 2019 Recensioner av doktorsavhandlingar · 241 vitet. Men om medier, med Marshall McLuhan, Kelly Hübben, A Genre of Animal Hanky-panky? är proteser – vilket Gardfors skriver under på vid Animal Representations, Anthropomorphism, and några tillfällen – kommer frågan om subjektivi- Interspecies Relations in The Little Golden Books.
    [Show full text]
  • Sign Language Orangutan Dead at 39 8 August 2017
    Sign language orangutan dead at 39 8 August 2017 individuals he did not know and often chose forms of communication which are more typical of orangutans, such as vocalizations and unique hand gestures." Orangutans "are considered geriatric after the age of about 35," making Chantek "one of the oldest living male orangutans" in North America, the zoo said in a statement. © 2017 AFP Veterinarians at Zoo Atlanta had been treating Chantek for heart disease, but officials were reluctant to give a firm cause of animal's death Chantek, a male orangutan that learned to communicate in sign language, has died at the age of 39, a US zoo announced. Veterinarians at Zoo Atlanta had been treating Chantek for heart disease, but officials were reluctant to give a firm cause of animal's death on Monday. Chantek was born in Atlanta in the southern US state of Georgia but raised as a human child by an anthropologist in Tennessee who taught him American Sign Language (ASL). He was the subject of a 2014 documentary titled "The Ape Who Went to College." The orangutan moved to Zoo Atlanta in 1997. The zoo said Chantek "frequently used ASL to communicate with his caregivers, with whom he developed close personal bonds throughout his years at Zoo Atlanta." However "he was shy about signing with 1 / 2 APA citation: Sign language orangutan dead at 39 (2017, August 8) retrieved 27 September 2021 from https://phys.org/news/2017-08-language-orangutan-dead.html This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission.
    [Show full text]
  • Death of a Trapped Chimpanzee: Survival and Conservation of Great Apes in Unprotected Agricultural Areas of Uganda
    African Primates 13: 47-56 (2019)/ 47 Case Study: Death of a Trapped Chimpanzee: Survival and Conservation of Great Apes in Unprotected Agricultural Areas of Uganda Marie Cibot1,2,3, Sarah Le Roux⁴, Jacqueline Rohen2, and Matthew R. McLennan1,2,5 1Department of Social Sciences, Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences, Oxford Brookes University, Oxford, United Kingdom; 2Bulindi Chimpanzee and Community Project, Hoima, Uganda; 3Anicoon Vétérinaires, Ploemeur/ Larmor- Plage, France; 4Département de Pathologie Cellulaire et Tissulaire, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire d’Angers, Angers, France; 5Centre for Ecology and Conservation, University of Exeter, Cornwall, United Kingdom Abstract: Rural, human-dominated landscapes present substantial risks to chimpanzees and other primates. In western Uganda, some farmers guard against crop losses to wildlife by placing large steel leg-hold traps (‘mantraps’) near their agricultural fields. Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) can step in these illegal devices, resulting in severe injury or death. Here, we report a case of trapping and subsequent death of a wild chimpanzee from the Bulindi community (Hoima District). The elderly female chimpanzee died 13 days after being injured by a mantrap. Her injury could have contributed to the cause of death by impacting her balance in low trees above a stream; her body was subsequently found in the water. Behavioural observations prior to death and evidence from physical inspection of her injured hand suggest she may have otherwise recovered from her
    [Show full text]
  • Chimpanzee Referents and the Emergence of Human Hunting Travis Rayne Pickering*,1,2 and Manuel Domínguez-Rodrigo3
    The Open Anthropology Journal, 2010, 3, 107-113 107 Open Access Chimpanzee Referents and the Emergence of Human Hunting Travis Rayne Pickering*,1,2 and Manuel Domínguez-Rodrigo3 1Department of Anthropology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, 1180 Observatory Drive, 5240 Social Building, Madison, Wisconsin, 53706, USA; 2Institute for Human Evolution, University of the Witwatersrand, Private Bag 3, WITS 2050, Johannesburg, South Africa; 3Department of Prehistory, Complutense University, 28040, Madrid, Spain Abstract: The use of referent taxa, and especially chimpanzee referents, in modeling human evolution has been harshly criticized. No doubt, chimpanzee data are often misused in models of early hominid behavior. But, those misuses are ex- amples of careless, formal analogizing. In contrast, it is equally possible to create non-trivial chimpanzee analogies. These analogies can, in turn, be linked together to construct credible models of human evolution, from which emanate hypothe- ses that are testable using paleoanthropological data. Unique among potential referents, chimpanzees are very closely re- lated to early hominids and some populations reside in ecological contexts that are comparable to those of our African an- cestors. It is these two variables that form the core of evolutionary behavioral ecology. We exploit chimpanzee and early hominid continuities and employ non-trivial analogies to provide a model of basal hominid hunting. The model is testable and the topic is worthy because hunting and meat-eating are argued by some to be the basis of human sociality. Keywords: Uniformitarianism, modeling, human evolution, predation, meat-eating. INTRODUCTION mandible and on the ventral surface of a rib implies filleting of fully fleshed limbs and the removal of the tongue and up- Human hunting is unique.
    [Show full text]
  • MA-5 Fllgh Termed Success
    VOL. 1, NO. 4 MANNED SPACECRAFTCENTER, LANGLEY AFB, VA. DECEMBER13, 1961 MA-5 Fllgh Termed Success Project Mercury Announces Glenn, Slayton Are Named Plans For Two-Man Spacecraft For Orbital Missions Manned Spacecraft Center officials expressed pleasure with the suc- National Aeronautics and Space ing misisons. Another possibility is cessful two-orbit flight of the Mercury-Alas 5 spacecraft which was Administration's Manned Space- the direct flight approach using a craft Center is extending its Pro- multi-million pound thrust Nova launched at Cape Canaveral at 10:07 am November 29. The spacecraft ject Mercury effort to develop man- booster. Both methods will be ex- was brought back to earth after two orbits when difficulties with the ned space flight rendezvous ted> plored in order to meet the national control system were noted. niques by producing a two-man goal of manned lunar landing by The retro-rockets were fired by Arnold Aldrich of MSC's Flight spacecraft. The new craft will be ]970. Operations Division, on command speaking a little later about the capable of docking with another NASA will negotiate with Mc- of Chris Kraft, Flight Director. A1- launch said "the boost was about vehicle while in Earth orbit. Donnell Aircraft Corporation of S_. drich was serving as Chief Flight as near perfect as you would ex- Orbital rendezvous is one con- Louis, blo., as prime contractor of Controller at the Point Argudlo, pect to see." sideration for carrying out later the new spacecraft. Based on the Calif., tracking station. Others participating in the press Project Apollo manned lunar land- (Continued to page 7) At a press conference held at conference w e r e D.
    [Show full text]
  • Comparing the Imitative Skills of Children and Nonhuman Apes
    Revue de primatologie Numéro 1 (2009) Varia ............................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................... Malinda Carpenter et Josep Call Comparing the imitative skills of children and nonhuman apes ............................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................... Avertissement Le contenu de ce site relève de la législation française sur la propriété intellectuelle et est la propriété exclusive de l'éditeur. Les œuvres figurant sur ce site peuvent être consultées et reproduites sur un support papier ou numérique sous réserve qu'elles soient strictement réservées à un usage soit personnel, soit scientifique ou pédagogique excluant toute exploitation commerciale. La reproduction devra obligatoirement mentionner l'éditeur, le nom de la revue, l'auteur et la référence du document. Toute autre reproduction est interdite sauf accord préalable de l'éditeur, en dehors des cas prévus par la législation en vigueur en France. Revues.org est un portail de revues en sciences humaines et sociales développé par le CLEO, Centre pour l'édition électronique ouverte (CNRS, EHESS, UP, UAPV). ..............................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Language and the Orang-Utan: the Old 'Person' of the Forest*
    Language and the Orang-utan: The Old 'Person' of the Forest* H. LYN WHITE MILES I still maintain, that his [the orang-utan] being possessed of the capacity of acquiring it [language], by having both the human intelligence and the organs of pronunciation, joined to the dispositions and affections of his mind, mild, gentle, and humane, is sufficient to denominate him a man. Lord J. B. Monboddo, Of the Origin and Progress of Language, 17731 If we base personhood on linguistic and mental ability, we should now ask, 'Are orang-utans or other creatures persons?' The issues this question raises are complex, but certainly arrogance and ignorance have played a role in our reluctance to recognise the intellectual capacity of our closest biological relatives - the nonhuman great apes. We have set ourselves apart from other animals, because of the scope of our mental abilities and cultural achievements. Although there were religious perspectives that did not emphasise our estrangement from nature, such as the doctrine of St. Francis and forms of nature religions, the dominant Judeo-Christian tradition held that white 'man' was separate and was given dominion over the earth, including other races, women, children and animals. Western philosophy continued this imperious attitude with the views of Descartes, who proposed that animals were just like machines with no significant language, feelings or thoughts. Personhood was denied even to some human groups enslaved by Euro-American colonial institutions. Only a century or so ago, scholarly opinion held that the speech of savages was inferior to the languages of complex societies. While nineteenth-century anthropologists arrogantly concerned themselves with measurements of human skulls to determine racial superiority, there was not much sympathy for the notion that animals might also be persons.
    [Show full text]
  • Between the Gorilla and the Chimpanzee: a History of Debate Concerning the Existence of the Kooloo-Kamba Or Gorilla-Like Chimpanzee
    "J. Ethnobiol. 4(1): 1-13 May 1984 BETWEEN THE GORILLA AND THE CHIMPANZEE: A HISTORY OF DEBATE CONCERNING THE EXISTENCE OF THE KOOLOO-KAMBA OR GORILLA-LIKE CHIMPANZEE BRIAN T. SHEA Department ofAnthropology and Cell Biology & Anatomy Northwestern University 2006 Sheridan Road Evanston, IL 60201 ABSTRACT.-The taxonomic scheme proposed in 1934 by Ernst Schwarz for the subspec­ ific classification of common chimpanzees (P(J.n troglody tes) has been accepted by the majority of subsequent primatologists. A notable exception to this general trend is that the late W. C. O. Hill continued and revived a long history of controversial debate over the existence of a rare gorilla-like chimpanzee subspecies known as the "kooloo-kamba." The history of the enigmatic kooloo-kamba is reviewed here, from its early discovery and des­ cription by DuChaillu, through the morphological investigations of Keith, Schwarz, Merfield, and others, and finally to the more recent claims of Hill. Almost all claims supporting the existence of the kooloo-kamba have invoked indigenous labels and folk taxonomies as t'yJdence. The prolonged debate provides insights into the relationships between folk taxonomies and our own classifications. Confusion, vanatlon, and me u~~ 0"/ ,i'i'&"J.~\"ru".a..tc.t.!'" or hybrid categories in both Western and indigenous classifications probably reflects a salient biologic fact-gorillas and chimpanzees are very closely related animals with patterns of morphological development which coincide and overlap. INTRODUCTION During the late 1800s and early 1900s, a large number of chimpanzee and gorilla species were described by various workers, often on the basis of a particular variation of facial coloring, hair distribution, or cranial shape.
    [Show full text]